Skip to main content
Erschienen in: AIDS and Behavior 8/2019

Open Access 19.12.2018 | Original Paper

Accuracy of HIV Risk Perception in East Zimbabwe 2003–2013

verfasst von: Robin Schaefer, Ranjeeta Thomas, Constance Nyamukapa, Rufurwokuda Maswera, Noah Kadzura, Simon Gregson

Erschienen in: AIDS and Behavior | Ausgabe 8/2019

Abstract

Risk perception for HIV infection is an important determinant for engaging in HIV prevention behaviour. We investigate the degree to which HIV risk perception is accurate, i.e. corresponds to actual HIV infection risks, in a general-population open-cohort study in Zimbabwe (2003–2013) including 7201 individuals over 31,326 person-years. Risk perception for future infection (no/yes) at the beginning of periods between two surveys was associated with increased risk of HIV infection (Cox regression hazard ratio = 1.38 [1.07–1.79], adjusting for socio-demographic characteristics, sexual behaviour, and partner behaviour). The association was stronger among older people (25+ years). This suggests that HIV risk perception can be accurate but the higher HIV incidence (1.27 per 100 person-years) illustrates that individuals may face barriers to HIV prevention behaviour even when they perceive their risks. Gaps in risk perception are underlined by the high incidence among those not perceiving a risk (0.96%), low risk perception even among those reporting potentially risky sexual behaviour, and, particularly, lack of accuracy of risk perception among young people. Innovative interventions are needed to improve accuracy of risk perception but barriers to HIV prevention behaviours need to be addressed too, which may relate to the partner, community, or structural factors.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10461-018-2374-0) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

HIV incidence remains high in many countries, particularly in sub-Saharan Africa, with reductions failing to meet international targets [1]. In part, this reflects continued low use of primary HIV prevention methods, including condoms, voluntary medical male circumcision (VMMC), and pre-exposure prophylaxis (PrEP) [2]. One factor that is considered important—often necessary—for motivation to engage in HIV prevention behaviour is perceiving a personal risk for HIV acquisition [3]. Associations have been found between HIV risk perception and delayed sexual debut [4], condom use [5, 6], and adherence to daily PrEP [79]. Given these links between risk perception and preventative behaviour, HIV prevention programmes frequently aim to raise awareness of risks and increase risk perception [2, 10]. Risk perception has also been proposed as the first step in early formulations of HIV prevention cascades [11], a framework to improve the planning, implementation, and evaluation of HIV prevention programmes and interventions. However, one common concern is that the lack of use of prevention methods, and thus continuing high HIV incidence, does not only reflect a widespread lack of risk perception but also a mismatch between actual and perceived risk of HIV infection—i.e. lack of accuracy of risk perception.
Even within generalised epidemics, HIV infection risk varies considerably across areas [12, 13] and within populations, with some groups, for example adolescent girls and young women [14], exhibiting disproportionally high HIV incidence. It is therefore vital that those with increased HIV infection risk perceive their risk and engage in protective behaviour, particularly because targeted HIV prevention activities may be more effective in reducing HIV incidence [15]. Nevertheless, while “unrealistic optimism”—underestimating one’s risk—has been demonstrated for HIV infection risk [1618], evidence for a match between self-perceived and actual HIV infection risk is limited—despite the importance widely attached to HIV risk perception.
Current evidence comes largely from cross-sectional studies that are difficult to interpret [1921]. Measuring accuracy of risk perception in terms of its association with actual HIV infection risk requires longitudinal data with objective measurement of HIV incidence. In a longitudinal study among injecting drug users in Canada, risk perception predicted HIV acquisition [22]. However, results from this high-risk population that is characterised by very high HIV incidence are not generalisable to settings with generalised epidemics. The only other previously published longitudinal study that analysed this association found that perceived risk in young South African women did not correspond to actual risk of acquiring HIV [23]. However, the study used self-reported HIV status to determine eligibility at baseline, so results may not be reliable. In this article, longitudinal data from a large, prospective HIV sero-survey, collected over a ten-year period of high HIV incidence, are used to measure accuracy of perceived risk of HIV infection in a representative sample of the population in Manicaland, east Zimbabwe.

Methods

Setting and Data

Data for this study were taken from the Manicaland General-Population Cohort Study (Manicaland Study) that was implemented in Manicaland, east Zimbabwe. In Manicaland, HIV prevalence declined from over 25% at the end of the 1990s to levels of about 11% in 2015–2016 [24], partially due to behaviour change [25, 26]. However, despite decreases from peaks of 1.8% in the mid-2000s, HIV incidence in the general population remains high at just under 1% for females and 0.5% for males [27]. Uptake of VMMC is low [11], and among young women, a target for PrEP in sub-Saharan Africa, sexual relationships with older men are common while condom use is limited [28]. Oral PrEP has recently become available in Zimbabwe through small-scale research and pilot projects, focusing largely on young female sex workers, leading to just over 3000 people being initiated on PrEP at the end of 2017 [29].
The Manicaland Study is a long-term general-population open-cohort study, with six surveys conducted in three districts since 1998. Each survey included a household census in 12 sites (eight in the most recent survey in 2012–2013) to identify participants. These were representative of the population in Manicaland that is characterised by different socio-economic strata, including small towns, subsistence farming areas, agricultural estates, and roadside business centres. Participants were prospectively followed in each survey but newly identified eligible individuals were included in surveys. Surveys included between 8000 and 15,000 adults aged 15–54 years. Participation rates varied between 73.0 and 79.5%. Periods between surveys were about 3 years and three attempts were made to reach participants for follow-up. Loss-to-follow-up resulted largely due to participants becoming ineligible through migrating out of the study area or death. Among those who remained eligible, follow-up ranged between 77.0 and 96.4%.
The Manicaland Study was originally set up to evaluate a cluster-randomised HIV prevention trial in the first two surveys but the research aims were expanded from survey round three to investigate the dynamics and determinants of the HIV epidemic in the area (we included only data from survey three for main analyses, see below). After survey two, data from the Manicaland Study was used to evaluate national HIV control programmes but the study itself did not implement interventions. Data collected in the Manicaland Study include HIV sero-testing, so HIV infection was objectively determined in this study, and information on demographic and socio-economic characteristics, sexual behaviour, and perceptions about HIV/AIDS. To reduce social desirability bias, informal confidential voting interview methods were used [30], in which participants answered more sensitive sexual behaviour questions on pieces of paper and inserted these into a box instead of responding directly to the interviewer. Ethical approval for the Manicaland Study was obtained from the Imperial College London Research Ethics Committee and the Medical Research Council of Zimbabwe. More details on the Manicaland Study are available elsewhere [27] and online (http://​www.​manicalandhivpro​ject.​org/​).

