Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Neurology 1/2020

Open Access 21.05.2020 | Original Communication

Cognitive functions in acute unilateral vestibular loss

verfasst von: Dilara Aktert Ayar, Emre Kumral, Nese Celebisoy

Erschienen in: Journal of Neurology | Sonderheft 1/2020

Abstract

Cognitive deficits mainly involving visuospatial functions have been defined in patients with bilateral and even unilateral vestibular loss (UVL). We compared the cognitive test results of 21 patients with acute UVL with age- and education-matched healthy controls. The diagnosis of UVL was based on the clinical findings, a normal magnetic resonance imaging with diffusion-weighted sequence and canal paresis on the affected side on caloric testing. Cognitive tests assessing visuospatial functions (Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation test, Verbal and non-verbal Cancellation tests, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure test) and global mental status, verbal memory, learning, retention of information, and recalling (Mini Mental State Examination, Oktem Verbal Memory Process Test, Forward and Backward Digit span) were used in addition to Beck depression and Anxiety inventories. Abnormalities in verbal and non-verbal cancellation tests (p < 0.005), Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation test (p = 0.042) and backward digit span (p = 0.029) was found. A very prominent difference regarding Beck depression (p = 0.012) and anxiety inventories (p < 0.001) was present. On multiple regression analysis, the abovementioned cognitive tests’ results lost their statistical significance (p > 0.05) when depression and anxiety scores were taken into consideration. The severity of canal paresis was found to be correlated with Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation test (p = 0.008, r = − 0.5639) and Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure test copying scores (p = 0.029, r = − 0.477). Comparison of all the results in right- and left-sided lesions did not reveal a significant difference (p > 0.05). Vestibular patients are prone to develop anxiety, and depression. Deficits in visuospatial functions, mental manipulation, psychomotor speed and short-term memory detected in our patients with acute UVL seem to be enhanced by accompanying anxiety and depression. The extent of vestibular dysfunction was correlated with the severity of deficits in visuospatial skills. Lesion side did not cause alterations in cognitive or emotional status.

Introduction

There are several studies dealing with cognitive functions in patients with vestibular loss. Spatial cognitive deficits were found to be affected in patients with bilateral vestibular loss (BVL) [15] which was attributed to disruption of the vestibulo-cortical projections mainly involved in spatial orientation and navigation [6]. Voxel-based morphometric magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies revealing hippocampal atrophy in patients with BVL strengthened the proposed pathomechanism [1, 3, 7].
Results of the tests in other cognitive domains are controversial. Brandt et al. [1] have reported normal results in tests assessing general memory and attention, whereas others have reported deficits in other domains of cognition [8, 9] including calculation [10], processing speed, short-term memory, and executive functions in patients with BVL and even in unilateral vestibular loss (UVL) [5].
Functional and structural reorganization of the widespread vestibular connections with projections beyond the parietal cortex and the hippocampus was proposed as the explanation of the non-spatial cognitive decline [5].
Few studies are present comparing BVL with UVL reporting more severe deficits in visuospatial functions in patients with BVL [2, 3, 5, 1113].
This prospective study was designed to evaluate different domains of cognition in patients with acute UVL in addition to assess anxiety and depression.

