Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Brain Structure and Function 4/2016

Open Access 02.04.2015 | Original Article

Association of white matter hyperintensities and gray matter volume with cognition in older individuals without cognitive impairment

verfasst von: Zoe Arvanitakis, Debra A. Fleischman, Konstantinos Arfanakis, Sue E. Leurgans, Lisa L. Barnes, David A. Bennett

Erschienen in: Brain Structure and Function | Ausgabe 4/2016

Abstract

Both presence of white matter hyperintensities (WMH) and smaller total gray matter volume on brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) are common findings in old age, and contribute to impaired cognition. We tested whether total WMH volume and gray matter volume had independent associations with cognition in community-dwelling individuals without dementia or mild cognitive impairment (MCI). We used data from participants of the Rush Memory and Aging Project. Brain MRI was available in 209 subjects without dementia or MCI (mean age 80; education = 15 years; 74 % women). WMH and gray matter were automatically segmented, and the total WMH and gray matter volumes were measured. Both MRI-derived measures were normalized by the intracranial volume. Cognitive data included composite measures of five different cognitive domains, based on 19 individual tests. Linear regression analyses, adjusted for age, sex, and education, were used to examine the relationship of logarithmically-transformed total WMH volume and of total gray matter volume to cognition. Larger total WMH volumes were associated with lower levels of perceptual speed (p < 0.001), but not with episodic memory, semantic memory, working memory, or visuospatial abilities (all p > 0.10). Smaller total gray matter volumes were associated with lower levels of perceptual speed (p = 0.013) and episodic memory (p = 0.001), but not with the other three cognitive domains (all p > 0.14). Larger total WMH volume was correlated with smaller total gray matter volume (p < 0.001). In a model with both MRI-derived measures included, the relation of WMH to perceptual speed remained significant (p < 0.001), while gray matter volumes were no longer related (p = 0.14). This study of older community-dwelling individuals without overt cognitive impairment suggests that the association of larger total WMH volume with lower perceptual speed is independent of total gray matter volume. These results help elucidate the pathological processes leading to lower cognitive function in aging.

Introduction

White matter hyperintensities (WMH) are brain white matter lesions with high signal on T 2-weighted fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). These lesions are believed to represent underlying pathologic changes which are variable in nature and severity, including alterations in myelin and axon structure, gliosis, and small vessel disease (Gouw et al. 2011). Clinical experience and mounting research data suggest that WMH are very common in aging and associated with a range of vascular diseases including stroke (Breteler et al. 1994; Raz et al. 2012; Burton et al. 2004). The clinical significance of WMH has been well studied, and associations have been demonstrated with impaired neurologic function including cognitive and motor impairment, as well as with death (Kerber et al. 2006; Silbert et al. 2008; Debette and Markus 2010). Yet, despite better recognition of WMH with increased availability and use of sophisticated neuroimaging technology, and the consensus that WMH should no longer be considered an “incidental” finding on neuroimaging, little information is available on the relationship of WMH with neurologic function in individuals without overt neurologic impairment. The data to date suggest that WMH are likely associated with subtle changes in neurologic function, including lower cognitive function, even among healthy older individuals without overt cognitive impairment [e.g., without Alzheimer’s disease or mild cognitive impairment (MCI)], and these results have been supported by meta-analyses (DeCarli et al. 1995, Gunning-Dixon and Raz 2000; Nordahl et al. 2006; Ishikawa et al. 2012). However, few studies have systematically examined the relationship of WMH with a range of cognitive domains among older individuals without neurologic conditions or diseases, and results of these few studies are mixed, with data suggesting associations with some domains (e.g., executive function) but not others (Vannorsdall et al. 2009; Murray et al. 2010; Hedden et al. 2012).
While a body of literature supports a relationship of gray matter to cognition in older persons, including among persons without known cognitive impairment (Jack et al. 2000; Kramer et al. 2007; Fleischman et al. 2013), few reports are available that take the effects on cognition of both WMH and gray matter volumes into account simultaneously. Indeed, among studies examining white matter together with gray matter, many had small samples of cognitively normal subjects, were restricted to MRI volumes in select brain regions, or did not measure WMH lesions (Hedden et al. 2012; Mueller et al. 2010; Taki et al. 2011; Fletcher et al. 2013; Royle et al. 2013; Wirth et al. 2013; Papp et al. 2014). No study that we know of has directly tested independence of both total WMH and total gray matter volume effects on cognition among a large group of older individuals without cognitive impairment. Studies that consider multiple brain measures simultaneously are needed to improve understanding of the interactions of neuropathologic events and the pathophysiologic cascade. Further, studies of cognitively-normal individuals are critical to shed light on the earliest factors that may play a role in neurologic decline and, as such, are important for identifying plausible strategies for prevention of cognitive impairment, and, ultimately, for improving public health.
This study examines the relation of total WMH volume to cognitive function in different cognitive domains, and tests whether the relation is independent of total gray matter volume. We used brain MRI and neuropsychologic data from more than 200 well-characterized, older community-dwelling women and men without dementia or MCI, participating in an epidemiologic study of aging, the Rush Memory and Aging Project. In the first set of analyses, we examined the relation of total WMH volume on brain MRI scans to level of cognitive function in composite measures of five separate cognitive domains. Secondary analyses determined the relative contribution of WMH to cognition, compared to that of demographic variables, and addressed whether findings were modified by age. In the second set of analyses, we examined for independence of effects of total WMH volume and total gray matter volume on cognition.

Methods

Cohort

Individuals were enrolled in an ongoing epidemiologic, cohort study of aging, the Memory and Aging Project. Older community-dwelling individuals without known dementia were invited to participate in the study. All participants consented to annual detailed clinical evaluations. The study was approved by the Rush University Medical Center Institutional Review Board, and is funded by the National Institute on Aging. Detailed methods of the cohort study are found elsewhere (Bennett et al. 2012a). Briefly, the Memory and Aging Project began enrolling participants in 1997, and 1740 individuals had enrolled as of December 2013. Brain MRI data collection began in 2009, and more than 450 individuals have undergone a scan since that time. Analyses for this study were conducted among individuals with MRI images processed to date, with data available on both WMH and gray matter volumes, in which neither dementia nor MCI was present at the time of the brain scan (see below). While this is the first study reporting our WMH results, we have previously published structural MRI results (Fleischman et al. 2010, 2014; Bis et al. 2012; Stein et al. 2012; Arfanakis et al. 2013; Chauhan et al. 2015).