Data Analysis

The main analysis was restricted to survey rounds three (2003–2005) to six (2012–2013) because the survey question measuring risk perception was different in the first two survey rounds (“Do you think you could become infected with HIV yourself in the future?” in surveys one and two as opposed to “If you are not infected, do you think you are in danger of getting infected now or in the future?” from survey three). While the change in measurement may be small, the effects of this are unclear, so excluding survey rounds one and two was considered more conservative. Another reason for restricting the main analysis to data from survey three was that measurements of some other key variables were different or data were not collected in earlier surveys (including on condom use and sexual risk factors; see below). Nevertheless, in a secondary analysis, data from the first two surveys were included (see Supplementary Material, p. 5).
The risk perception measure allowed ‘yes’, ‘no’, and ‘don’t know’ responses. ‘Don’t know’ answers (9.6% over surveys three to six) were excluded from all analyses since these could not be categorised as either ‘yes’ or ‘no’, as described in the Supplementary Material (p. 3). To implement longitudinal analyses for capturing incident HIV infections and estimate HIV incidence, data were restricted to those who (1) participated in at least two surveys; (2) were HIV-negative at the beginning of the period between two surveys; and (3) those who reported having had sex at the beginning of inter-survey period since HIV is nearly exclusively sexually transmitted in the study population [31]. The beginning of the period between surveys refers to the interview date of the first of the two interviews of the survey pair. Individuals could contribute more than one survey pair by participating in more than two surveys but had to be HIV-negative at the beginning of each survey pair.
Those who started sexual activity during survey rounds were excluded because sexual debut is likely to have a strong influence on risk perception and other key variables were unavailable for those not sexually active. Trends in risk perception, potentially risky sexual behaviour, perceived risky behaviour of the partner, and condom use at the beginning of each period between surveys were described. This included data for survey six (the end of the final inter-survey period), as well as one (1998–2000) and two (2001–2003) to describe trends comprehensively, although these data were not included in the main regression analyses. A sexual risk variable was created based on the number of sexual risk factors (none, one, more than one), including multiple partners in the past 12 months, casual partners in the past 3 years, and concurrent sexual relationships at the moment. Perceived partner risky behaviour was based on reporting that the partner has other partners (partner concurrency). Condom use was based on reported use during last sexual intercourse.
Risk perception was tested for its longitudinal association with HIV acquisition as a measure of accuracy. Methods for estimating HIV incidence in the study data are described elsewhere [28]. In short, variables at the beginning of the period between surveys were tested for association with HIV infection in Cox regression. For those who turned HIV-positive between two surveys, the date of HIV infection was unknown, so 30 random infection dates between surveys were imputed and results for imputed data sets were pooled. This approach was chosen as using the mid-point date between surveys may introduce bias [32, 33]. Individuals were censored at their date of HIV infection or 55th birthday. Regression models controlled for age and sex (model 1); marital status, educational attainment, and household wealth index (model 2) (identified as important socio-demographic characteristics in preliminary analyses; see Supplementary Material, p. 4); and own sexual risk, partner risky behaviour, and condom use (model 3). Models were estimated separately including: (1) risk perception (no/yes); and (2) risk perception with reported reasons for perceiving an infection risk (multiple partners, partner has other partners, marrying someone who is HIV-positive, and ‘other’). Risk perception itself does not cause HIV infection; rather, any association between risk perception and HIV incidence reflects accurate recognition of other risk factors. Changes in the association between risk perception and HIV incidence in models 2 and 3 could provide insights into how risk perception was linked with HIV infection risk.
Sub-analyses tested for associations between risk perception and HIV acquisition risk (controlling for age and sex) in different time periods relating to the introduction of antiretroviral treatment (ART) (ART roll-out phase [2003–2008] and post-ART period [2009–2013]) and by sex, age group (15–24; 25–54 years), marital status, sexual risk (none; at least one risk factor), condom use, and perceived partner risk (partner had no other partners; had other partners). Interactions were also tested for in separate regression models including the socio-demographic or behavioural variable and an interaction term with risk perception.
All regressions included survey round and study site as covariates. The inclusion of these variables was important to account for any broader environmental, potentially time-varying factors that may confound the relationship between risk perception and HIV incidence. Study location-level (which meant village-level in most cases) cluster-robust standard error estimation was used. Proportional hazards assumptions were met (Supplementary Material, p. 6). All variables and their measurements are further described in the Supplementary Material (p. 2).

Results

Over survey rounds three to six, 10,774 observations met the inclusion criteria for this study (67.0% female), based on 7201 individuals. 2830 individuals (39.3%) participated in more than two surveys and 743 (10.3%) participated in all four included surveys. Patterns of HIV risk perception by socio-demographic and behavioural characteristics are shown in Table 1. Among males (N = 3553), 13.0% (95% confidence interval [CI] = 11.9–14.1%) perceived a risk of HIV infection, and 47.5% (46.4–48.7%) among females (N = 7221), with declines over time observed for both sexes (Fig. 1a). For both sexes, risk perception was higher in those with sexual risk factors and in those reporting that their partners had other partners. However, even among those with two or more sexual risk factors, 44.8% (32.3–58.1%) of females (N = 60) and 77.1% (73.4–80.4%) of males (N = 556) reported that they do not perceive a risk of HIV infection. Similarly, 35.0% (32.3–37.8%) of females (N = 1172) and 78.0% (70.3–84.2%) of males (N = 141) who reported that their partners had other partners did not perceive a risk of HIV infection.
Table 1
HIV risk perception by socio-demographic and behavioural characteristics, Manicaland, Zimbabwe, 2003–2011
 
Males (N = 3553)
Females (N = 7221)
N (%)
% perceives risk (95% CI)
N (%)
% perceives risk (95% CI)
Age
    
 15–24 years
790 (22.2)
17.8 (15.3–20.7)
1344 (18.6)
40.5 (37.9–43.1)
 25–54 years
2763 (77.8)
11.6 (10.4–12.8)
5877 (81.4)
49.2 (47.9–50.4)
Marital status
    
 Never married
763 (21.5)
21.5 (18.7–24.6)
202 (2.81)
45.3 (38.5–52.3)
 Married
2635 (74.4)
10.1 (9.00–11.3)
5673 (78.9)
50.8 (49.5–52.1)
 Separated/divorced
116 (3.27)
19.8 (13.5–28.2)
494 (6.87)
40.0 (35.7–44.4)
 Widowed
29 (0.82)
20.7 (9.12–40.4)
812 (11.4)
29.8 (26.7–33.0)
Education
    
 None/primary
966 (27.3)
11.0 (9.17–13.1)
3324 (46.7)
46.5 (44.8–48.2)
 Secondary/higher
2571 (72.7)
13.7 (12.5–15.1)
3794 (53.3)
48.6 (47.0–50.2)
Wealth index quintile
    
 Poorest
493 (14.0)
12.6 (9.92–15.8)
1103 (15.4)
46.4 (43.5–49.4)
 2nd poorest
1623 (45.9)
12.2 (10.7–13.8)
3545 (49.5)
45.9 (44.3–47.5)
 3rd poorest
1052 (29.8)
14.1 (12.2–16.4)
1936 (27.0)
51.2 (49.0–53.4)
 4th poorest
340 (9.62)
14.8 (11.4–19.0)
530 (7.40)
49.1 (44.9–53.4)
 Least poor
25 (0.71)
4.00 (0.48–26.3)
45 (0.63)
44.4 (30.3–59.6)
Sexual risk factorsa
    
 None
2175 (61.8)
9.17 (8.03–10.5)
6650 (92.9)
47.2 (46.0–48.4)
 1
786 (22.4)
16.8 (14.4–19.6)
449 (6.27)
51.1 (46.5–55.7)
 2+
556 (15.8)
22.9 (19.6–26.6)
60 (0.84)
55.2 (41.9–67.7)
Partner has other partners
    