Methods

Twenty-one patients with acute UVL were assessed for the signs of vestibular deficits and cognitive dysfunction. The presence of horizontal-torsional spontaneous nystagmus with the fast phase beating to the contralateral ear and a deficient vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) in the head-impulse test were the clinical findings indicating a peripheral vestibular loss. Patients with clinical signs of central involvement (skew deviation, gaze-evoked nystagmus, associated neurological deficits and abnormal magnetic resonance imaging with diffusion-weighted sequence) were excluded. Canal paresis on the affected side using the Jongkees’s formula on caloric test and cervical VEMP testing with absent or delayed p13/n23 potential were additional features indicating peripheral vestibular failure.
Mini Mental State Examination, Oktem Verbal Memory Process Test, Forward and Backward Digit span, Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation test, verbal and non-verbal cancellation tests, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure test were performed to measure different domains of cognition in addition to Beck depression and Anxiety inventories. The same tests were given to 20 age- and sex-matched healthy controls and the results were compared. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Ege University Medical School and was performed in accordance with the ethical standards outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki (Project No: 17-5.2/16). Informed consent was gathered from all the participants.
Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) [14] was performed to measure global mental status.
Oktem Verbal Memory Process Test (OVMPT) [15] was used to evaluate the processes of verbal memory, learning or acquiring knowledge, retention of information, and recalling. The subjects were given 15 different words every other second and were asked to repeat the ones remembered at the end. The number of repeated words was recorded as the score of short memory. The same list was read to the subjects nine more times and the number of words remembered was noted to decide the ability of verbal learning. After a period of 30 min at the end of all other tests, the subjects were asked once again to repeat the given words to test retention and recalling. Number of words remembered for the three conditions were recorded as the test score.
Forward and Backward Digit span [16] was used to check short-term memory and mental manipulation.
In the forward test, the subject was asked to repeat increasing spans of digits in the order they were presented; and in backward test, increasing spans of digits in reverse order was expected to be repeated. In both test conditions, two trials were administered for each span length. Each condition was discontinued when the subject failed both trials for a given span. The score was the number of digits correctly repeated.
For spatial functions, verbal [17] and non-verbal [18] cancellation tests were used to measure selective spatial attention, psychomotor speed, visual searching and motor coordination.
For the cancellation test, the individuals were asked to find out and mark target items presented on a paper sheet. The test was composed by four test forms, consisting of random and structured arrays of verbal and non-verbal stimuli. Each sheet of paper contained 60 targets. Number of targets cancelled, omitted and errors, all through the four sessions were scored. The test time of the four sessions was also evaluated in seconds.
Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation test [19] was given to measure spatial thinking. Subjects were asked to match two angled lines to a set of 11 lines that are arranged in a semicircle and separated 18 degrees from each other. A 30-item test was given. The number of items correctly matched was the score.
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure test [20] was used for the assessment of visuo-constructional ability, visual memory, and executive functions. Subjects were asked to reproduce a complicated line drawing, first by copying it and then drawing from memory. Each copy was scored for the accurate reproduction and placement of 18 specific design elements.
Beck depression inventory [21] measures the severity of depression. The 21-question multiple-choice self-report inventory about how the subject had been feeling in the last week was given. Each question had a set of at least four possible responses, ranging in intensity from 0 (not at all) to 3 (severely). Higher total scores indicated more severe depressive symptoms.
Beck anxiety inventory [22] measures via self-report the presence and severity of current symptoms of anxiety and a generalized propensity to be anxious. The 21-question multiple-choice self-report inventory was given with the answers scored 0 (not at all) to 3 (severely). Higher total scores indicated more severe anxiety symptoms.
All the tests had validity and reliability studies performed on Turkish population [2325].
All test results were compared with the results of 20 healthy controls matched for age, sex and education years.

Statistical analysis

IBM SPSS Statistics 25.0 package program was used for analysis (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.). The normal distribution of the numerical variables was checked by the Shapiro Wilk test. Data not showing normal distribution were evaluated with non-parametric tests. Mann–Whitney U test was used to compare the results of the patient group with the healthy controls. Multiple logistic regression analysis was used to adjust the effect of Beck depression or anxiety inventory scores on cognitive tests.
Kruskal–Wallis test was used for the comparison of the cognitive test results of the patients regarding right- and left-sided involvement. For the tests found to be significantly different between three groups (all patients, patients with involvement of the right and the left side), Dunn test was performed with Bonferroni correction to make pairwise comparisons.
Spearman’s correlation analysis was used to find if the extent of canal paresis was related to cognitive function tests, depression and anxiety scores.
Categorical variables were compared with chi-square test. All the tests were performed at a 0.05 level of significance.