Clinical evaluations

A uniform, structured, baseline clinical evaluation was administered to all subjects, including testing of cognitive function (see below). Annual follow-up evaluations were identical in all essential components. For this study, we used data from the clinical evaluation at the same follow-up cycle as the MRI scan (mean time interval between clinical evaluation and MRI scan 65.7 days, SD 57.8).
Cognitive function was evaluated using a battery of 21 individual neuropsychological tests, as previously published (Bennett et al. 2012a). The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) was used to describe the cohort and complex ideational material was used for diagnostic classification. The other 19 tests were grouped to form composite scores in five different cognitive domains (episodic memory, semantic memory, working memory, perceptual speed, and visuospatial abilities), as shown in Table 1. For each composite score, raw scores on individual tests were converted to z scores (based on the mean and SD from baseline) and z scores were averaged. A neuropsychologist, blinded to clinical data, reviewed results and summarized impairment. The clinical diagnosis of dementia followed accepted and validated criteria, as recommended by a joint working group, and were made by clinicians with expertise in the evaluation of older persons following review of all clinical data, including cognitive performance, and an in-person examination of each subject (McKhann et al. 1984).
Table 1
Cognitive tests used to form cognitive domain scores
Cognitive domain
Cognitive tests
Perceptual speed
Symbol Digit Modalities test; Number Comparison
Episodic memory
Word List Memory, Word List Recall and Word List Recognition from the procedures established by the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD); immediate and delayed recall of Story A from the Logical Memory subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised; immediate and delayed recall of the East Boston Story
Semantic memory
Verbal Fluency; an abbreviated version of the Boston Naming Test; an abbreviated version of the National Adult Reading Test
Working memory
Digit Span Forward and Backward of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised; Digit Ordering; two indices from a modified version of the Stroop Neuropsychological Test
Visuospatial ability
Items from Judgment of Line Orientation and Standard Progressive Matrices

MRI data acquisition

Brain MR imaging was conducted on a 1.5 Tesla General Electric (Waukesha, WI) MRI scanner. High-resolution T 1-weighted anatomical data were obtained using a 3D magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence with the following parameters: echo-time (TE) = 2.8 ms, repetition time (TR) = 6.3 ms, preparation time = 1000 ms, flip angle = 8°, field of view (FOV) = 24 cm × 24 cm, 160 sagittal slices, 1 mm slice thickness, no gap, 224 × 192 acquisition matrix reconstructed to a 256 × 256 image matrix, and two repetitions. T 2-weighted fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) data were collected using a 2D fast spin-echo sequence with the following parameters: TE = 120 ms, TR = 8 s, inversion time = 2 s, FOV = 24 cm × 24 cm, 42 oblique axial slices, slice thickness = 3 mm, no gap, 256 × 224 acquisition matrix reconstructed to a 256 × 256 image matrix.

Image processing

MRI data were automatically segmented to generate total gray matter and total WMH volume data. First, the two copies of T 1-weighted MPRAGE data collected on each participant were spatially co-registered and averaged. Gray matter was automatically segmented using FreeSurfer (http://​surfer.​nmr.​mgh.​harvard.​edu), as recently described elsewhere (Fleischman et al. 2014). The total gray matter volume was then measured for each participant and normalized by the corresponding intracranial volume (ICV) generated by FreeSurfer, by dividing by the corresponding ICV, as previously described (Fleischman et al. 2014). The total gray matter volume variable used in the analyses included cortical, subcortical, and other gray matter regions (including the cerebellum).
The average T 1-weighted MPRAGE data for each participant was registered to the corresponding T 2-weighted FLAIR data using affine registration (FLIRT, FMRIB, University of Oxford, UK) (Smith et al. 2004). The brain was extracted from the co-registered MPRAGE and FLAIR image volumes (BET, FMRIB, University of Oxford, UK) (Smith 2002). WMH lesions were then automatically segmented for each participant using a support vector machine classifier considering both T 1-weighted MPRAGE and T 2-weighted FLAIR information (WMLS, SBIA, University of Pennsylvania, PA) (Zacharaki et al. 2008). Maps of WMH were generated. The total volume of brain tissue affected by WMH was measured for each participant and then divided by the corresponding ICV.
For each subject, the average T 1-weighted MPRAGE data in FLAIR space were non-linearly registered to those of all other participants using the Automatic Registration Toolbox (ART) (Ardekani et al. 2005). The resulting ART transformations were averaged and applied to the map of WMH of the subject. The same approach was followed for all other subjects, transforming all WMH maps to population space for the purposes of voxel-wise analysis.

Analytic approach

We first examined basic statistical characteristics of WMH data, including distribution and outliers. Given the skewed distribution of the total WMH volumes, the values (which were first divided by ICV) were logarithmically transformed (base 10), a transformation employed by others (Gunning-Dixon and Raz 2003; Jeerakathil et al. 2004; Hedden et al. 2012).
All subsequent analyses adjusted for age (at the time of the MRI), sex, and education. The first set of analyses examined the relation of total WMH volume with level of cognition. To do this, we constructed a set of five multiple linear regression models with each of the cognitive domains scores as separate outcomes. Secondary analyses were then conducted to examine the relative effects of WMH on cognition, compared to that of basic demographic variables, and explore whether other factors (e.g., age) modify associations of total WMH volume with cognition.
The second set of analyses examined the relation of WMH volume and gray matter volume to cognition. First, we conducted linear regression analyses with the cognitive domains as separate outcomes, and included terms for age, sex, education, and gray matter volumes. Next, we obtained the partial correlation of the two MRI volumes controlling for age, sex, and education. We then constructed a set of analyses including both MRI measures in the models, to examine for independence of the relation of WMH volume and gray matter volume to cognition. We have used this analytic approach in previous studies to examine for independence of effects and potential sequence of events (Bennett et al. 2003, 2004). Analyses were programmed in SAS version 9.3 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC), and models were validated graphically and analytically.
In addition, voxel-wise analysis was conducted for those cognitive domains that were significantly associated with total WMH volume, to identify those brain regions in which presence of WMH was associated with lower cognition. Voxel-wise multiple linear regressions were adjusted for age, sex, education, and total gray matter volume. The null distribution was built using the ‘‘randomise’’ tool in FSL (FMRIB, University of Oxford, UK) and 5000 permutations of the data. Differences were considered significant at p < 0.05, family wise error (FWE) corrected. The threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) method was used to define clusters with significant differences (Smith and Nichols 2009).

Results

Subjects

There were 1740 women and men enrolled in the Memory and Aging Project, of whom 1683 had completed the baseline evaluation when the data were extracted for this study. Of these, 462 had a brain MRI scan, and 423 of these scans (92 %) passed quality control. T 1-weighted MPRAGE and T 2-weighted FLAIR data were processed on the first 333 scans. After excluding 119 subjects with MCI and another 5 subjects with dementia identified at or before the year of the MRI, there were 209 subjects without overt cognitive impairment remaining who were included in analyses for this study. Demographic, clinical, and radiographic characteristics of these individuals are described in Table 2.
Table 2
Subject characteristics
Characteristica
n = 209
Age, years
80.2 (7.2)
Female, n (%)
154 (74 %)
Education, years
15.2 (2.9)
Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score, /30
28.9 (1.2)
Perceptual speed score
0.371 (0.642)
Episodic memory score
0.602 (0.494)
Semantic memory score
0.438 (0.526)
Working memory score
0.335 (0.648)
Visuospatial ability score
0.420 (0.594)
Total gray matter volume, (tenths of % ICV)
339.6 (41.4)
Total WMH volume (% ICV)b
0.89 % (0.91 %)
Total WMH volume (% ICV), after logarithmc
−0.24 (0.40)
aMean (SD), unless otherwise specified
b WMH white matter hyperintensities; values expressed as  % of intracranial volume
cLogarithm, base 10; used in regression models

WMH and age

All subjects in this study of older community-dwelling individuals without dementia or MCI (mean age 80 years, range 60–100 years), had some WMH detected on brain MRI (mean 0.89 %-ICV, SD 0.91 %-ICV). The WMH volumes quantified ranged from 0.05 to 5.04 % of ICV, with the largest value more than 100 times the smallest value. Figure 1 shows images from individuals with WMH at the first and third quartiles of volume. The coefficient of variation of WMH volume data (values expressed as % of ICV) was 103 %, and the skewness coefficient was 2.0. To improve the suitability of regression methods, WMH as % of ICV was transformed using the base 10 logarithm. The mean value of the transformed data was −0.24 units (SD 0.40), and the distribution was less skewed (skewness = 0.2). All subsequent analyses used this transformation of total WMH volume. A Spearman correlation showed that larger WMH volumes were associated with increased age (r s = 0.41; p < 0.001).