 No
3381 (96.0)
12.6 (11.5–13.8)
5888 (83.4)
44.6 (43.4–45.9)
 Yes
141 (4.00)
22.0 (15.8–29.7)
1172 (16.6)
65.0 (62.2–67.7)
Condom use during last sex
    
 No
2738 (77.5)
11.0 (9.83–12.2)
6489 (90.3)
47.5 (46.3–48.7)
 Yes
793 (22.5)
20.0 (17.3–22.9)
697 (9.70)
48.1 (44.4–51.9)
Values represent the sample sizes (N) and relative sizes in percent (%) of the different categories of variables as well as the percentage of those in these categories perceiving a risk for HIV infection with 95% confidence intervals (95% CI). Values may not add up to 100% due to rounding. All statistics are based on the sample as used in the main analyses (i.e. data from the beginning of the period between surveys from survey round 3 to 6)
aThe sexual risk variable was based on three variables: reporting more than one sexual partner in the past 12 months; reporting at least one casual partner in the past 3 years; and reporting concurrent sexual partner at the time of the survey
38.2% (36.6–39.8%) of males and 7.1% (6.6–7.8%) of females reported at least one sexual risk factor. For males, proportions reporting of risk factors declined over time but increased in the most recent survey (Fig. 1b); for females, there was no clear trend. Condom use was low in the population, with 22.5% (21.1–23.9%) of males and 9.7% (9.0–10.4%) of females reporting condom use during last sexual intercourse. For males, there was a marked decrease in condom use followed by a sharp increase in the most recent survey (Fig. 1c); while, for females, there was a slight increase over time. Risk perception was higher among males reporting condom use while there was no difference among females (Table 1). 4.0% (3.4–4.7%) of males and 16.6% (15.8–17.5%) of females reported that their partners had other partners, with a long-term decreasing trend for females (Fig. 1d).

Accuracy of Risk Perception

343 new HIV infections occurred over 31,326 person-years. HIV incidence was similar in males (1.19 per 100 person-years [95% CI 0.99–1.40%]) and females (1.04% [0.90–1.18%]). HIV incidence among those who perceived a risk for HIV infection was 1.27% (1.06–1.48%) compared to 0.96% (0.83–1.10%) among those who did not (adjusted hazard ratio [aHR] = 1.34 [1.05–1.72], adjusted for age, sex, survey round, and study site). This roughly one-third higher risk was not markedly affected when controlling for other socio-demographic characteristics, own and partner sexual risk factors, or condom use (Table 2). The association was stronger among females (aHR = 1.48 [1.09–1.99]) than males (aHR = 1.28 [0.81–2.00]) (Table 3) (although the estimates for males and females were not significantly different and there was no significant interaction by sex: Table 4). Results were similar when including data from earlier survey rounds (model 1, both sexes combined: aHR = 1.36 [1.13–1.65]; Supplementary Material, p. 5), despite the changing risk perception measure.
Table 2
Risk perception and HIV incidence (both sexes combined), Manicaland, Zimbabwe, 2003–2013
Variable
N (%)
Inf/pyrs (IR)
Model 1 (n = 10,732)
Model 2 (n = 10,494)
Model 3 (n = 10,214)
aHR (95% CI)
p-value
aHR (95% CI)
p-value
aHR (95% CI)
p-value
Risk perception
        
 No
6857 (63.9)
191/19,884 (0.96)
1 (Reference)
 
1 (Reference)
 
1 (Reference)
 
 Yes
3879 (36.1)
144/11,348 (1.27)
1.34 (1.05–1.72)
0.021
1.41 (1.11–1.80)
0.005
1.38 (1.07–1.79)
0.014
Risk perception: reason
        
 No
 
191/19,884 (0.96)
1 (Reference)
 
1 (Reference)
 
1 (Reference)
 
 Yes: has multiple partners
121 (3.19)
16/354 (2.52)
3.88 (2.38–6.33)
< 0.001
3.66 (2.26–5.91)
< 0.001
3.30 (1.89–5.77)
< 0.001
 Yes: partner has other partners
1244 (32.8)
51/3709 (1.38)
1.28 (0.87–1.91)
0.213
1.35 (0.90–2.03)
0.145
1.35 (0.87–2.08)
0.178
 Yes: marry HIV–positive partner
210 (5.50)
20/640 (3.07)
2.34 (1.50–3.66)
< 0.001
2.32 (1.43–3.74)
< 0.001
2.34 (1.43–3.83)
< 0.001
 Yes: other
2222 (58.5)
55/6407 (0.87)
0.96 (0.69–1.33)
0.803
1.05 (0.75–1.47)
0.771
1.05 (0.76–1.48)
0.763
Values are sample sizes (N) and percentages (%) for variable categories, new HIV infections (inf) per person-years (pyrs), crude incidence rates per 100 person-years (IR), adjusted hazard ratios (aHR), 95% confidence intervals (CI), and p-values. Different models estimated associations for risk perception (no/yes) (top panel) and risk perception by reason (bottom panel). Sample sizes and percentages for reasons for risk perception refer to the sample of those who perceived a risk. The covariate results are not shown. Regression results are based on 30 imputed random dates of HIV infection between surveys. Participants were censored at their 55th birthday. Sample sizes differ between the models due to missing data on variables included in the models
Model 1: age, sex, survey round, study site
Model 2: age, sex, marital status, educational attainment, household wealth index, survey round, study site
Model 3: age, sex, marital status, educational attainment, household wealth index, sexual risk factors, condom use (last sex), partner has other partners, survey round, study site
Table 3
Risk perception and HIV incidence by sex, Manicaland, Zimbabwe, 2003–2013
Variable
Males
Females
N (%)
Inf/pyrs (IR)
Model 3 (n = 3433)
N (%)
Inf/pyrs (IR)
Model 3 (n = 6781)
aHR (95% CI)
p-value
aHR (95% CI)
p-value
Risk perception
        
 No
3083 (87.0)
102/9287 (1.10)
1 (Reference)
 
3774 (52.5)
89/10,597 (0.84)
1 (Reference)
 
 Yes
460 (13.0)
24/1458 (1.66)
1.28 (0.81–2.00)
0.289
3419 (47.5)
120/9890 (1.21)
1.48 (1.09–1.99)
0.011
Risk perception: reason
        
 No
 
102/9287 (1.10)
1 (Reference)
  
89/10,597 (0.84)
1 (Reference)
 
