Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Neurology 1/2020

Open Access 17.06.2020 | Original Communication

Body-maps of emotions in bilateral vestibulopathy

Erschienen in: Journal of Neurology | Sonderheft 1/2020

download
DOWNLOAD
print
DRUCKEN
insite
SUCHEN
Hinweise
Bigna Lenggenhager and Christophe Lopez share last authorship. These authors contributed equally to the present study.
Patients with a bilateral vestibulopathy (BVP) report a poorer quality of life, in both its physical and social dimensions, than healthy control participants [1]. BVP patients may report spatial anxiety with deficits in spatial cognition [2], and abnormal autonomous regulation [3]. Yet, they have lower anxiety related to vertigo [4] and less psychiatric comorbidities [5] than most patients with episodic vertigo. How BVP patients experience emotions—other than anxiety and depression—in an embodied way, is poorly documented, despite evidence of tight connections between vestibular, emotional, and embodiment processes [6]. Here, we investigated the embodiment of emotions in BVP patients. Emotions are strongly embodied: happiness, sadness and fear, for example, are associated with a variety of physiological changes [7] involving the interoceptive and somatosensory systems [8]. We used the computerized emBODY tool [9, 10] to document body-maps of emotions in BVP patients and healthy controls. Experiments with the emBODY tool in large samples of healthy participants revealed that different emotions are consistently associated with different body-maps [9, 10]. These body-maps of emotions are consistent across different cultures [9], developed between age 6 and 17 to become spatially specific [11], and they appear to be altered in clinical conditions [12].
We tested 21 BVP patients and 21 healthy controls matched for age, sex, education, and body-mass index (Table 1). Anxiety and depression [13] did not differ between groups. Standard otoneurological examinations established that patients presented a severe bilateral vestibular hypofunction without neurological disorder (Table 1). Controls reported no history of otoneurological disorder. Participants reported the bodily sensations associated with a neutral state, their current state, four basic emotions (fear, anger, disgust, happiness), and four non-basic emotions (anxiety, love, depression, pride). Each emotional word was presented once in a randomized order. Participants indicated where exactly in their body and how intensely they perceived an increase or decrease in activation when experiencing that emotion by coloring 2D human silhouettes on a computer screen (see Fig. 1a for details on procedures). Increase or decrease in activation was meant to reflect changes in interoceptive signals, such as higher/lower heart and breathing rates, palm sweating, skin temperature, or tonus in the legs and facial musculature. Thus, all sensed and reported changes in such inner bodily signals (no matter if the feeling is of activation or deactivation) would correspond to high interoception (i.e., awareness of inner bodily states).
Table 1
Demographic and clinical characteristics of BVL patients and healthy controls
 
Patients
Controls
Statistics
Males/females (n)
8/13
8/13
Age (years)
62 ± 13
62 ± 12
t = − 0.40, p = 0.69
Education (n)
 Level 1
3
1
χ2 = 8.4, p = 0.08
 Level 2
0
2
 
 Level 3
3
3
 
 Level 4
8
2
 
 Level 5
7
13
 
Body-mass index
24 ± 4
24 ± 3
t = − 0.06, p = 0.96
Anxiety score
8.5 ± 2.6
7.3 ± 3.2
t = − 0.52, p = 0.60
Depression score
4.3 ± 2.3
4.0 ± 2.7
t = 0.50, p = 0.62
Caloric test (mean slow phase eye velocity in °/s)
 Left ear at 44 °C
0.92 ± 1.91
 Right ear at 44 °C
1.76 ± 2.76
  
 Left ear at 30 °C
2.16 ± 2.92
  
 Right ear at 30 °C
2.14 ± 5.84
  
vHIT (gain)
 Horizontal canals
0.32 ± 0.28
 Anterior canals
 Posterior canals
0.22 ± 0.25
0.25 ± 0.27
  
cVEMPs (μV)
 Left ear
121 ± 23 (n = 6)
 Right ear
73 ± 51 (n = 9)
  
oVEMPs (μV)
 Left ear
2.0 ± 1.8 (n = 3)
 Right ear
2.7 ± 0.48 (n = 3)
  