Total WMH volume and cognition

In the first set of analyses, we examined the relation of total WMH volume to level of cognitive function, in linear regression models adjusted for age, sex, and education. We found that larger (smaller) WMH volumes were associated with lower (higher) perceptual speed scores (p < 0.001), but not with levels in the four other cognitive domains, episodic memory, semantic memory, working memory, or visuospatial abilities (all p > 0.10), as shown in Table 3.
Table 3
Relationship of total WMH volume to five cognitive domains
Composite score of cognition
Estimate
SE
p value
Perceptual speed
−0.462
0.110
<0.001
Episodic memory
−0.102
0.089
0.255
Semantic memory
−0.145
0.090
0.109
Working memory
0.032
0.122
0.792
Visuospatial ability
−0.062
0.103
0.543
Estimates are coefficient of log10 (total WMH volume) in linear regression models adjusted for age, sex, and education
We next wished to assess the magnitude of the effect of WMH on cognition. To do so, we compared the effect of total WMH volume on perceptual speed to effects of demographic variables. We constructed two models with perceptual speed as the outcome measure: one model with three demographic terms (age, sex, and education), and the other with an additional term for total WMH volume (Table 4). As shown in model 2 of Table 4, after accounting for the demographic variables of age, sex, and education, total WMH volume explained an additional 6 % of the variance in perceptual speed (20–14 = 6). This represents nearly half (6/14, or 43 %) of the variance explained by demographic variables in this sample.
Table 4
Estimated perceptual speed, with and without a term for total WMH volume
Variable
Model 1
Model 2
Adjusted R 2
0.14
0.20
Age
−0.024 (0.006), <0.001
−0.013 (0.006), 0.040
Sex
−0.040 (0.097), 0.677
−0.110 (0.094), 0.245
Education
0. 055 (0.015), <0.001
0.054 (0.014), <0.001
WMH volume
−0.462 (0.110), <0.001
Adjusted R 2 is given as a fraction. Other values are estimate (SE), p value
Because age is associated with changes in white matter and cognitive performance, including in cognitively normal adults (Westlye et al. 2012), we conducted additional analyses to examine whether age modifies the relation of total WMH volume to the cognitive domains. In linear regression models with terms for age, sex, and education, and the additional interaction term of WMH × age, we did not find evidence for any interaction (all p > 0.33), suggesting that the effect of total WMH volume on cognitive domains is independent of age.

Total WMH and gray matter volumes, and cognition

The association of total WMH volume with total gray matter volume was first examined, using a Pearson partial correlation of the two volumes controlling for age, sex, and education. We found an inverse correlation of WMH with gray matter, such that larger total WMH volume was correlated with smaller total gray matter volume (r = −0.28; p < 0.001).
Next, we examined whether total WMH volume and total gray matter volume had independent associations with cognition. We first examined the relation of total gray matter volume to the five cognitive domains, in multiple linear regression analyses adjusted for age, sex, and education. In these models, smaller total gray matter volumes were associated with lower levels of perceptual speed and episodic memory, but not with the other three cognitive domains (Table 5). Because both MRI measures (total WMH and gray matter volumes) were related to lower perceptual speed, we were next able to test whether the MRI measures had independent associations with perceptual speed. To do this, we created an additional model with terms for both MRI measures included (in addition to age, sex, and education). We found that the association of total WMH volume with perceptual speed was essentially unchanged (compared to the model without the term for gray matter volume) and remained significant, while the association of total gray matter volume with perceptual speed was reduced and no longer significant (Table 6). Figure 2 illustrates the relationship of total WMH and gray matter volumes to perceptual speed. In panel A, the solid line shows the relationship between total WMH volume and level of perceptual speed from a model adjusted for age, sex, and education. Larger WMH volumes are associated with lower levels of perceptual speed. The dotted line represents the relationship after including a term for total gray matter volume. Note that the dotted line is nearly parallel to the solid line, suggesting that gray matter volume does not affect the relationship between WMH volume and cognition (which remains essentially unchanged). In panel B, the solid line shows the relationship of total gray matter volume with level of perceptual speed, with smaller gray matter volumes being associated with lower perceptual speed. The dotted line represents the relationship after including a term for total WMH volume. Note that the dotted line has a less pronounced slope than the solid line, suggesting that the relationship between gray matter volume and cognition is reduced when accounting for WMH volume. In a separate analysis, there was no evidence for an interaction of total WMH and gray matter volumes (p = 0.93). Taken together, these results suggest that the association of increased total WMH volume with lower perceptual speed is independent of total gray matter volume.
Table 5
Relationship of total gray matter volume to five cognitive domains
Composite score of cognition
Estimate
SE
p value
Perceptual speed
0.003
0.001
0.013
Episodic memory
0.003
0.001
0.001
Semantic memory
0.001
0.001
0.237
Working memory
0.001
0.001
0.631
Visuospatial ability
0.002
0.001
0.143
All models adjusted for age, sex, and education
Table 6
Relationship of total WMH and gray matter volumes to perceptual speed
MRI measure
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Estimate (SE), p
Estimate (SE), p
Estimate (SE), p
Adjusted R 2
0.20
0.16
0.21
WMH volume
−0.462 (0.110), <0.001
−0.452 (0.114), <0.001
Gray matter volume
0.003 (0.001), 0.013
0.002 (0.001), 0.138
All models adjusted for age, sex, and education
Model 1 WMH volume only, Model 2 gray matter volume only, Model 3 both WMH and gray matter volumes
–, term not included in model
We also assessed brain voxels in which the presence of WMH was associated with a lower perceptual speed score. Using voxel-wise multiple linear regression analyses controlling for total gray matter volume, we found that the presence of WMH in a number of periventricular areas was associated with a lower perceptual speed score (Fig. 3). These areas included portions of the anterior thalamic radiations, the corpus callosum, and the corona radiata.