 Yes: has multiple partners
52 (11.6)
8/158 (5.06)
3.34 (1.51–7.37)
0.003
69 (2.06)
8/196 (4.09)
3.17 (1.23–8.15)
0.017
 Yes: partner has other partners
97 (21.7)
3/314 (0.96)
0.66 (0.15–2.86)
0.589
1147 (34.2)
48/3396 (1.42)
1.51 (0.95–2.40)
0.078
 Yes: marry HIV-positive partner
114 (25.5)
8/371 (2.06)
1.77 (0.79–3.94)
0.165
96 (2.87)
12/268 (4.47)
2.70 (1.37–5.32)
0.004
 Yes: other
184 (41.2)
6/572 (0.98)
0.84 (0.34–2.05)
0.701
2038 (60.8)
50/5835 (0.85)
1.24 (0.86–1.78)
0.257
Values are sample sizes (N) and percentages (%) for variable categories, new HIV infections (inf) per person-years (pyrs), crude incidence rates per 100 person-years (IR), adjusted hazard ratios (aHR), 95% confidence intervals (CI), and p-values. Different models estimated associations for risk perception (no/yes) (top panel) and risk perception by reason (bottom panel), for males and females separately. Sample sizes and percentages for reasons for risk perception refer to the sample of those who perceived a risk. The covariate results are not shown. Regression results are based on 30 imputed random dates of HIV infection between surveys. Participants were censored at their 55th birthday. Only results for model 3 are shown
Model 3: age, marital status, educational attainment, household wealth index, sexual risk factors, condom use (last sex), partner has other partners, survey round, study site
Table 4
Risk perception and HIV incidence by socio-demographic characteristics and behaviour, Manicaland, Zimbabwe, 2003–2013
Variable
Inf/pyrs (IR)
Hazard ratio of HIV infection when perceiving a risk (vs no risk perception)
p-value of interaction
N
aHR
95% CI
Sex
     
 Males
128/10,774 (1.19)
3543
1.27
(0.82–1.99)
 
 Females
215/20,562 (1.05)
7193
1.41
(1.07–1.85)
0.723
Age group (years)a
     
 15–24
89/6585 (1.35)
2134
1.08
(0.69–1.70)
 
 25–54
255/24,751 (1.03)
8602
1.58
(1.19–2.10)
0.644
Marital statusb
     
 Never married
39/3104 (1.26)
964
2.05
(1.04–4.05)
 
 Currently married
237/24,029 (0.99)
8282
1.29
(0.94–1.76)
 
 Formerly married
64/4079 (1.58)
1447
1.54
(0.92–2.57)
0.079
Time periodc
     
 ART roll-out
276/23,062 (1.20)
7384
1.44
(1.10–1.89)
 
 Post-ART
68/8274 (0.83)
3352
1.25
(0.74–2.11)
0.722
Sexual risk
     
 No risk factor
239/25,377 (0.94)
8794
1.41
(1.07–1.87)
 
 At least one risk factor
99/5689 (1.74)
1849
1.18
(0.75–1.88)
0.694
Condom use (last sex)
     
 No use
276/26,672 (1.04)
9200
1.17
(0.88–1.56)
 
 Used condom
67/4493 (1.48)
1479
2.58
(1.61–4.13)
< 0.001
Partner has other partners
     
 No
282/26,939 (1.05)
9238
1.38
(1.06–1.80)
 
 Yes
56/3853 (1.45)
1307
1.00
(0.53–1.89)
0.950
The table shows for each sub-group for each variable the number of new HIV infections (inf) per person-years (pyrs) and crude incidence rates per 100 person-years (IR). For each of these sub-groups, Cox regression models were implemented to test for the association between HIV risk perception and HIV infection risk, with adjusted hazard ratios (aHR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) referring to the ratio of perceiving a risk (vs not perceiving a risk). Sample sizes (N) refer to the samples for the regression for each sub-group. Each regression model included age and sex as additional variables. A higher aHR suggest that the association between risk perception and HIV infection was stronger in that sub-group, thus suggesting higher accuracy. This interaction was tested in separate models that included the socio-demographic or behavioural variable and an interaction term of this variable with risk perception; the p-values refer to this interaction
aAge (continuous) was not included as a covariate in analyses of age groups
bThose divorced/separated and those widowed were grouped together into the ‘formerly married’ category. The p-value of the interaction term is for the interaction as a whole, not between specific categories
cSurvey round was not included as a covariate in the analyses by time period. The ART roll-out period refers to the inter-survey periods of survey 3 (2003–2005) to 4 (2006–2008) and 4 to 5 (2009–2011). The post-ART period refers to the inter-survey period of survey 5 to 6 (2012–2013)
Excluding ‘other’ reasons, suspecting that the partner had other partners was the most common reason for HIV risk perception among females; men were more likely to state having multiple partners as the reason for risk perception, although suspecting partner concurrency and marrying an HIV-infected person were more common reasons (Table 3). Risk perception was associated with increased HIV infection risk regardless of the reason (excluding ‘other’ reasons) (Table 2), although to varying degree. Controlling for socio-demographic characteristics and own and partner sexual behaviour, HIV infection risk was 230% higher among those who perceived a risk because they had multiple partners compared to those not perceiving a risk (aHR = 3.30 [1.89–5.77]) (similar for both sexes, Table 3), but only 35% higher in those perceiving a risk because they thought their partner had other partners (aHR = 1.35 [0.87–2.08]). Those perceiving a risk because they might marry a partner who is HIV-infected were also at greater risk of HIV infection (aHR = 2.34 [1.43–3.83]).
When stratifying by socio-demographic and behavioural characteristics, the general trend of higher HIV infection risk among those perceiving a risk was seen in most sub-groups, although with varying strength (Table 4). The strength of the association—so the accuracy of HIV risk perception—was higher among those who were older and those who had never been married, during the ART roll-out phase, in those without sexual risk factors, reporting that their partner had no other partners, and who used a condom during last sexual intercourse. However, sample sizes in some sub-groups were small and interaction terms in were not statistically significant, except for marital status and condom use (Table 4).