Education level according to the French education system; Level 1: before high school; Level 2: accomplished high school; Level 3: 2 years after high school; Level 4: Bachelor’s degree, Level 5: Master’s degree, Engineering degree, PhD, MD. Levels of anxiety and depression the week before the experiment were reported using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale [13]. All patients had weak responses to a bithermal caloric test with water at 44 °C and 30 °C (mean slow phase eye velocity < 5°/s) and reduced responses to the video head impulse test (vHIT) with a mean gain < 0.7. The saccular and utricular functions were evaluated by cervical vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials (cVEMPs) over the sternocleidomastoid muscles and ocular vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials (oVEMPs) over the inferior oblique muscles. The VEMP amplitude is reported only for the patients with VEMPs (number in brackets). All data are reported as mean ± SD
First, individual maps were averaged to generate group body-maps representing the mean intensity of reported emotions for patients and controls [9, 10] (Fig. 1b). Visual inspection of group body-maps showed lower activation for positive emotions (e.g., happiness, pride, love), and lower deactivation for negative emotions (e.g., fear, anger, disgust, depression) in patients compared to controls. However, for none of the emotions, Mann–Whitney U tests revealed a significant difference between patients and controls after correcting for multiple comparisons.
Second, we analyzed the number of colored pixels corresponding to activation and deactivation for each emotional word per participant [12] (Fig. 1c). Friedman’s ANOVA revealed a significant effect of emotions on the number of colored pixels in patients (χ2 = 82.60, p < 0.001) as well as in controls (χ2 = 83.63, p < 0.001). Thus, as expected, the type of emotion significantly modulated the extent of the reported activation for both groups. Mann–Whitney U tests indicated that patients colored significantly less pixels for activation for happiness, when compared to controls (U = 135.0, Z = − 2.15, p = 0.03), whereas there was no difference for the other emotions and the current state (all U < 219.5, Z < − 1.82, p > 0.07). By contrast, Friedman’s ANOVA revealed no effect of emotions for deactivation maps in patients (χ2 = 15.82, p = 0.07), whereas the effect of emotions was significant in controls (χ2 = 29.39, p = 0.001). This indicates a lack of emotion-specific response for BVP patients. Mann–Whitney U tests indicated no difference in the number of pixels colored for deactivation between patients and controls for all emotions (all U < 220.5, Z < 451.5, p > 0.10).
Our exploratory study suggests that, overall, the topography of bodily sensations was not significantly different in BVP patients  and in controls. Yet, a limitation is that vestibular function was not tested in the controls. The task is mainly based on the recall of bodily sensations typically experienced during one specific emotion. The lack of clear topographical difference between BVP patients and controls suggests that this type of memory is not altered in severe long-term bilateral vestibular hypofunction. This is in contrast with deficits in spatial memory found in BVP (e.g., navigation in a virtual Morris water maze [14]). Functional MRI [15] revealed that basic emotions were best classified by activity patterns in structures along the brain midline (medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, precuneus), in the frontal pole and sensorimotor regions (post- and precentral gyri, insula). Midline structures, involved in self processing, are likely to integrate interoceptive signals with representations from the self and memory [15]. Thus, BVP may have a weaker impact on these midline structures than on the neural networks underpinning spatial memory [2, 14], sparing the topography of bodily sensations, while disrupting extrapersonal space memory.
When considering the number of colored pixels, we found a significantly reduced activation for happiness in the patients. This may reflect a form of emotional numbing, a symptom belonging to depersonalization–derealization, shown to be more frequent and more intense in patients with peripheral vestibular disorders [16]. Importantly, unlike healthy controls, BVP patients showed no emotion-specific modulation of the extent of the deactivation response. This difference cannot be accounted for by the participants’ age, body-mass index, anxiety, and depression scores. This suggests that BVP patients may be less aware of their interoceptive signals, i.e., the signals from the gastro-intestinal tract, blood vessels, kidneys, heart, etc., when presented with different emotional words. The emBODY tool revealed abnormal, less clear, and less differentiated body-maps of emotions in patients with schizophrenia [12], known to have lower interoceptive accuracy and higher disembodiment than healthy controls [17]. There is evidence that anosmia [18] and sensorimotor deafferentation in spinal cord injury [19] can decrease interoceptive accuracy. Similarly, BVP may alter interoceptive accuracy and attenuate the emotion-specific modulation of the spatial extent of bodily sensations, possibly due to the strong connections between the vestibular and interoceptive signals at both anatomical and functional levels [20].
In our sample, the general anxiety and depression scores (as measured with the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale [13]) did not differ between patients and controls. It has previously been argued that such findings might reflect a reduced anxiety, even though BVP patients encounter objectively more dangerous situations (e.g., they fall more often) [4, 21]. This is further confirmed by the fact that BVP patients show significantly lower anxiety scores and less psychiatric comorbidities in comparison to most patients with episodic vestibular disorders and chronic functional dizziness [4, 5, 21]. However, higher salivary cortisol levels have been found in BVP patients compared to healthy controls [3], indicating that physiological markers of anxiety might be increased, although not reflected in subjective reports. We tentatively suggest that the reduced sensitivity to emotionally induced changes in deactivations found in our study might corroborate such findings. However, more evidence will be needed to establish such effect, especially as our finding was specific to perceived deactivation.
In conclusion, although embodiment of emotions should be assessed in larger samples of participants with vestibular disorders, we believe that the present work helps understand the complex symptomatology of BVP patients, which needs to be better characterized, especially regarding own-body, self and emotional processing.