Discussion

In this study of more than 200 older individuals without dementia or mild cognitive impairment, we found that WMH on brain MRI were common, associated with older age, and related to cognition. Specifically, larger total WMH volumes were associated with lower levels of perceptual speed, but not with memory or other cognitive functions. The effect of total WMH volume on perceptual speed was nearly half that of the demographic variables combined, and the association of total WMH volume with perceptual speed was independent of age. Notably, when considering total WMH and gray matter simultaneously, we found that the association of larger total WMH volume with lower perceptual speed in older individuals without overt cognitive impairment was independent of total gray matter volume.
It is now well recognized that WMH are commonly observed in the brains of older persons, and that the burden of WMH increases with age (Breteler et al. 1994; Ylikoski et al. 1995; Gouw et al. 2008; Raz et al. 2012). This burden may be, in part, due to increased neurologic diseases in aging, including cerebrovascular disease (Burton et al. 2004; Rost et al. 2010). Yet, WMH are also common in middle-aged healthy adults (with one study reporting 51 % of 428 healthy subjects in their 40’s having WMH), and in older adults without known neurologic diseases or conditions (Wen et al. 2009; Silbert et al. 2008). Findings from our study are consistent with this literature. With a mean age of about 80 years and no clinically recognized cognitive impairment, all of the subjects in this study had some WMH, mostly in relatively small volumes, as would be expected in a healthy volunteer cohort.
A large body of literature shows that WMH have detrimental effects on neurologic function, including cognition (Silbert et al. 2008, 2012; Debette and Markus 2010; Wakefield et al. 2010; Kloppenborg et al. 2014). WMH have been associated with cognition among persons with a range of cognitive function including dementia, and clinical diagnoses including Alzheimer’s disease (Aggarwal et al. 2010; Smith et al. 2011; Lo et al. 2012; Carmichael et al. 2012; Maillard et al. 2012). Associations with cognition among persons with mild cognitive impairment have also been reported (Yoshita et al. 2006; Debette et al. 2007; Carmichael et al. 2010; Lo et al. 2012). However, little is known about WMH and cognition among persons without dementia (Aggarwal et al. 2010), and even less so among persons without dementia or mild cognitive impairment. Several factors may contribute to the limited knowledge in this area. Some studies have evaluated cognition in seemingly healthy older individuals, but have not excluded persons with mild cognitive dysfunction (Au et al. 2006; Wright et al. 2008; Ishikawa et al. 2012). Other studies identified cognitive impairment but employed a non-specific screening test (e.g., MMSE < 24), which does not exclude all cases with mild dementia or mild cognitive impairment (Söderlund et al. 2006; van den Heuvel et al. 2006; Wakefield et al. 2010). Finally, some studies used only a single measure of cognition or had a small sample (e.g., <50 subjects) (van der Flier et al. 2005; Silbert et al. 2009; Raji et al. 2012).
A few larger studies of WMH and cognition in the elderly, specifically excluding persons with dementia and mild cognitive impairment, have examined the association of WMH with different cognitive domains. In one study, WMH burden was related to performance on timed tasks (Vannorsdall et al. 2009). Two subsequent studies showed an association of WMH with lower executive function but not memory (Murray et al. 2010; Hedden et al. 2012). Here, we found that larger total WMH volume was related to lower scores on a composite measure of perceptual speed. Our finding contributes to the mounting evidence showing that greater WMH is associated with lower cognition, and lower perceptual speed in particular, in individuals without overt cognitive impairment. Further, while the effect of WMH on cognition may be comparable to that of age as suggested by one study (Hedden et al. 2012), we found that WMH volume accounted for nearly half of the variance in perceptual speed explained by demographic variables including age, underscoring the strength of the effect of WMH on cognition in older individuals without overt cognitive impairment.
Factors accounting for the relation of WMH to cognition remain to be elucidated. As increasing age is associated with WMH and also with cognitive impairment, we considered the role of age in our study. All analyses controlled for age, yet we found an association of larger WMH volume with lower cognition, suggesting that age is not a crucial confounder in this relationship. Furthermore, we found no evidence of modification by age of the association between WMH and cognition, suggesting that the relation of WMH to cognition is independent of age. Additional research is needed that examines other factors which may modify the relationship of WMH with cognition among overtly healthy older individuals. Plausible factors include genetic factors, vascular risk factors and diseases (such as hypertension and diabetes), inflammation, and oxidative stress (Novak et al. 2006; Wright et al. 2009; Xu et al. 2010; Satizabal et al. 2012; Raz et al. 2012).
Few studies have examined effects of both WMH and gray matter volumes on different cognitive domains. We are aware of only one other study which considered both total WMH and total gray matter volumes that included persons with normal cognition (He et al. 2012). In this clinic-based study of persons with a range of cognitive function (47 with dementia, 65 with MCI, and 97 with normal cognition), WMH and gray matter volume were separately associated with episodic memory, but in analyses taking both MRI measures into account simultaneously, only gray matter remained associated (He et al. 2012). The authors concluded that gray matter is more closely related to cognition than WMH. A more recent study included exclusively persons without dementia (n = 81, all with MMSE ≥ 24), and considered volume in a specific gray matter region (the hippocampus), in addition to WMH (Papp et al. 2014). In this study, WMH and hippocampal volumes were independently associated with processing speed when both MRI measures were considered simultaneously (Papp et al. 2014). Thus, while research has been done in persons without dementia as noted above, we are not aware of any other study in older persons that also restricted their study to those without mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and directly examined the effects simultaneously of both total WMH and total gray matter volumes on different cognitive domains. In comparison to the studies by He et al. and Papp et al., we found, in about 200 community-dwelling persons without dementia or MCI, that larger total WMH volume and smaller total gray matter volume were each separately associated with lower perceptual speed, but when taking both MRI measures into account, WMH remained related to perceptual speed while gray matter volume was no longer related. These results suggest that the association of WMH volume with perceptual speed is independent of gray matter volume. Furthermore, the results suggest that the relationship of gray matter volume to cognition may be accounted for, at least in part, by WMH volume. Also, our finding of an association of gray matter volume with episodic memory, similar to the finding by He et al. (2012) suggests that the relation of MRI data to cognition may vary depending on the particular domain of cognition being examined.
We found regional specificity of the association of WMH with cognition, consistent with a recently published study of adults with a lower age on average and high cognitive function (mean age 60 years, and all with MMSE ≥ 25; Birdsill et al. 2014). In that study, authors exploited voxel-wise analyses and found an association of WMH, most notably in the superior corona radiata, with lower cognitive speed and flexibility (Birdsill et al. 2014). Our study extends this finding, by showing that the periventricular brain regions with WMH, including the corona radiata, and also the thalamic radiations and corpus callosum, play a role in the association of WMH with lower perceptual speed, after controlling for total gray matter volume. While the literature has previously suggested that periventricular lesions are associated with impaired perceptual speed, further studies using modern imaging and analytic techniques are needed to examine regional relationships of WMH with cognition (van den Heuvel et al. 2006).
There are several possible interpretations of our results evaluating both WMH and gray matter volumes. First, WMH volume may be a confounder in the relation of gray matter volume with cognition. Yet, the association of gray matter volume with cognition is biologically plausible and extensive literature supports such an association. Second, early pathologic processes may occur in the gray matter and lead to loss of gray matter volume (e.g., neuronal cell body dysfunction and neuronal death, resulting in gray matter atrophy), and may then be followed by changes in the white matter (e.g., changes in axons or the myelin covering the axons, and WMH), and then ultimately lead to brain dysfunction (e.g., lower cognitive function). In this case, WMH would mediate the relationship between gray matter volume and cognition. This sequence of events in the pathophysiologic cascade, in which WMH are more closely related to cognition than gray matter, contrasts with a previously published study (He et al. 2012). The discrepant results may be due to differences in source of participants (e.g., community-based individuals without overt cognitive impairment vs. persons with a range of cognitive function). Indeed, the associations of brain pathology with cognition may not be the same across a spectrum of health/disease. It is possible that once individuals develop cognitive impairment or dementia, that gray matter volumes become a more important factor in the association with cognitive function, compared to WMH. By analogy, we have previously observed differing results in the relation of two Alzheimer’s disease pathology measures (amyloid and neurofibrillary tangles) to cognition, in studies restricted to persons without cognitive impairment vs. not (Bennett et al. 2004, 2012b). In addition, discrepant results regarding the importance of WMH may also be due to differences in study design and methodology, including MRI data acquisition and image processing, neuropsychological test selection, data summary and analyses. Further study is clearly needed to definitively establish the relationship of WMH, gray matter, and cognition. Elucidation of the pathological processes that lead to lower cognitive function, and of sequences of events in that pathway (e.g., whether reduced gray matter volume occurs before WMH, or vice versa), is important to better understand mechanisms of aging and disease, and to inform on targets for intervention to prevent cognitive decline. To our knowledge, this study is the first that has tested independence of total WMH and gray matter volume effects on different cognitive domains in older individuals without overt cognitive impairment.
Weaknesses of this study should be noted. First, our aim was to examine for independent effects of total WMH and total gray matter volumes on cognition, and thus we did not examine relationships in analyses that considered both regional WMH and regional gray matter data. While our study’s novelty is enhanced by voxel-wise analyses of the association of WMH with perceptual speed, further examination of the relation of regional MRI data to cognitive domains is likely to shed additional light on underlying pathophysiologic processes and will need to be explored in subsequent studies. Second, this study presents cross-sectional analyses of the relation of WMH to cognition. Data on the evolution of WMH changes is expected to be more informative and we have plans to analyze the relation of change in WMH to cognition in the future as data become available. Third, while secondary analyses tested for effect modification by age, we did not take other factors, such as vascular factors, into account. We are collecting MRI data on brain infarcts and, as all participants were enrolled in a clinical-pathologic cohort study with a high autopsy rate, neuropathologic data on cerebral infarcts will become available for future analyses. We plan to examine this issue more directly in individuals with clinical, brain MRI, and neuropathologic data.
Strengths of the study are also worth noting. First, analyses were conducted in older community-dwelling women and men who were clinically well-characterized, and did not have dementia or mild cognitive impairment. This allowed for the examination of the relationship of WMH to cognition in a group of older individuals without overt cognitive impairment. Second, detailed neuropsychological evaluations proximate to the MRI scan were conducted, and outcome measures included five different cognitive domains. Each cognitive domain was a composite measure based on two or more individual tests, thus decreasing ceiling effects. Ceiling effects are of particular concern in this study given the high cognitive performance of individuals included in analyses. Finally, several strengths of the study are derived from the methods used to assess and analyze WMH data. Total brain WMH volume data were collected in an automated fashion, removing user bias, and were based on information from both T 1-weighted and FLAIR images, improving robustness of segmentation and lending internal validity to the study. Furthermore, the consideration of WMH as continuous measures of volume of lesions, rather than as categories, increased the power to detect associations with cognition. Finally, we used voxel-wise analyses to examine brain regions in which presence of WMH was associated with lower cognitive function.