Discussion

In this large general-population cohort in east Zimbabwe, sexually active individuals who perceived a risk of future HIV infection had a one-third greater risk of acquiring HIV infection than those who did not, accounting for a range of socio-demographic and behavioural characteristics as well as potential time-varying and broader environmental confounders. This represents the first scientifically robust evidence from a general-population sample in a generalised HIV epidemic that HIV risk perception can be accurate. Accurate risk perception is vital so that individuals who are actually at increased risk of HIV infection also perceive themselves to be at risk and thus are motivated to protect themselves against infection.
The relationship between behaviour, perceptions, and HIV infection risk is complex. Someone who engages in behaviours associated with increased risk of HIV infection (e.g. having multiple or non-regular partners [3436]) but uses protective measures (e.g. condoms) may not perceive a risk for HIV infection. This may be accurate if condoms are used consistently, but individuals may actually still be at an increased risk if condoms are used only some of the time. An advantage of this study was that it used biomarkers for HIV infection to objectively determine HIV infection risks. We therefore considered the outcome of behaviours and it was not necessary to know each individual’s behaviour for making conclusions about the accuracy of perceptions. With this approach, we demonstrate significant gaps in risk perception. Many individuals did not perceive a risk despite engaging in potentially high-risk behaviour. 45% of females and 80% of males reporting two or more sexual risk factors did not report that they were at risk of HIV infection. While engaging in these behaviours is not inherently ‘risky’, we show that HIV incidence was high (1%) in individuals who did not perceive themselves to be at risk, thus these individuals did not accurately evaluate their HIV infection risks. Furthermore, while the higher HIV infections risk among those who perceive a risk demonstrates the accuracy of these perceptions, it also underlines that these individuals may face barriers preventing them from translating this perception into protective behaviour. In fact, if they engaged in protective behaviour, they may not have reported risk perception (although risk perception was higher among males who used condoms).
The observed relationship between risk perception and HIV incidence differed markedly across sub-groups, although risk perception tended to be associated with higher incidence in all groups. The relationship was stronger among those who were older and was weak among those aged under 25. Therefore, on average, young people who perceived and who did not perceive a risk were at the same risk of HIV infection, so risk perception did not correspond to increased risk of HIV infection. This does not mean that every young person was at the same risk of HIV infection; rather, many young people at increased infection risk did not perceive this increased risk and some young people not at increased risk perceived themselves to be at risk. This leads to inappropriate patterns of motivation to engage in HIV prevention, which is of concern since HIV incidence was generally higher in younger people, particularly young women [28].
The association between risk perception and HIV incidence was stronger in those who had not yet married than in currently married people. This may be because never married people had only short-term partners, so they only need to evaluate their own behaviour, not the risk resulting from their long-term partners, and those who engage in risky behaviours are aware of their risks. This is further supported by the strong association between risk perception and HIV incidence when one’s own risky behaviour is given as the reason. Individuals who reported that their partners had other partners were more likely to perceive a risk for HIV infection; however, the relationship between risk perception and infection risk was weak among those reporting risk perception because their partners had other partners. This may be because there are more possible sources of error when assessing infection risks from the partner as opposed to one’s own behaviour, as there may be errors in assessing whether or not the partner actually has other partners and in assessing the risk associated with these partners. HIV risk perception was more strongly associated with HIV incidence in people who used condoms than in those who did not. Our measure of condom use was based on use during last sexual intercourse and therefore, in most cases, probably represents condom use with regular partners. The relatively high accuracy of risk perception in this group may be because many of these individuals know or have good reason to suspect that their partners are HIV-positive, but, again, the high HIV incidence underscores that these individuals failed to adequately protect themselves against HIV infection.
This study analysed the association between risk perception and HIV infection risk completely relying on biomarkers for HIV infection, differing from a study in South Africa that excluded individuals at baseline (in 2005) based on self-reported HIV status and that did not find an association between HIV infection risk and HIV risk perception [23]. In 2005, HIV testing was likely to be uncommon (30% of South Africans were ever tested in 2005 [37]), so participants may already have been unknowingly infected with HIV at baseline, which could have introduced significant noise into the data. Despite this, the results of the two studies are not inconsistent as the South African study was limited to young women and we also found low accuracy of risk perception in this group in east Zimbabwe. The results of the current study may be more generalisable to other parts of sub-Saharan Africa, however, since patterns of marriage and sexual behaviour are probably more representative [38] than those from the metropolitan area of Cape Town, South Africa. The considerable decline in HIV incidence in Zimbabwe over time is unlikely to limit the generalisability of the findings to other settings with more moderate declines in incidence given that accuracy of risk perception does not necessarily depend on background levels of incidence and populations across sub-Saharan Africa have been extensively exposed to HIV prevention messages and programmes, although it is unclear whether these may have been more successful in improving accuracy of HIV risk perception in Zimbabwe.
Reported risk perception has been declining over time in the study population. To the degree that individuals accurately recognise their risks, declining risk perception may reflect declines in reported sexual risk factors (among males) and suspecting that the partner has other partners (among females), and indirectly the decline in HIV incidence. The increase in risk perception among males in the most recent survey round also corresponds to an increase in risk behaviour. The increasing availability of ART may have further contributed to reductions in perceived risk. The association between risk perception and HIV incidence was weaker in the post-ART period compared to the ART roll-out phase, possibly because ART attenuates risks of HIV infection, making risk perceptions less accurate—e.g. sexual intercourse with an HIV-positive partner may be perceived as risky but is actually not associated with an increased risk if the partner is on ART. In this context of declining risk perception, and possibly reduced accuracy of risk perception, it is worrying that men’s condom use declined until the most recent survey and that women’s condom use remained low. Even in the post-ART period, HIV incidence has been high (0.83%) (which, as an average, masks heterogeneity in incidence among different population groups), with ART coverage still below 40% in the 2012–2013 survey [27]. However, statistical power for these sub-analyses was limited and interactions were not statistically significant in most cases.
While HIV incidence was measured objectively, this study relied on self-reports for other variables. Due to social desirability bias, risk perception may be under-reported to avoid being associated with risky behaviour. This may partly explain the high HIV incidence among those not reporting risk perception, so the difference in incidence between those who did and did not perceive a risk may be underestimated, making our findings conservative. Similarly, sexual risk behaviour may be under-reported, despite the informal confidential voting interview methods to reduce social desirability bias [30]. Inaccurate measurement of sexual behaviour may also explain why the association between risk perception and HIV incidence did not markedly change when controlling for sexual risk factors. If these risk factors had been perfectly measured, the strength of the association between risk perception and incidence would likely have been affected as risk perception is associated with HIV infection risk through the recognition of these sexual risk factors. However, while reported levels of risk perception and risky sexual behaviour may be biased, observed trends are unlikely to be affected by this. Another limitation is the simple binary measure for risk perception. While this measure refers to future HIV infection—in contrast to other studies that only considered perceptions of current infection status [39]—it does not permit investigation of whether different levels of risk perception are associated with different levels of HIV incidence.
Despite limitations in the data, this study demonstrates that subjective perceptions of HIV infection risk can be accurate, and so supports HIV prevention programmes aiming at increasing risk perception. At the same time, the higher HIV incidence among those perceiving a risk underlines the considerable barriers to engaging in HIV prevention behaviour individuals may face even if they recognise their risks, which may be beyond the individual’s control [40]. This includes partner refusal—which is important for condom use as well as adherence to PrEP [41] and uptake of VMMC [42]—social norms [43], and structural barriers [44], including those relating to the legal system. This study supports calls to increase attention towards HIV prevention [45] given the continuing high HIV incidence in this population and declines and considerable gaps in risk perception—despite long-term exposure to HIV prevention programmes. The variation in accuracy of risk perception across sub-groups is also a cause of concern—particularly the low accuracy of risk perception among young people and the difficulties in determining exposure to risks from the partner compared to one’s own behaviour. This underscores the need for innovative approaches to improve risk perception such as the recent application of methods from behavioural economics to correct risk perception in South African teenagers [46]. However, given the broad range of factors influencing HIV prevention behaviour, as is increasingly recognised in approaches to HIV prevention [1, 43, 44, 4749], interventions focusing on increasing risk perception must be accompanied by other interventions to strengthen motivation for using prevention methods, access to these methods—including removing structural barriers—and individual capacity for effective use of these, which may involve partner-based interventions [50].

Acknowledgements

Our sincerest gratitude goes out to everyone who has been involved in the Manicaland Project since its inception over 20 years ago. This study only exists because of those involved in planning, data collection and processing, and, particularly, because of the study participants. We would also thank Dr Nadine Schur who developed the methodology for calculating the household wealth index used in this study. R.S., S.G., N.K., R.M., R.T. and C.N. designed the study. R.S. analysed the data with input from R.T. and S.G. All authors contributed to interpretation of results and read and approved the final manuscript. Data produced by the Manicaland Project can be obtained from the project website: http://​www.​manicalandhivpro​ject.​org/​data.​html. Here we provide a core dataset which contains a sample of socio-demographic, sexual behaviour and HIV testing variables from all 6 rounds of the main survey. If further data is required, a data request form must be completed (available to download from our website) and submitted to simon.gregson@imperial.ac.uk. This study was funded by a Wellcome Trust Programme Grant (084401/Z/07/B). Robin Schaefer is supported by separate funding by the Wellcome Trust. Ranjeeta Thomas is supported by funding from the HIV Prevention Trials Network 071 Study (HPTN 071). HPTN 071 is sponsored by the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases (NIAID) under Co-operative Agreements UM1-AI068619, UM1-AI068617, and UM1-AI068613, with funding from the U.S. President’s Emergency Plan for AIDS Relief (PEPFAR). Additional funding is provided by the International Initiative for Impact Evaluation (3ie) with support from the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation, as well as by NIAID, the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) and the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), all part of NIH. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIAID, NIMH, NIDA, PEPFAR, 3ie, or the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

Simon Gregson declares shareholding in pharmaceutical companies (GSK and Astra Zeneca). Ranjeeta Thomas declares personal fees received for consultancy for the International Decision Support Initiative. The authors declare no further potential conflicts of interests.