Results

Twelve of the 21 patients were male and 9 were female with a mean age of 54.3 years (range 35–68 years). Thirteen of the healthy controls were male and 7 were female with a mean age of 49.3 years (range 33–65 years). Mean education years was 12.3 years (range 5–16 years) for the patients and 13.7 years (range 5–16 years) for the healthy controls. No significant difference between the patients and healthy controls was present regarding age (p = 0.08) or education (p = 0.36). In 10 patients right and in 11 left-sided vestibular loss was present. Mean percentage of canal paresis was 61.5% (SD: 22.1%). In four patients, a p13/n23 potential could not be recorded from the affected side. In the remaining 17 delayed p13 (affected side: 14.2 ± 3.5 ms, non-affected side: 12.3 ± 1.0 ms) (p = 0.006) and n23 (affected side: 25.4 ± 5.1 ms, non-affected side: 23.4 ± 3.1 ms) (p = 0.001) potentials were recorded when compared with the non-affected side. Cognitive tests were performed within the first 2 weeks of the UVL (mean: 15.6 days, SD: 4.7). Mini Mental State Examination, Oktem Verbal Memory Process Test, Forward Digit span and Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure test results of the patients were not significantly different from the results of the healthy controls (p > 0.05) (Table 1). On the other hand, scores of backward digit span (p = 0.029) and Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation were low (p = 0.042). The number of targets cancelled on cancellation test was low (p = 0.017), omitted targets (p = 0.006) was high and the test time was long (p = 0.005) for the patient group (Table 1). A very prominent difference was present regarding Beck anxiety (p < 0.001) and depression inventories (p = 0.012) indicating that the patients had very high anxiety and depression scores (Table 1). Graphical presentation of the test results is given in Fig. 1.
Table 1
Test scores of the healthy controls and the patients
 
MMS
Oktem verbal memory process test
Forward digit span
Backward digit span
Cancellation test
Rey–Osterrieth complex figure test
BJLO
Beck anxiety
Beck depression
Short-term memory
Learning
Recall
Cancelled
Error
Omitted
Time (sec)
Copying
Short-term memory
Recall
Healthy controls
 Median
29.0
5.5
13.0
15.0
6.0
4.5
237.5
0
2.5
350
36
17
16
26
3
4
 Min
28.0
4.0
10.0
12.0
3.0
3.0
228.0
0
0
256
26
3
1
4
0
0
 Max
30.0
8.0
15.0
15.0
8.0
7.0
240
3
12
448
36
24
25
30
24
13
Patients
 Median
29.0
4.0
12.0
15.0
5.0
4.0
235.0
0
5
424
34
13
13
21
11
10
 Min
27.0
2.0
7.0
12.0
4.0
3.0
219.0
0
0
250
19
6
5
9
1
0
 Max
30.0
9.0
15.0
15.0
7.0
5.0
240
10
21
657
36
33
33
28
40
39
p
0.858
0.16
0.06
0.27
0.12
0.029
0.017
0.06
0.006
0.005
0.07
0.11
0.18
0.042
 < 0.001
0.012
Significant p values are given in bold
MMS mini mental state examination, BJLO Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation test
Though significant differences were noted in univariate analysis multiple regression analysis revealed that cancellation, Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation or backward digit span test scores were not significantly different from the results of the healthy controls (p > 0.05) when Beck depression and anxiety scores were taken into consideration (Table 2). On the other hand, depression and anxiety scores preserved their significance in all these analyses (p < 0.05).
Table 2
Results of the multiple logistic regression analysis
 