Acknowledgments

Authors wish to thank participants enrolled in the Memory and Aging Project. The study was funded by the National Institutes of Health (R01AG17917, R01AG40039, P20MD6886) and the Illinois Department of Public Health. Authors are grateful to the Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center staff, in particular Niranjini Rajendran for scan post-processing, John Gibbons for data management, Woojeong Bang and Donna Esbjornson for statistical analyses, and Traci Colvin, Tracey Nowakowski, and Kuheli Mukherjee for the study coordination.

Conflict of interest

Authors have no actual or potential conflicts of interest.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Ardekani BA, Guckemus S, Bachman A, Hoptman MJ, Wojtaszek M, Nierenberg J (2005) Quantitative comparison of algorithms for inter-subject registration of 3D volumetric brain MRI scans. J Neurosci Methods 42:67–76CrossRef Ardekani BA, Guckemus S, Bachman A, Hoptman MJ, Wojtaszek M, Nierenberg J (2005) Quantitative comparison of algorithms for inter-subject registration of 3D volumetric brain MRI scans. J Neurosci Methods 42:67–76CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Au R, Massaro JM, Wolf PA, Young ME, Beiser A, Seshadri S, D’Agostino RB, DeCarli C (2006) Association of white matter hyperintensity volume with decreased cognitive functioning: the Framingham Heart Study. Arch Neurol 63(2):246–250CrossRefPubMed Au R, Massaro JM, Wolf PA, Young ME, Beiser A, Seshadri S, D’Agostino RB, DeCarli C (2006) Association of white matter hyperintensity volume with decreased cognitive functioning: the Framingham Heart Study. Arch Neurol 63(2):246–250CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Aggarwal NT, Arnold SE, Cochran EJ, Berry-Kravis E, Bienias JL (2003) Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele, AD pathology, and the clinical expression of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 60(2):246–252CrossRefPubMed Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Aggarwal NT, Arnold SE, Cochran EJ, Berry-Kravis E, Bienias JL (2003) Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele, AD pathology, and the clinical expression of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 60(2):246–252CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Arnold SE (2004) Neurofibrillary tangles mediate the association of amyloid load with clinical Alzheimer disease and level of cognitive function. Arch Neurol 61(3):378–384CrossRefPubMed Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Arnold SE (2004) Neurofibrillary tangles mediate the association of amyloid load with clinical Alzheimer disease and level of cognitive function. Arch Neurol 61(3):378–384CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Buchman AS, Barnes LL, Boyle PA, Wilson RS (2012a) Overview and findings from the Rush Memory and Aging Project. Curr Alzheimer Res 9(6):646–663CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Buchman AS, Barnes LL, Boyle PA, Wilson RS (2012a) Overview and findings from the Rush Memory and Aging Project. Curr Alzheimer Res 9(6):646–663CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Birdsill AC, Koscik RL, Jonaitis EM, Johnson SC, Okonkwo OC, Hermann BP, Larue A, Sager MA, Bendlin BB (2014) Regional white matter hyperintensities: aging, Alzheimer’s disease risk, and cognitive function. Neurobiol Aging 35(4):769–776 (Epub 2013 Oct 10)CrossRefPubMed Birdsill AC, Koscik RL, Jonaitis EM, Johnson SC, Okonkwo OC, Hermann BP, Larue A, Sager MA, Bendlin BB (2014) Regional white matter hyperintensities: aging, Alzheimer’s disease risk, and cognitive function. Neurobiol Aging 35(4):769–776 (Epub 2013 Oct 10)CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bis JC, DeCarli C, Smith AV, van der Lijn F, Crivello F, Fornage M, Debette S, Shulman JM, Schmidt H, Srikanth V, et al. Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology Consortium (2012) Common variants at 12q14 and 12q24 are associated with hippocampal volume. Nat Genet 44(5):545–551 Bis JC, DeCarli C, Smith AV, van der Lijn F, Crivello F, Fornage M, Debette S, Shulman JM, Schmidt H, Srikanth V, et al. Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology Consortium (2012) Common variants at 12q14 and 12q24 are associated with hippocampal volume. Nat Genet 44(5):545–551
Zurück zum Zitat Breteler MM, van Swieten JC, Bots ML, Grobbee DE, Claus JJ, van den Hout JH, van Harskamp F, Tanghe HL, de Jong PT, van Gijn J et al (1994) Cerebral white matter lesions, vascular risk factors, and cognitive function in a population-based study: the Rotterdam Study. Neurology 44(7):1246–1252CrossRefPubMed Breteler MM, van Swieten JC, Bots ML, Grobbee DE, Claus JJ, van den Hout JH, van Harskamp F, Tanghe HL, de Jong PT, van Gijn J et al (1994) Cerebral white matter lesions, vascular risk factors, and cognitive function in a population-based study: the Rotterdam Study. Neurology 44(7):1246–1252CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Burton EJ, Kenny RA, O’Brien J, Stephens S, Bradbury M, Rowan E, Kalaria R, Firbank M, Wesnes K, Ballard C (2004) White matter hyperintensities are associated with impairment of memory, attention, and global cognitive performance in older stroke patients. Stroke 35(6):1270–1275 (Epub 2004 Apr 29)CrossRefPubMed Burton EJ, Kenny RA, O’Brien J, Stephens S, Bradbury M, Rowan E, Kalaria R, Firbank M, Wesnes K, Ballard C (2004) White matter hyperintensities are associated with impairment of memory, attention, and global cognitive performance in older stroke patients. Stroke 35(6):1270–1275 (Epub 2004 Apr 29)CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Carmichael O, Schwarz C, Drucker D, Fletcher E, Harvey D, Beckett L, Jack CR Jr, Weiner M, DeCarli C, Initiative Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging (2010) Longitudinal changes in white matter disease and cognition in the first year of the Alzheimer disease neuroimaging initiative. Arch Neurol 67(11):1370–1378. doi:10.1001/archneurol.2010.284 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Carmichael O, Schwarz C, Drucker D, Fletcher E, Harvey D, Beckett L, Jack CR Jr, Weiner M, DeCarli C, Initiative Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging (2010) Longitudinal changes in white matter disease and cognition in the first year of the Alzheimer disease neuroimaging initiative. Arch Neurol 67(11):1370–1378. doi:10.​1001/​archneurol.​2010.