Ethical Approval

The Manicaland Study Ethical was approved by the Imperial College London Research Ethics Committee and the Medical Research Council of Zimbabwe. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committees and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments.
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Anhänge

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat UNAIDS. HIV Prevention 2020 Road Map—Accelerating HIV prevention to reduce new infections by 75%. Geneva: UNAIDS; 2017. UNAIDS. HIV Prevention 2020 Road Map—Accelerating HIV prevention to reduce new infections by 75%. Geneva: UNAIDS; 2017.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat UNAIDS. Prevention Gap Report. Geneva: UNAIDS; 2016. UNAIDS. Prevention Gap Report. Geneva: UNAIDS; 2016.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Napper LE, Reynolds GL, Fisher DG. Measuring perceived susceptibility, perceived vulnerability and perceived risk of HIV infection. In: Lavino JG, Neumann RB, editors. Psychology of risk perception. Hauppauge: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.; 2010. Napper LE, Reynolds GL, Fisher DG. Measuring perceived susceptibility, perceived vulnerability and perceived risk of HIV infection. In: Lavino JG, Neumann RB, editors. Psychology of risk perception. Hauppauge: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.; 2010.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Tenkorang EY, Rajulton F, Maticka-Tyndale E. Perceived risks of HIV/AIDS and first sexual intercourse among youth in Cape Town, South Africa. AIDS Behav. 2009;13(2):234–45.CrossRefPubMed Tenkorang EY, Rajulton F, Maticka-Tyndale E. Perceived risks of HIV/AIDS and first sexual intercourse among youth in Cape Town, South Africa. AIDS Behav. 2009;13(2):234–45.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Cederbaum JA, Gilreath TD, Barman-Adhikari A. Perceived risk and condom use among adolescents in sub-Saharan Africa: a latent class analysis. Afr J Reprod Health. 2014;18(4):26–33.PubMed Cederbaum JA, Gilreath TD, Barman-Adhikari A. Perceived risk and condom use among adolescents in sub-Saharan Africa: a latent class analysis. Afr J Reprod Health. 2014;18(4):26–33.PubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Maharaj P, Cleland J. Risk perception and condom use among married or cohabiting couples in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Int Fam Plan Perspect. 2005;31(1):24–9.CrossRefPubMed Maharaj P, Cleland J. Risk perception and condom use among married or cohabiting couples in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Int Fam Plan Perspect. 2005;31(1):24–9.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Haberer JE, Kidoguchi L, Heffron R, et al. Alignment of adherence and risk for HIV acquisition in a demonstration project of pre-exposure prophylaxis among HIV serodiscordant couples in Kenya and Uganda: a prospective analysis of prevention-effective adherence. J Int AIDS Soc. 2017;20(1):21842.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Haberer JE, Kidoguchi L, Heffron R, et al. Alignment of adherence and risk for HIV acquisition in a demonstration project of pre-exposure prophylaxis among HIV serodiscordant couples in Kenya and Uganda: a prospective analysis of prevention-effective adherence. J Int AIDS Soc. 2017;20(1):21842.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat van der Straten A, Stadler J, Montgomery E, et al. Women’s experiences with oral and vaginal pre-exposure prophylaxis: the VOICE-C qualitative study in Johannesburg, South Africa. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(2):e89118.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral van der Straten A, Stadler J, Montgomery E, et al. Women’s experiences with oral and vaginal pre-exposure prophylaxis: the VOICE-C qualitative study in Johannesburg, South Africa. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(2):e89118.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Corneli A, Wang M, Agot K, Ahmed K, Lombaard J, Van Damme L. Perception of HIV risk and adherence to a daily, investigational pill for HIV prevention in FEM-PrEP. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2014;67(5):555–63.CrossRefPubMed Corneli A, Wang M, Agot K, Ahmed K, Lombaard J, Van Damme L. Perception of HIV risk and adherence to a daily, investigational pill for HIV prevention in FEM-PrEP. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2014;67(5):555–63.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat UNAIDS Inter-agency Task Team on Young People. Preventing HIV/AIDS in young people: a systematic review of the evidence from developing countries. In: Ross DA, Dick B, Ferguson J, editors. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2006. UNAIDS Inter-agency Task Team on Young People. Preventing HIV/AIDS in young people: a systematic review of the evidence from developing countries. In: Ross DA, Dick B, Ferguson J, editors. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2006.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Garnett GP, Hallett TB, Takaruza A, et al. Providing a conceptual framework for HIV prevention cascades and assessing feasibility of empirical measurement with data from east Zimbabwe: a case study. Lancet HIV. 2016;3(7):e297–306.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Garnett GP, Hallett TB, Takaruza A, et al. Providing a conceptual framework for HIV prevention cascades and assessing feasibility of empirical measurement with data from east Zimbabwe: a case study. Lancet HIV. 2016;3(7):e297–306.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Schaefer R, Gregson S, Akaruza A, et al. Spatial patterns of HIV prevalence and service use in East Zimbabwe: implications for future targeting of interventions. J Int AIDS Soc. 2017;19(1):1–10. Schaefer R, Gregson S, Akaruza A, et al. Spatial patterns of HIV prevalence and service use in East Zimbabwe: implications for future targeting of interventions. J Int AIDS Soc. 2017;19(1):1–10.
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanser F, Barnighausen T, Grapsa E, Zaidi J, Newell ML. High coverage of ART associated with decline in risk of HIV acquisition in rural KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Science. 2013;339(6122):966–71.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tanser F, Barnighausen T, Grapsa E, Zaidi J, Newell ML. High coverage of ART associated with decline in risk of HIV acquisition in rural KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Science. 2013;339(6122):966–71.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
14.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson S-J, Cherutich P, Kilonzo N, et al. Maximising the effect of combination HIV prevention through prioritisation of the people and places in greatest need: a modelling study. Lancet. 2014;384(9939):249–56.CrossRefPubMed Anderson S-J, Cherutich P, Kilonzo N, et al. Maximising the effect of combination HIV prevention through prioritisation of the people and places in greatest need: a modelling study. Lancet. 2014;384(9939):249–56.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Corneli AL, McKenna K, Headley J, et al. A descriptive analysis of perceptions of HIV risk and worry about acquiring HIV among FEM-PrEP participants who seroconverted in Bondo, Kenya, and Pretoria, South Africa. J Int AIDS Soc. 2014;17(3):19152.PubMedPubMedCentral Corneli AL, McKenna K, Headley J, et al. A descriptive analysis of perceptions of HIV risk and worry about acquiring HIV among FEM-PrEP participants who seroconverted in Bondo, Kenya, and Pretoria, South Africa. J Int AIDS Soc. 2014;17(3):19152.PubMedPubMedCentral