Acknowledgements

The research leading to these results has received funding from the Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant number: PP00P1_170511).
This manuscript is part of a supplement supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research within the funding initiative for integrated research and treatment centers.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Ethical approval

Procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee and followed the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 declaration of Helsinki.
All participants were informed about the study and gave a written informed consent.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Neuer Inhalt

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Literatur
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Craig AD (2002) How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:655–666CrossRef Craig AD (2002) How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:655–666CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Brandt T, Schautzer F, Hamilton DA et al (2005) Vestibular loss causes hippocampal atrophy and impaired spatial memory in humans. Brain 128:2732–2741CrossRef Brandt T, Schautzer F, Hamilton DA et al (2005) Vestibular loss causes hippocampal atrophy and impaired spatial memory in humans. Brain 128:2732–2741CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Jauregui-Renaud K, Ramos-Toledo V, Aguilar-Bolanos M et al (2008) Symptoms of detachment from the self or from the environment in patients with an acquired deficiency of the special senses. J Vestib Res 18:129–137PubMed Jauregui-Renaud K, Ramos-Toledo V, Aguilar-Bolanos M et al (2008) Symptoms of detachment from the self or from the environment in patients with an acquired deficiency of the special senses. J Vestib Res 18:129–137PubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Montoya P, Schandry R (1994) Emotional experience and heartbeat perception in patients with spinal cord injury and control subjects. J Psychophysiol 8:289–296 Montoya P, Schandry R (1994) Emotional experience and heartbeat perception in patients with spinal cord injury and control subjects. J Psychophysiol 8:289–296
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Balaban CD, Porter JD (1998) Neuroanatomic substrates for vestibulo-autonomic interactions. J Vestib Res 8:7–16PubMed Balaban CD, Porter JD (1998) Neuroanatomic substrates for vestibulo-autonomic interactions. J Vestib Res 8:7–16PubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Body-maps of emotions in bilateral vestibulopathy
Publikationsdatum
17.06.2020
Erschienen in
Journal of Neurology / Ausgabe Sonderheft 1/2020
Print ISSN: 0340-5354
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-1459
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-020-09888-z

Weitere Artikel der Sonderheft 1/2020

Journal of Neurology 1/2020 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.