​284 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Debette S, Bombois S, Bruandet A, Delbeuck X, Lepoittevin S, Delmaire C, Leys D, Pasquier F (2007) Subcortical hyperintensities are associated with cognitive decline in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Stroke 38(11):2924–2930 (Epub 2007 Sep 20)CrossRefPubMed Debette S, Bombois S, Bruandet A, Delbeuck X, Lepoittevin S, Delmaire C, Leys D, Pasquier F (2007) Subcortical hyperintensities are associated with cognitive decline in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Stroke 38(11):2924–2930 (Epub 2007 Sep 20)CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat DeCarli C, Murphy DG, Tranh M, Grady CL, Haxby JV, Gillette JA, Salerno JA, Gonzales-Aviles A, Horwitz B, Rapoport SI et al (1995) The effect of white matter hyperintensity volume on brain structure, cognitive performance, and cerebral metabolism of glucose in 51 healthy adults. Neurology 45(11):2077–2084CrossRefPubMed DeCarli C, Murphy DG, Tranh M, Grady CL, Haxby JV, Gillette JA, Salerno JA, Gonzales-Aviles A, Horwitz B, Rapoport SI et al (1995) The effect of white matter hyperintensity volume on brain structure, cognitive performance, and cerebral metabolism of glucose in 51 healthy adults. Neurology 45(11):2077–2084CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Fleischman DA, Yu L, Arfanakis K, Han SD, Barnes LL, Arvanitakis Z, Boyle PA, Bennett DA (2013) Faster cognitive decline in the years prior to MR imaging is associated with smaller hippocampal volumes in cognitively healthy older persons. Front Aging Neurosci 5:21. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2013.00021 (eCollection 2013)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fleischman DA, Yu L, Arfanakis K, Han SD, Barnes LL, Arvanitakis Z, Boyle PA, Bennett DA (2013) Faster cognitive decline in the years prior to MR imaging is associated with smaller hippocampal volumes in cognitively healthy older persons. Front Aging Neurosci 5:21. doi:10.​3389/​fnagi.​2013.​00021 (eCollection 2013)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Fleischman DA, Leurgans S, Arfanakis K, Arvanitakis Z, Barnes LL, Boyle PA, Han SD, Bennett DA (2014) Gray-matter macrostructure in cognitively healthy older persons: associations with age and cognition. Brain Struct Funct 219(6):2029–2049CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fleischman DA, Leurgans S, Arfanakis K, Arvanitakis Z, Barnes LL, Boyle PA, Han SD, Bennett DA (2014) Gray-matter macrostructure in cognitively healthy older persons: associations with age and cognition. Brain Struct Funct 219(6):2029–2049CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Gouw AA, van der Flier WM, Fazekas F, van Straaten EC, Pantoni L, Poggesi A, Inzitari D, Erkinjuntti T, Wahlund LO, Waldemar G, Schmidt R, Scheltens P, Barkhof F, LADIS Study Group (2008) Progression of white matter hyperintensities and incidence of new lacunes over a 3-year period: the Leukoaraiosis and Disability study. Stroke 39(5):1414–1420. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.498535 (Epub 2008 Mar 6)CrossRefPubMed Gouw AA, van der Flier WM, Fazekas F, van Straaten EC, Pantoni L, Poggesi A, Inzitari D, Erkinjuntti T, Wahlund LO, Waldemar G, Schmidt R, Scheltens P, Barkhof F, LADIS Study Group (2008) Progression of white matter hyperintensities and incidence of new lacunes over a 3-year period: the Leukoaraiosis and Disability study. Stroke 39(5):1414–1420. doi:10.​1161/​STROKEAHA.​107.​498535 (Epub 2008 Mar 6)CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gouw AA, Seewann A, van der Flier WM, Barkhof F, Rozemuller AM, Scheltens P, Geurts JJ (2011) Heterogeneity of small vessel disease: a systematic review of MRI and histopathology correlations. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 82(2):126–135. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2009.204685 (Epub 2010 Oct 9)CrossRefPubMed Gouw AA, Seewann A, van der Flier WM, Barkhof F, Rozemuller AM, Scheltens P, Geurts JJ (2011) Heterogeneity of small vessel disease: a systematic review of MRI and histopathology correlations. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 82(2):126–135. doi:10.​1136/​jnnp.​2009.​204685 (Epub 2010 Oct 9)CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gunning-Dixon FM, Raz N (2000) The cognitive correlates of white matter abnormalities in normal aging: a quantitative review. Neuropsychology 14(2):224–232CrossRefPubMed Gunning-Dixon FM, Raz N (2000) The cognitive correlates of white matter abnormalities in normal aging: a quantitative review. Neuropsychology 14(2):224–232CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gunning-Dixon FM, Raz N (2003) Neuroanatomical correlates of selected executive functions in middle-aged and older adults: a prospective MRI study. Neuropsychologia 41(14):1929–1941CrossRefPubMed Gunning-Dixon FM, Raz N (2003) Neuroanatomical correlates of selected executive functions in middle-aged and older adults: a prospective MRI study. Neuropsychologia 41(14):1929–1941CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat He J, Wong VS, Fletcher E, Maillard P, Lee DY, Iosif AM, Singh B, Martinez O, Roach AE, Lockhart SN, Beckett L, Mungas D, Farias ST, Carmichael O, DeCarli C (2012) The contributions of MRI-based measures of gray matter, white matter hyperintensity, and white matter integrity to late-life cognition. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 33(9):1797–1803. doi:10.3174/ajnr.A3048 (Epub 2012 Apr 26)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral He J, Wong VS, Fletcher E, Maillard P, Lee DY, Iosif AM, Singh B, Martinez O, Roach AE, Lockhart SN, Beckett L, Mungas D, Farias ST, Carmichael O, DeCarli C (2012) The contributions of MRI-based measures of gray matter, white matter hyperintensity, and white matter integrity to late-life cognition. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 33(9):1797–1803. doi:10.​3174/​ajnr.​A3048 (Epub 2012 Apr 26)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Jack CR Jr, Petersen RC, Xu Y, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Boeve BF, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E (2000) Rates of hippocampal atrophy correlate with change in clinical status in aging and AD. Neurology 55(4):484–489CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Jack CR Jr, Petersen RC, Xu Y, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Boeve BF, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E (2000) Rates of hippocampal atrophy correlate with change in clinical status in aging and AD. Neurology 55(4):484–489CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Jeerakathil T, Wolf PA, Beiser A, Massaro J, Seshadri S, D’Agostino RB, DeCarli C (2004) Stroke risk profile predicts white matter hyperintensity volume: the Framingham Study. Stroke 35(8):1857–1861 (Epub 2004 Jun 24)CrossRefPubMed Jeerakathil T, Wolf PA, Beiser A, Massaro J, Seshadri S, D’Agostino RB, DeCarli C (2004) Stroke risk profile predicts white matter hyperintensity volume: the Framingham Study. Stroke 35(8):1857–1861 (Epub 2004 Jun 24)CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kerber KA, Whitman GT, Brown DL, Baloh RW (2006) Increased risk of death in community-dwelling older people with white matter hyperintensities on MRI. J Neurol