Logistic regression
Multiple logistic regression
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
Backward digit span
0.46*
0.233–0.899
0.51
0.251–1.044a
0.51
0.241–1.092b
Cancellation test-cancelled
0.81*
0.649–0.997
0.839
0.691–1.019a
0.79
0.612–1.008b
Cancellation test-omitted
1.3*
1.03–1.615
1.22
0.996–1.50a
1.28
1.00–1.649b
Cancellation test-time
1.01*
1.00–1.023
1.01
0.999–1.02a
1.01
0.999–1.02b
Benton’s judgment of line orientation
0.89
0.784–1.026
0.92
0.797–1.064a
0.89
0.776–1.031b
*p < 0.05
aBeck depression
bBeck anxiety
The extent of canal paresis was found to be correlated with Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation test (p = 0.008, r = − 0.5639) and Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test copying scores (p = 0.029, r = − 0.477).
The test results of the patients with right-sided vestibular loss (n: 10) were compared with the results of the left vestibular loss (n: 11). None of the test results including Beck depression and anxiety scores showed significant differences regarding right- or left-sided involvement (p > 0.05) though patients with left-sided lesions had lower scores on cancellation test and Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation test.

Discussion

After studies on patients with BVL revealing cognitive deficits mainly on visuospatial abilities, few others on patients with UVL began to appear with varied results.
Grabherr et al. [2] reported a decline in spatial imagery only in patients with bilateral vestibular loss. UVL patients had comparable results with healthy controls indicating disruption of spatial imagery in bilateral lesions [2]. Conrad et al. [26] reported that UVL did not cause spatial hemineglect, but was associated with mild attentional deficits in both visual hemifields.
Hüfner et al. [11] reported subtle deficits in spatial memory and navigation in patients with right vestibular loss, compatible with the dominance of the right labyrinth and the vestibular cortex in the right hemisphere. On the other hand, patients with left vestibular loss had no deficit and volumetric studies did not reveal hippocampal atrophy in unilateral deficits though bilateral deficits were associated with bilateral atrophy of the hippocampus [11].
Peruch et al. [12] have shown that all mental imagery tasks were impaired in patients with vestibular deafferentation and the performance in bilateral patients was worse than the unilateral cases. In a more recent study, UVL has been shown to impair embodied spatial cognition in patients with left vestibular neurectomy [13]. The authors explained this finding with the task used in their study involving a bilateral network of insular and parieto-frontal areas. A left vestibular insult disrupting bilateral vestibulo-thalamocortical projections from the left vestibular receptors was proposed as the pathomechanism [13].
Popp et al. [5] reported widespread cognitive impairment in patients with BVL, including short-term memory, executive function, and attention, in addition to affected visuospatial abilities. The same findings were present in patients with UVL to a lesser degree and the side of the lesion had no influence on cognitive performance [5]. They reported the cognitive scores to be correlated with the degree of vestibular dysfunction and the duration of the disease.
In our patient group, cancellation tests assessing visuospatial attention and psychomotor speed seemed to be affected in addition to Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation test given to measure spatial thinking. In addition to visuospatial functions, short-term memory and mental manipulation assessed by backward digit span was also impaired. The most important finding was the significantly high scores on Beck depression (p = 0.012) and anxiety inventories (p < 0.001). Multivariate analysis performed to determine the effect of anxiety and depression on the altered tests revealed that the abovementioned test results were not significantly different from the results of the healthy controls when anxiety and depression scores were taken into consideration (p > 0.05). However, the significance of depression and anxiety scores was consistent. The extent of vestibular dysfunction was found to be correlated with Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation test and Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test copying scores. Severe vestibular loss was associated with lower scores in tests assessing visuospatial skills.
Though the numbers were low comparison of the results of patients with right- and left-sided lesions did not show significant difference (p > 0.05).
It is very well known that vestibular patients are prone to develop anxiety, and depression [3, 27, 30]. Depression and anxiety accompanying vestibular disorders is explained by the shared pathways involving a vestibulo-parabrachial nucleus network, a cerebral cortical network (including the insula, orbitofrontal cortex, prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex), a raphe nuclear–vestibular network, a coeruleo–vestibular network and a raphe–locus coeruleus loop [31]. Emotional symptoms triggered, in case of a UVL, is due to the close interaction between the vestibular and the emotional systems at several lower as well as higher brain levels.
A hemispheric lateralization influencing these systems has also been proposed arguing that greater right hemisphere vestibulo-cortical dominance is associated with lower anxiety [32]. In a recent report, anxiety is reported to be low in patients with BVL [33]. In contrary, it is high in all episodic vertigo syndromes with an acute tone imbalance indicating that a functioning peripheral vestibular system is a prerequisite for the development of anxiety related to vertigo [34].
In our acute UVL patients, very prominent anxiety and depression was present and alterations in visuospatial functions, psychomotor speed and mental manipulation was closely associated with the emotional features. Accompanying anxiety and depression seem to enhance deficits of spatial attention, thinking as well as short-term memory storage. The close interaction between the vestibular, cognitive and affective systems makes it difficult to disentangle the effects of emotion on cognition in patients with UVL. Lesion lateralization was not a factor influencing cognitive functions or psychological symptoms in our group. On the other hand, the extent of canal paresis was associated with the severity of deficits in visuospatial skills.
The main limitation of the study is the low number of patients taken into consideration. This was especially the case for the comparison of right- (10 patients) and left-sided (11 patients) UVL. Our patients were tested in the acute stage when brain metabolism disorganization was maximal [35] and when compensatory mechanisms were not yet fully expressed [12]. It would be interesting to retest the same group during the chronic phase.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank to Associate Professor Timur Kose PhD for his contributions for the statistical analysis.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval

Approved by the Ethics Committee of Ege University Medical School.
All authors gave their consent to participate in preparation and submission of the manuscript.
All authors gave their consent for the publication of the manuscript.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Neuer Inhalt

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Brandt T, Schautzer F, Hamilton DA, Bruning R, Markowitsch HJ, Kalla R, Darlington C, Smith P, Strupp M (2005) Vestibular loss causes hippocampal atrophy and impaired spatial memory in humans. Brain 128:2732–2741CrossRef Brandt T, Schautzer F, Hamilton DA, Bruning R, Markowitsch HJ, Kalla R, Darlington C, Smith P, Strupp M (2005) Vestibular loss causes hippocampal atrophy and impaired spatial memory in humans. Brain 128:2732–2741CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Grabherr L, Cuffel C, Guyot JP, Mast FW (2011) Mental transformation abilities in patients with unilateral and bilateral vestibular loss. Exp Brain Res 209(2):205–214CrossRef Grabherr L, Cuffel C, Guyot JP, Mast FW (2011) Mental transformation abilities in patients with unilateral and bilateral vestibular loss. Exp Brain Res 209(2):205–214CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Kremmyda O, Huefner K, Flanagin VL, Hamilton DA, Linn J, Strupp M, Jahn K, Brandt T (2016) Beyond dizziness: virtual navigation, spatial anxiety and hippocampal volume in bilateral vestibulopathy. Front Hum Neurosci 10:139CrossRef Kremmyda O, Huefner K, Flanagin VL, Hamilton DA, Linn J, Strupp M, Jahn K, Brandt T (2016) Beyond dizziness: virtual navigation, spatial anxiety and hippocampal volume in bilateral vestibulopathy. Front Hum Neurosci 10:139CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Xie Y, Bigelow RT, Frankenthaler SF, Studenski SA, Moffat SD, Agrawal Y (2017) Vestibular loss in older adults is associated with impaired spatial navigation: data from the triangle completion task. Front Neurol 8:173CrossRef Xie Y, Bigelow RT, Frankenthaler SF, Studenski SA, Moffat SD, Agrawal Y (2017) Vestibular loss in older adults is associated with impaired spatial navigation: data from the triangle completion task. Front Neurol 8:173CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Popp P, Wulff M, Finke K, Ruhl M, Brandt T, Dieterich M (2017) Cognitive deficits in patients with a chronic vestibular failure. J Neurol 264:554–563CrossRef Popp P, Wulff M, Finke K, Ruhl M, Brandt T, Dieterich M (2017) Cognitive deficits in patients with a chronic vestibular failure. J Neurol 264:554–563CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith PF (2017) The vestibular system and cognition. Curr Opin Neurol 30:84–89CrossRef Smith PF (2017) The vestibular system and cognition. Curr Opin Neurol 30:84–89CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Gottlich M, Jandl NM, Sprenger A, Wojak JF, Munte TF, Kramer UM, Helmchen C (2016) Hippocampal gray matter volume in bilateral vestibular failure. Hum Brain Mapp 37:1998–2006CrossRef Gottlich M, Jandl NM, Sprenger A, Wojak JF, Munte TF, Kramer UM, Helmchen C (2016) Hippocampal gray matter volume in bilateral vestibular failure. Hum Brain Mapp 37:1998–2006CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Bessot N, Denise P, Toupet M, Van Nechel C, Chavoix C (2012) Interference between walking and a cognitive task is increased in patients with bilateral vestibular loss. Gait Posture 36:319–321CrossRef Bessot N, Denise P, Toupet M, Van Nechel C, Chavoix C (2012) Interference between walking and a cognitive task is increased