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Prata N, Morris L, Mazive E, Vahidnia F, Stehr M. Relationship between HIV risk perception and condom use: evidence from a population-based survey in Mozambique. Int Fam Plan Perspect. 2006;32(4):192–200.CrossRefPubMed Prata N, Morris L, Mazive E, Vahidnia F, Stehr M. Relationship between HIV risk perception and condom use: evidence from a population-based survey in Mozambique. Int Fam Plan Perspect. 2006;32(4):192–200.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Price JT, Rosenberg NE, Vansia D, et al. Predictors of HIV, HIV risk perception, and HIV worry among adolescent girls and young women in Lilongwe, Malawi. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2018;77(1):53–63.PubMedPubMedCentral Price JT, Rosenberg NE, Vansia D, et al. Predictors of HIV, HIV risk perception, and HIV worry among adolescent girls and young women in Lilongwe, Malawi. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2018;77(1):53–63.PubMedPubMedCentral
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Weinstein ND, Nicolich M. Correct and incorrect interpretations of correlations between risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Health Psychol. 1993;12(3):235–45.CrossRefPubMed Weinstein ND, Nicolich M. Correct and incorrect interpretations of correlations between risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Health Psychol. 1993;12(3):235–45.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Gerrard M, Gibbons FX, Bushman BJ. Relation between perceived vulnerability to HIV and precautionary sexual behavior. Psychol Bull. 1996;119(3):390–409.CrossRefPubMed Gerrard M, Gibbons FX, Bushman BJ. Relation between perceived vulnerability to HIV and precautionary sexual behavior. Psychol Bull. 1996;119(3):390–409.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Protogerou C, Johnson BT, Hagger MS. An integrated model of condom use in Sub-Saharan African youth: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2018;37(6):586–602.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Protogerou C, Johnson BT, Hagger MS. An integrated model of condom use in Sub-Saharan African youth: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2018;37(6):586–602.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Wood E, Li K, Miller CL, et al. Baseline self-perceived risk of HIV infection independently predicts the rate of HIV seroconversion in a prospective cohort of injection drug users. Int J Epidemiol. 2005;34(1):152–8.CrossRefPubMed Wood E, Li K, Miller CL, et al. Baseline self-perceived risk of HIV infection independently predicts the rate of HIV seroconversion in a prospective cohort of injection drug users. Int J Epidemiol. 2005;34(1):152–8.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Maughan-Brown B, Venkataramani AS. Accuracy and determinants of perceived HIV risk among young women in South Africa. BMC Public Health. 2018;18:42.CrossRef Maughan-Brown B, Venkataramani AS. Accuracy and determinants of perceived HIV risk among young women in South Africa. BMC Public Health. 2018;18:42.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Ministry of Healt and Child Care (MOHCC) Zimbabwe. Zimbabwe Population-Based HIV Impact Assessment (ZIMPHIA) 2015-16: First Report. Harare: MOHCC; 2017. Ministry of Healt and Child Care (MOHCC) Zimbabwe. Zimbabwe Population-Based HIV Impact Assessment (ZIMPHIA) 2015-16: First Report. Harare: MOHCC; 2017.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Gregson S, Garnett GP, Nyamukapa CA, et al. HIV decline associated with behavior change in eastern Zimbabwe. Science. 2006;311(5761):664–6.CrossRefPubMed Gregson S, Garnett GP, Nyamukapa CA, et al. HIV decline associated with behavior change in eastern Zimbabwe. Science. 2006;311(5761):664–6.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Gregson S, Nyamukapa C, Schumacher C, et al. Did National HIV prevention programs contribute to HIV decline in Eastern Zimbabwe? Evidence from a prospective community survey. Sex Transm Dis. 2011;38(6):475–82.PubMedPubMedCentral Gregson S, Nyamukapa C, Schumacher C, et al. Did National HIV prevention programs contribute to HIV decline in Eastern Zimbabwe? Evidence from a prospective community survey. Sex Transm Dis. 2011;38(6):475–82.PubMedPubMedCentral
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Gregson S, Mugurungi O, Eaton J, et al. Documenting and explaining the HIV decline in east Zimbabwe: the Manicaland General Population Cohort. BMJ Open. 2017;7(10):e015898.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gregson S, Mugurungi O, Eaton J, et al. Documenting and explaining the HIV decline in east Zimbabwe: the Manicaland General Population Cohort. BMJ Open. 2017;7(10):e015898.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Schaefer R, Gregson S, Eaton JW, et al. Age-disparate relationships and HIV incidence in adolescent girls and young women: evidence from Zimbabwe. AIDS. 2017;31(10):1461–70.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Schaefer R, Gregson S, Eaton JW, et al. Age-disparate relationships and HIV incidence in adolescent girls and young women: evidence from Zimbabwe. AIDS. 2017;31(10):1461–70.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Ministry of Healt and Child Care Zimbabwe. Implementation plan for HIV pre-exposure prophylaxis in Zimbabwe 2018-2020. Harare: Ministry of Health and Child Care Zimbabwe; 2018. Ministry of Healt and Child Care Zimbabwe. Implementation plan for HIV pre-exposure prophylaxis in Zimbabwe 2018-2020. Harare: Ministry of Health and Child Care Zimbabwe; 2018.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Gregson S, Zhuwau T, Ndlovu J, Nyamukapa CA. Methods to reduce social desirability bias in sex surveys in low-development settings: experience in Zimbabwe. Sex Transm Dis. 2002;29(10):568–75.CrossRefPubMed Gregson S, Zhuwau T, Ndlovu J, Nyamukapa CA. Methods to reduce social desirability bias in sex surveys in low-development settings: experience in Zimbabwe. Sex Transm Dis. 2002;29(10):568–75.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Lopman BA, Garnett GP, Mason PR, Gregson S. Individual level injection history: a lack of association with HIV incidence in rural Zimbabwe. PLoS Med. 2005;2(2):e37.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lopman BA, Garnett GP, Mason PR, Gregson S. Individual level injection history: a lack of association with HIV incidence in rural Zimbabwe. PLoS Med. 2005;2(2):e37.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Sun J. The statistical analysis of interval-censored failure time data. New York: Springer; 2006. Sun J. The statistical analysis of interval-censored failure time data. New York: Springer; 2006.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Law CG, Brookmeyer R. Effects of mid-point imputation on the analysis of doubly censored data. Stat Med. 1992;11(12):1569–78.CrossRefPubMed Law CG, Brookmeyer R. Effects of mid-point imputation on the analysis of doubly censored data. Stat Med. 1992;11(12):1569–78.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Braunstein SL, van de Wijgert JH, Nash D. HIV incidence in sub-Saharan Africa: a review of available data with implications for surveillance and prevention planning. AIDS Rev. 2009;11(3):140–56.PubMed Braunstein SL, van de Wijgert JH, Nash D. HIV incidence in sub-Saharan Africa: a review of available data with implications for surveillance and prevention planning. AIDS Rev. 2009;11(3):140–56.PubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Grabowski MK, Serwadda DM, Gray RH, et al. HIV prevention efforts and incidence of HIV in Uganda. N Engl J Med. 2017;377(22):2154–66.CrossRefPubMed Grabowski MK, Serwadda DM, Gray RH, et al. HIV prevention efforts and incidence of HIV in Uganda. N Engl J Med. 2017;377(22):2154–66.