Sci 250(1–2):33–38 (Epub 2006 Aug 4)CrossRefPubMed Kerber KA, Whitman GT, Brown DL, Baloh RW (2006) Increased risk of death in community-dwelling older people with white matter hyperintensities on MRI. J Neurol Sci 250(1–2):33–38 (Epub 2006 Aug 4)CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kloppenborg RP, Nederkoorn PJ, Geerlings MI, van den Berg E (2014) Presence and progression of white matter hyperintensities and cognition: a meta-analysis. Neurology 82(23):2127–2138 (Epub 2014 May 9)CrossRefPubMed Kloppenborg RP, Nederkoorn PJ, Geerlings MI, van den Berg E (2014) Presence and progression of white matter hyperintensities and cognition: a meta-analysis. Neurology 82(23):2127–2138 (Epub 2014 May 9)CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kramer JH, Mungas D, Reed BR, Wetzel ME, Burnett MM, Miller BL, Weiner MW, Chui HC (2007) Longitudinal MRI and cognitive change in healthy elderly. Neuropsychology 21(4):412–418CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kramer JH, Mungas D, Reed BR, Wetzel ME, Burnett MM, Miller BL, Weiner MW, Chui HC (2007) Longitudinal MRI and cognitive change in healthy elderly. Neuropsychology 21(4):412–418CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Lo RY, Jagust WJ, Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2012) Vascular burden and Alzheimer disease pathologic progression. Neurology 79(13):1349–1355 (Epub 2012 Sep 12)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lo RY, Jagust WJ, Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2012) Vascular burden and Alzheimer disease pathologic progression. Neurology 79(13):1349–1355 (Epub 2012 Sep 12)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM (1984) Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 34(7):939–944CrossRefPubMed McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM (1984) Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 34(7):939–944CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Murray ME, Senjem ML, Petersen RC, Hollman JH, Preboske GM, Weigand SD, Knopman DS, Ferman TJ, Dickson DW, Jack CR Jr (2010) Functional impact of white matter hyperintensities in cognitively normal elderly subjects. Arch Neurol 67(11):1379–1385. doi:10.1001/archneurol.2010.280 PubMedPubMedCentral Murray ME, Senjem ML, Petersen RC, Hollman JH, Preboske GM, Weigand SD, Knopman DS, Ferman TJ, Dickson DW, Jack CR Jr (2010) Functional impact of white matter hyperintensities in cognitively normal elderly subjects. Arch Neurol 67(11):1379–1385. doi:10.​1001/​archneurol.​2010.​280 PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Nordahl CW, Ranganath C, Yonelinas AP, Decarli C, Fletcher E, Jagust WJ (2006) White matter changes compromise prefrontal cortex function in healthy elderly individuals. J Cogn Neurosci 18(3):418–429CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Nordahl CW, Ranganath C, Yonelinas AP, Decarli C, Fletcher E, Jagust WJ (2006) White matter changes compromise prefrontal cortex function in healthy elderly individuals. J Cogn Neurosci 18(3):418–429CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Novak V, Last D, Alsop DC, Abduljalil AM, Hu K, Lepicovsky L, Cavallerano J, Lipsitz LA (2006) Cerebral blood flow velocity and periventricular white matter hyperintensities in type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care 29(7):1529–1534CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Novak V, Last D, Alsop DC, Abduljalil AM, Hu K, Lepicovsky L, Cavallerano J, Lipsitz LA (2006) Cerebral blood flow velocity and periventricular white matter hyperintensities in type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care 29(7):1529–1534CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Raji CA, Lopez OL, Kuller LH, Carmichael OT, Longstreth WT Jr, Gach HM, Boardman J, Bernick CB, Thompson PM, Becker JT (2012) White matter lesions and brain gray matter volume in cognitively normal elders. Neurobiol Aging 33(4):834.e7–834.e16. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.08.010 (Epub 2011 Sep 23)CrossRef Raji CA, Lopez OL, Kuller LH, Carmichael OT, Longstreth WT Jr, Gach HM, Boardman J, Bernick CB, Thompson PM, Becker JT (2012) White matter lesions and brain gray matter volume in cognitively normal elders. Neurobiol Aging 33(4):834.e7–834.e16. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neurobiolaging.​2011.​08.​010 (Epub 2011 Sep 23)CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Raz N, Yang Y, Dahle CL (1822) Land S (2012) Volume of white matter hyperintensities in healthy adults: contribution of age, vascular risk factors, and inflammation-related genetic variants. Biochim Biophys Acta 3:361–369. doi:10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.08.007 (Epub 2011 Aug 25) Raz N, Yang Y, Dahle CL (1822) Land S (2012) Volume of white matter hyperintensities in healthy adults: contribution of age, vascular risk factors, and inflammation-related genetic variants. Biochim Biophys Acta 3:361–369. doi:10.​1016/​j.​bbadis.​2011.​08.​007 (Epub 2011 Aug 25)
Zurück zum Zitat Rost NS, Rahman RM, Biffi A, Smith EE, Kanakis A, Fitzpatrick K, Lima F, Worrall BB, Meschia JF, Brown RD Jr, Brott TG, Sorensen AG, Greenberg SM, Furie KL, Rosand J (2010) White matter hyperintensity volume is increased in small vessel stroke subtypes. Neurology 75(19):1670–1677. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181fc279a CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rost NS, Rahman RM, Biffi A, Smith EE, Kanakis A, Fitzpatrick K, Lima F, Worrall BB, Meschia JF, Brown RD Jr, Brott TG, Sorensen AG, Greenberg SM, Furie KL, Rosand J (2010) White matter hyperintensity volume is increased in small vessel stroke subtypes. Neurology 75(19):1670–1677. doi:10.​1212/​WNL.​0b013e3181fc279a​ CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Satizabal CL, Zhu YC, Mazoyer B, Dufouil C, Tzourio C (2012) Circulating IL-6 and CRP are associated with MRI findings in the elderly: the 3C-Dijon Study. Neurology 78(10):720–727CrossRefPubMed Satizabal CL, Zhu YC, Mazoyer B, Dufouil C, Tzourio C (2012) Circulating IL-6 and CRP are associated with MRI findings in the elderly: the 3C-Dijon Study. Neurology 78(10):720–727CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Smith SM, Nichols TE (2009) Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localization in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44:83–98CrossRefPubMed Smith SM, Nichols TE (2009) Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localization in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44:83–98CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23(Suppl 1):S208–S219CrossRefPubMed Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23(Suppl 1):S208–S219CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Söderlund H, Nilsson LG, Berger K, Breteler MM, Dufouil C, Fuhrer R, Giampaoli S, Hofman A, Pajak