in patients with bilateral vestibular loss. Gait Posture 36:319–321CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Pineault K, Pearson D, Wei E, Kamil R, Klatt B, Agrawal Y (2019) Association between saccule and semicircular canal impairments and cognitive performance among vestibular patients. Ear Hear [Online ahead of print] Pineault K, Pearson D, Wei E, Kamil R, Klatt B, Agrawal Y (2019) Association between saccule and semicircular canal impairments and cognitive performance among vestibular patients. Ear Hear [Online ahead of print]
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Risey J, Briner W (1990) Dyscalculia in patients with vertigo. J Vestib Res 1(1):31–37PubMed Risey J, Briner W (1990) Dyscalculia in patients with vertigo. J Vestib Res 1(1):31–37PubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Hüfner K, Hamilton DA, Kalla R, Stephan T, Glasauer S, Ma J, Brüning R, Markowitsch HJ, Labudda K, Schichor C, Strupp M, Brandt T (2007) Spatial memory and hippocampal volume in humans with unilateral vestibular deafferentation. Hippocampus 17(6):471–485CrossRef Hüfner K, Hamilton DA, Kalla R, Stephan T, Glasauer S, Ma J, Brüning R, Markowitsch HJ, Labudda K, Schichor C, Strupp M, Brandt T (2007) Spatial memory and hippocampal volume in humans with unilateral vestibular deafferentation. Hippocampus 17(6):471–485CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Péruch P, Lopez C, Redon-Zouiteni C, Escoffier G, Zeitoun A, Sanjuan M, Devèze A, Magnan J, Borel L (2011) Vestibular information is necessary for maintaining metric properties of representational space: evidence from mental imagery. Neuropsychologia 49(11):3136–3144CrossRef Péruch P, Lopez C, Redon-Zouiteni C, Escoffier G, Zeitoun A, Sanjuan M, Devèze A, Magnan J, Borel L (2011) Vestibular information is necessary for maintaining metric properties of representational space: evidence from mental imagery. Neuropsychologia 49(11):3136–3144CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Deroualle D, Borel L, Tanguy B, Bernard-Demanze L, Devèze A, Montava M, Lavieille JP, Lopez C (2019) Unilateral vestibular deafferentation impairs emboided spatial cognition. J Neurol 266(Suppl 1):149–159CrossRef Deroualle D, Borel L, Tanguy B, Bernard-Demanze L, Devèze A, Montava M, Lavieille JP, Lopez C (2019) Unilateral vestibular deafferentation impairs emboided spatial cognition. J Neurol 266(Suppl 1):149–159CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) Mini-mental status. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189–198CrossRef Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) Mini-mental status. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189–198CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Oktem O (1992) A verbal test of memory processes: a preliminary study. Arch Neuropsychatry 29:196–206 Oktem O (1992) A verbal test of memory processes: a preliminary study. Arch Neuropsychatry 29:196–206
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D (1997) WMS-III administration and scoring manual. The Psychological Corporation. Harcourt Brace & Co, San Antonio, TX Wechsler D (1997) WMS-III administration and scoring manual. The Psychological Corporation. Harcourt Brace & Co, San Antonio, TX