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Geis S, Maboko L, Saathoff E, et al. Risk factors for HIV-1 infection in a longitudinal, prospective cohort of adults from the Mbeya Region, Tanzania. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2011;56(5):453–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Geis S, Maboko L, Saathoff E, et al. Risk factors for HIV-1 infection in a longitudinal, prospective cohort of adults from the Mbeya Region, Tanzania. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2011;56(5):453–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Human Sciences Research Council (HSRC). South African national HIV prevalence, incidence, behaviour and communication survey, 2008. Cape Town: HSRC Press; 2009. p. 2009. Human Sciences Research Council (HSRC). South African national HIV prevalence, incidence, behaviour and communication survey, 2008. Cape Town: HSRC Press; 2009. p. 2009.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Marston M, Slaymaker E, Cremin I, et al. Trends in marriage and time spent single in sub-Saharan Africa: a comparative analysis of six population-based cohort studies and nine Demographic and Health Surveys. Sex Transm Infect. 2009;85(Suppl 1):i64–71.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Marston M, Slaymaker E, Cremin I, et al. Trends in marriage and time spent single in sub-Saharan Africa: a comparative analysis of six population-based cohort studies and nine Demographic and Health Surveys. Sex Transm Infect. 2009;85(Suppl 1):i64–71.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Evangeli M, Baker LLE, Pady K, Jones B, Wroe AL. What leads some people to think they are HIV-positive before knowing their diagnosis? A systematic review of psychological and behavioural correlates of HIV-risk perception. AIDS Care. 2016;28(8):943–53.CrossRefPubMed Evangeli M, Baker LLE, Pady K, Jones B, Wroe AL. What leads some people to think they are HIV-positive before knowing their diagnosis? A systematic review of psychological and behavioural correlates of HIV-risk perception. AIDS Care. 2016;28(8):943–53.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaufman MR, Cornish F, Zimmerman RS, Johnson BT. Health behavior change models for HIV prevention and AIDS care: practical recommendations for a multi-level approach. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2014;66(Suppl 3):S250–8.CrossRefPubMed Kaufman MR, Cornish F, Zimmerman RS, Johnson BT. Health behavior change models for HIV prevention and AIDS care: practical recommendations for a multi-level approach. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2014;66(Suppl 3):S250–8.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Thomson KA, Baeten JM, Mugo NR, Bekker L-G, Celum CL, Heffron R. Tenofovir-based oral PrEP prevents HIV infection among women. Curr Opin HIV AIDS. 2016;11(1):18–26.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Thomson KA, Baeten JM, Mugo NR, Bekker L-G, Celum CL, Heffron R. Tenofovir-based oral PrEP prevents HIV infection among women. Curr Opin HIV AIDS. 2016;11(1):18–26.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Westercamp N, Bailey RC. Acceptability of male circumcision for prevention of HIV/AIDS in sub-Saharan Africa: a review. AIDS Behav. 2007;11(3):341–55.CrossRefPubMed Westercamp N, Bailey RC. Acceptability of male circumcision for prevention of HIV/AIDS in sub-Saharan Africa: a review. AIDS Behav. 2007;11(3):341–55.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell C, Cornish F. Towards a “fourth generation” of approaches to HIV/AIDS management: creating contexts for effective community mobilisation. AIDS Care. 2010;22(sup2):1569–79.CrossRefPubMed Campbell C, Cornish F. Towards a “fourth generation” of approaches to HIV/AIDS management: creating contexts for effective community mobilisation. AIDS Care. 2010;22(sup2):1569–79.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Gupta GR, Parkhurst JO, Ogden JA, Aggleton P, Mahal A. Structural approaches to HIV prevention. Lancet. 2008;372(9640):764–75.CrossRefPubMed Gupta GR, Parkhurst JO, Ogden JA, Aggleton P, Mahal A. Structural approaches to HIV prevention. Lancet. 2008;372(9640):764–75.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Isbell MT, Kilonzo N, Mugurungi O, Bekker L-G. We neglect primary HIV prevention at our peril. Lancet HIV. 2016;3(7):e284–5.CrossRefPubMed Isbell MT, Kilonzo N, Mugurungi O, Bekker L-G. We neglect primary HIV prevention at our peril. Lancet HIV. 2016;3(7):e284–5.CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Datta S, Burns J, Maughan-Brown B, Darling M, Eyal K. Risking it all for love? Resetting beliefs about HIV risk among low-income South African teens. J Econ Behav Organ. 2015;118:184–98.CrossRef Datta S, Burns J, Maughan-Brown B, Darling M, Eyal K. Risking it all for love? Resetting beliefs about HIV risk among low-income South African teens. J Econ Behav Organ. 2015;118:184–98.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Narasimhan M, Askew I, Vermund SH. Advancing sexual and reproductive health and rights of young women at risk of HIV. Lancet Global Health. 2016;4(10):e684–5.CrossRefPubMed Narasimhan M, Askew I, Vermund SH. Advancing sexual and reproductive health and rights of young women at risk of HIV. Lancet Global Health. 2016;4(10):e684–5.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Sgaier SK, Baer J, Rutz DC, et al. Toward a systematic approach to generating demand for voluntary medical male circumcision: insights and results from field studies. Glob Health Sci Pract. 2015;3(2):209–29.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Sgaier SK, Baer J, Rutz DC, et al. Toward a systematic approach to generating demand for voluntary medical male circumcision: insights and results from field studies. Glob Health Sci Pract. 2015;3(2):209–29.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Chandra-Mouli V, Lane C, Wong S. What does not work in adolescent sexual and reproductive health: a review of evidence on interventions commonly accepted as best practices. Glob Health Sci Pract. 2015;3(3):333–40.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Chandra-Mouli V, Lane C, Wong S. What does not work in adolescent sexual and reproductive health: a review of evidence on interventions commonly accepted as best practices. Glob Health Sci Pract. 2015;3(3):333–40.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Hargreaves JR, Delany-Moretlwe S, Hallett TB, et al. The HIV prevention cascade: integrating theories of epidemiological, behavioural, and social science into programme design and monitoring. Lancet HIV. 2016;3(7):e318–22.CrossRefPubMed Hargreaves JR, Delany-Moretlwe S, Hallett TB, et al. The HIV prevention cascade: integrating theories of epidemiological, behavioural, and social science into programme design and monitoring. Lancet HIV. 2016;3(7):e318–22.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Accuracy of HIV Risk Perception in East Zimbabwe 2003–2013
verfasst von
Robin Schaefer
Ranjeeta Thomas
Constance Nyamukapa
Rufurwokuda Maswera
Noah Kadzura
Simon Gregson
Publikationsdatum
19.12.2018
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
AIDS and Behavior / Ausgabe 8/2019
Print ISSN: 1090-7165
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-3254
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10461-018-2374-0

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 8/2019

AIDS and Behavior 8/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Innere Medizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Innere Medizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.