A, de Ridder M, Sans S, Schmidt R, Launer LJ (2006) Cerebral changes on MRI and cognitive function: the CASCADE study. Neurobiol Aging 27(1):16–23CrossRefPubMed Söderlund H, Nilsson LG, Berger K, Breteler MM, Dufouil C, Fuhrer R, Giampaoli S, Hofman A, Pajak A, de Ridder M, Sans S, Schmidt R, Launer LJ (2006) Cerebral changes on MRI and cognitive function: the CASCADE study. Neurobiol Aging 27(1):16–23CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Stein JL, Medland SE, Vasquez AA, Hibar DP, Senstad RE, Winkler AM, Toro R, Appel K, Bartecek R, Bergmann Ø, Enhancing Neuro Imaging Genetics through Meta-Analysis Consortium et al (2012) Identification of common variants associated with human hippocampal and intracranial volumes. Nat Genet 44(5):552–561. doi:10.1038/ng.2250 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Stein JL, Medland SE, Vasquez AA, Hibar DP, Senstad RE, Winkler AM, Toro R, Appel K, Bartecek R, Bergmann Ø, Enhancing Neuro Imaging Genetics through Meta-Analysis Consortium et al (2012) Identification of common variants associated with human hippocampal and intracranial volumes. Nat Genet 44(5):552–561. doi:10.​1038/​ng.​2250 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat van der Flier WM, van Straaten EC, Barkhof F, Verdelho A, Madureira S, Pantoni L, Inzitari D, Erkinjuntti T, Crisby M, Waldemar G, Schmidt R, Fazekas F, Scheltens P (2005) Small vessel disease and general cognitive function in nondisabled elderly: the LADIS study. Stroke 36(10):2116–2120CrossRefPubMed van der Flier WM, van Straaten EC, Barkhof F, Verdelho A, Madureira S, Pantoni L, Inzitari D, Erkinjuntti T, Crisby M, Waldemar G, Schmidt R, Fazekas F, Scheltens P (2005) Small vessel disease and general cognitive function in nondisabled elderly: the LADIS study. Stroke 36(10):2116–2120CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat van den Heuvel DM, ten Dam VH, de Craen AJ, Admiraal-Behloul F, Olofsen H, Bollen EL, Jolles J, Murray HM, Blauw GJ, Westendorp RG, van Buchem MA (2006) Increase in periventricular white matter hyperintensities parallels decline in mental processing speed in a non-demented elderly population. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 77(2):149–153CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral van den Heuvel DM, ten Dam VH, de Craen AJ, Admiraal-Behloul F, Olofsen H, Bollen EL, Jolles J, Murray HM, Blauw GJ, Westendorp RG, van Buchem MA (2006) Increase in periventricular white matter hyperintensities parallels decline in mental processing speed in a non-demented elderly population. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 77(2):149–153CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Wen W, Sachdev PS, Li JJ, Chen X, Anstey KJ (2009) White matter hyperintensities in the forties: their prevalence and topography in an epidemiological sample aged 44–48. Hum Brain Mapp 30(4):1155–1167. doi:10.1002/hbm.20586 CrossRefPubMed Wen W, Sachdev PS, Li JJ, Chen X, Anstey KJ (2009) White matter hyperintensities in the forties: their prevalence and topography in an epidemiological sample aged 44–48. Hum Brain Mapp 30(4):1155–1167. doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​20586 CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wirth M, Villeneuve S, Haase CM, Madison CM, Oh H, Landau SM, Rabinovici GD, Jagust WJ (2013) Associations between Alzheimer disease biomarkers, neurodegeneration, and cognition in cognitively normal older people. JAMA Neurol 70(12):1512–1519. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.4013 PubMed Wirth M, Villeneuve S, Haase CM, Madison CM, Oh H, Landau SM, Rabinovici GD, Jagust WJ (2013) Associations between Alzheimer disease biomarkers, neurodegeneration, and cognition in cognitively normal older people. JAMA Neurol 70(12):1512–1519. doi:10.​1001/​jamaneurol.​2013.​4013 PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Xu H, Stamova B, Jickling G, Tian Y, Zhan X, Ander BP, Liu D, Turner R, Rosand J, Goldstein LB, Furie KL, Verro P, Johnston SC, Sharp FR, Decarli CS (2010) Distinctive RNA expression profiles in blood associated with white matter hyperintensities in brain. Stroke 41(12):2744–2749. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.591875 (Epub 2010 Oct 21)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Xu H, Stamova B, Jickling G, Tian Y, Zhan X, Ander BP, Liu D, Turner R, Rosand J, Goldstein LB, Furie KL, Verro P, Johnston SC, Sharp FR, Decarli CS (2010) Distinctive RNA expression profiles in blood associated with white matter hyperintensities in brain. Stroke 41(12):2744–2749. doi:10.​1161/​STROKEAHA.​110.​591875 (Epub 2010 Oct 21)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Ylikoski A, Erkinjuntti T, Raininko R, Sarna S, Sulkava R, Tilvis R (1995) White matter hyperintensities on MRI in the neurologically nondiseased elderly. Analysis of cohorts of consecutive subjects aged 55–85 years living at home. Stroke 26(7):1171–1177CrossRefPubMed Ylikoski A, Erkinjuntti T, Raininko R, Sarna S, Sulkava R, Tilvis R (1995) White matter hyperintensities on MRI in the neurologically nondiseased elderly. Analysis of cohorts of consecutive subjects aged 55–85 years living at home. Stroke 26(7):1171–1177CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Yoshita M, Fletcher E, Harvey D, Ortega M, Martinez O, Mungas DM, Reed BR, DeCarli CS (2006) Extent and distribution of white matter hyperintensities in normal aging, MCI, and AD. Neurology 67(12):2192–2198CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Yoshita M, Fletcher E, Harvey D, Ortega M, Martinez O, Mungas DM, Reed BR, DeCarli CS (2006) Extent and distribution of white matter hyperintensities in normal aging, MCI, and AD. Neurology 67(12):2192–2198CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Zacharaki EI, Kanterakis S, Bryan RN, Davatzikos C (2008) Measuring brain lesion progression with a supervised tissue classification system. Med Image Comput Comput Assist Interv 11(Pt 1):620–627PubMed Zacharaki EI, Kanterakis S, Bryan RN, Davatzikos C (2008) Measuring brain lesion progression with a supervised tissue classification system. Med Image Comput Comput Assist Interv 11(Pt 1):620–627PubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Association of white matter hyperintensities and gray matter volume with cognition in older individuals without cognitive impairment
verfasst von
Zoe Arvanitakis
Debra A. Fleischman
Konstantinos Arfanakis
Sue E. Leurgans
Lisa L. Barnes
David A. Bennett
Publikationsdatum
02.04.2015
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Brain Structure and Function / Ausgabe 4/2016
Print ISSN: 1863-2653
Elektronische ISSN: 1863-2661
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-015-1034-7

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2016

Brain Structure and Function 4/2016 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.