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Weintraub S, Mesulam MM (1988) Visual hemispatial inattention: Stimulus parameters and exploratory strategies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51:1481–1488CrossRef Weintraub S, Mesulam MM (1988) Visual hemispatial inattention: Stimulus parameters and exploratory strategies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51:1481–1488CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Mesulam MM (1985) Principles of behavioral neurology. FA Davis Company, Philadelphia Mesulam MM (1985) Principles of behavioral neurology. FA Davis Company, Philadelphia
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Benton AL, Varney N, Hamsher K (1978) Visuospatial judgment: a clinical test. Arch Neurol 35:364–367CrossRef Benton AL, Varney N, Hamsher K (1978) Visuospatial judgment: a clinical test. Arch Neurol 35:364–367CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Osterrieth PA (1944) The Complex Figure Test: Contribution to the study of perception and memory. Arch Psychol 28:1021–1034 Osterrieth PA (1944) The Complex Figure Test: Contribution to the study of perception and memory. Arch Psychol 28:1021–1034
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Beck AT (1978) The beck depression inventory. Center for Cognitive Therapy, Philadelphia Beck AT (1978) The beck depression inventory. Center for Cognitive Therapy, Philadelphia
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA (1988) An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol 56:893–897CrossRef Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA (1988) An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol 56:893–897CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Karakaş S, Eski R, Başar E (1996) Türk kültürü için standardizasyonu yapılmış bir nöro-psikolojik testler topluluğu: BILNOT bataryası. 32. Ulusal Nöroloji Kongresi Kitabı. Türk Nöroloji Dergisi ve Bakırköy Ruh ve Sinir Hastalıkları Hastanesi. İstanbul. Ufuk Mat Karakaş S, Eski R, Başar E (1996) Türk kültürü için standardizasyonu yapılmış bir nöro-psikolojik testler topluluğu: BILNOT bataryası. 32. Ulusal Nöroloji Kongresi Kitabı. Türk Nöroloji Dergisi ve Bakırköy Ruh ve Sinir Hastalıkları Hastanesi. İstanbul. Ufuk Mat
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Hisli N (1988) Beck Depresyon Envanteri’nin geçerliği üzerine bir çalışma. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi 6:118–126 Hisli N (1988) Beck Depresyon Envanteri’nin geçerliği üzerine bir çalışma. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi 6:118–126
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Ulusoy (1993) Beck Anksiyete Envanteri: Geçerlik ve güvenirlik çalışması. Yayınlanmamış uzmanlık tezi. Bakırköy Ruh ve Sinir Hastalıkları Hastanesi, İstanbul Ulusoy (1993) Beck Anksiyete Envanteri: Geçerlik ve güvenirlik çalışması. Yayınlanmamış uzmanlık tezi. Bakırköy Ruh ve Sinir Hastalıkları Hastanesi, İstanbul
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Conrad J, Habs M, Brandt T, Dieterich M (2015) Acute unilateral vestibular failure does not cause spatial hemineglect. PLoS One 6:10(8):135147 Conrad J, Habs M, Brandt T, Dieterich M (2015) Acute unilateral vestibular failure does not cause spatial hemineglect. PLoS One 6:10(8):135147
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Pollak L, Klein C, Rafael S, Vera K, Rabey JM (2003) Anxiety in the first attack of vertigo. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 128:829–834CrossRef Pollak L, Klein C, Rafael S, Vera K, Rabey JM (2003) Anxiety in the first attack of vertigo. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 128:829–834CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith PF, Zheng Y (2013) From ear to uncertainty: vestibular contributions to cognitive function. Front Integr Neurosci 7:84CrossRef Smith PF, Zheng Y (2013) From ear to uncertainty: vestibular contributions to cognitive function. Front Integr Neurosci 7:84CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Van de Berg R, van Tilburg M, Kingma H (2015) Bilateral vestibular hypofunction: challenges in establishing the diagnosis in adults. ORL 77:197–218CrossRef Van de Berg R, van Tilburg M, Kingma H (2015) Bilateral vestibular hypofunction: challenges in establishing the diagnosis in adults. ORL 77:197–218CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Lucieer F, Vonk P, Guinand N, Stokroos R, Kingma H, van de Berg R (2016) Bilateral vestibular hypofunction: insights in etiologies, clinical subtypes, and diagnostics. Front Neurol 7:26CrossRef Lucieer F, Vonk P, Guinand N, Stokroos R, Kingma H, van de Berg R (2016) Bilateral vestibular hypofunction: insights in etiologies, clinical subtypes, and diagnostics. Front Neurol 7:26CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Balaban CD, Jacob RG, Furman JM (2011) Neurologic bases for comorbidity of balance disorders, anxiety disorders and migraine: neurotherapeutic implications. Expert Rev Neurother 11:379–394CrossRef Balaban CD, Jacob RG, Furman JM (2011) Neurologic bases for comorbidity of balance disorders, anxiety disorders and migraine: neurotherapeutic implications. Expert Rev Neurother 11:379–394CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Bednarczuk NF, Casanovas Ortega M, Fluri AS, Arshad Q (2018) Vestibulo-cortical hemispheric dominance: the link between anxiety and the vestibular system? Eur J Neurosci 47(12):1517–1524CrossRef Bednarczuk NF, Casanovas Ortega M, Fluri AS, Arshad Q (2018) Vestibulo-cortical hemispheric dominance: the link between anxiety and the vestibular system? Eur J Neurosci 47(12):1517–1524CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Decker J, Limburg K, Henningsen P, Lahmann C, Brandt T, Dieterich M (2019) Intact vestibular function is relevant for anxiety related to vertigo. J Neurol 266(Suppl 1):89–92CrossRef Decker J, Limburg K, Henningsen P, Lahmann C, Brandt T, Dieterich M (2019) Intact vestibular function is relevant for anxiety related to vertigo. J Neurol 266(Suppl 1):89–92CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Brandt T, Dieterich M (2020) ‘Excess anxiety’ and ‘less anxiety’: both depend on vestibular function. Curr Opin Neurol 33(1):136–141CrossRef Brandt T, Dieterich M (2020) ‘Excess anxiety’ and ‘less anxiety’: both depend on vestibular function. Curr Opin Neurol 33(1):136–141CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Becker-Bense S, Dieterich M, Buchholz H-G et al (2014) The differential effects of acute right- vs. left-sided vestibular failure on brain metabolism. Brain Struct Funct 219:1355–1367CrossRef Becker-Bense S, Dieterich M, Buchholz H-G et al (2014) The differential effects of acute right- vs. left-sided vestibular failure on brain metabolism. Brain Struct Funct 219:1355–1367CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Cognitive functions in acute unilateral vestibular loss
verfasst von
Dilara Aktert Ayar
Emre Kumral
Nese Celebisoy
Publikationsdatum
21.05.2020
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Journal of Neurology / Ausgabe Sonderheft 1/2020
Print ISSN: 0340-5354
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-1459
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-020-09829-w

Weitere Artikel der Sonderheft 1/2020

Journal of Neurology 1/2020 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.