Introduction
Many researchers have previously examined the stability of schizophrenia symptoms over the time [
1‐
5]. One approach to investigate how these symptoms change is to estimate the trait versus state aspects of different symptoms [
6‐
12]. For instance, Chiappelli et al. [
6] reported that depressive symptoms in schizophrenia patients have state as well as trait aspects in patients with schizophrenia. Maat et al. [
7] found that emotional processing is also both state dependent (i.e., present during psychosis and significantly associated with symptom severity) and trait dependent (i.e., associated with the disorder across different stages). Although these studies provide evidence concerning the stability of different schizophrenia symptoms, their results are limited to one single symptom dimension in each study. Moreover, they focused on whether schizophrenia symptoms generally have a trait and/or a state component, but not how large these components are. However, both—including several symptom dimensions and estimating the size of trait versus state components—may offer valuable clues about the most effective treatment of patients with schizophrenia, e.g., which symptoms are rather variable and thus may potentially better influenceable by treatment? How large is the potential for change? On the other side, which symptoms are rather stable and are, therefore, less likely to change?
Two studies have tried to answer these questions using a latent state–trait (LST) model as introduced by Steyer et al. [
13‐
15]. However, both studies restricted their sample to clinical high-risk state and/or first-episode psychosis individuals (and not patients with schizophrenia). Hochstrasser et al. [
16] analyzed the longitudinal LST structure of the different dimensions of psychosis symptoms in clinical high-risk state and first-episode psychosis individuals over a 1-year timespan. Authors examined if the different symptom clusters (positive, negative, affectivity, resistance, activation, and excitement) according to the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) differ in their trait and state characters and found that negative symptoms had the highest trait component, although positive symptoms and affectivity also exhibited high trait components [
16]. The researchers concluded that an increasing proportion of psychosis symptoms persists over time [
16]. Rössler et al. [
17], on the other hand, applied the LST theory to sub-clinical psychosis symptoms over a 30-year time span. According to their findings, sub-clinical psychosis is predominantly influenced by stable traits at the age of 30, while before, at the age of 20, and after, at the age of 50, occasion-specific states were found to be more influential [
17]. In the present study, we extend previous research by investigating a sample of patients with a pre-existing diagnosis of schizophrenia.
It is important to note that symptom variability may have several underlying causes from the natural, individual course of the disorder to independent environmental influences, e.g., antipsychotic medication [
18,
19]. Nonetheless, treatment choices and outcomes can also differ on an individual level due to responsiveness to treatment and symptom combinations present as mentioned above [
20,
21]. As such, it is well known that positive symptoms often respond better to antipsychotic treatment than negative or cognitive symptoms do [
22,
23], while hostile or aggressive symptoms are also well addressed by antipsychotic treatment [
24]. Therefore, in the present study, we compare acute schizophrenia patients with antipsychotic treatment (receiving cariprazine) to those without antipsychotic treatment (receiving placebo) in terms of the LST structure of their different symptom domains.
Discussion
To the knowledge of the authors, this is the first analysis that examines the LST structure of the five-factor model [
32] of schizophrenia symptoms comparing patients receiving antipsychotic treatment to those without antipsychotic treatment. All symptom dimensions behaved more in a trait- rather than a state-like manner across the whole observation period of 6 weeks. Occasion-specific fluctuations appeared to have a negligible effect on how schizophrenia symptoms change over time. The proportions of all sources of variability changed over the course of the observational period, with a bent around weeks 3 and 4. Visually inspected, no major differences were found between the two treatment groups regarding the LST structure across all symptom dimensions. Among the small differences that were found, the group receiving placebo showed a slightly higher proportion of inter-individual stability. Patients receiving cariprazine, however, tended to have a larger proportion of error variance.
Previous studies investigating the stability of schizophrenia symptom dimensions have addressed trait versus state aspects of single symptoms, however, without quantifying the proportion of these aspects [
6‐
12]. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to quantify these proportions using an LST model as introduced by Steyer et al. [
13,
14].
Previously, two studies applied the LST model in the context of psychotic symptoms in clinical high-risk state and first-episode psychosis samples [
16,
17]. Hochstrasser et al. showed that the proportion of variability explained by stable traits differed between symptom dimensions (importantly, “trait” referring to inter-individual (and not absolute) stability). While positive and negative symptoms exhibited high trait proportions (72.2% resp. 81.1%), only 13.2% of the variance of excitement component was explained by trait differences [
16]. Since, in the present study, all symptom dimensions showed similar proportions of trait variability, our results contradict the abovementioned findings. Nonetheless, it is important to note that the observation period was different in the Hochstrasser and in the present study (1 year versus 6 weeks) which could provide an explanation to the difference. Indeed, symptoms like excitation can fluctuate in the long run, while being stable during acute episodes which last a few weeks to months [
36,
37]. Our findings are, therefore, not generalizable to the long-term symptom course. Negative symptoms, on the other hand, might become more prominent after acute psychosis as reported by several other research groups [
4,
38,
39]. Yet, Hochstrasser and colleagues have not analyzed the proportion of explained variance by latent states, measurement errors or method effects. Systematic changes in symptom scores may have, therefore, mistakenly attributed to trait differences which hinders in-depth comparisons between the two studies in this regard.
In contrast to Hochstrasser and colleagues, Rössler et al. [
17] also reported the change of state aspects. Rössler and colleagues investigated the sub-clinical psychotic symptoms in a clinical high-risk state and first-episode psychosis population. They found that these symptoms are predominantly influenced by stable traits at the age of 30, while before and after occasion-specific states were influential [
17]. Contrary to these findings, we found that situational fluctuations and/or person–situation interactions between study visits have a relatively negligible effect on how schizophrenic symptoms change over time. Averaged across the observation period, no proportion of state variability exceeded 10% in our study, while Rössler et al. [
17] reported proportions up to 61.2%. This may be due to similar reasons as mentioned above; given the short observation period of our study, some symptomatic fluctuations may only become visible in the long run. Another reason for such difference may be explained by the different samples. While Rössler et al. investigated a highly diverse sample including healthy controls, clinical high-risk state individuals as well as patients with first-episode psychosis, this study included a more homogenous sample of acute schizophrenia patients. Therefore, the heterogeneous population might have led to a high diversity in terms of symptomatic change and the more homogenous and highly standardized treatment setting resulted in less fluctuation between visits. Indeed, previous research also showed that up to 90% of the sub-clinical psychotic symptoms are transitory and disappear over time [
40].
It is important to note, however, that there are also similarities between the three LST studies. For instance, a large proportion of variance was explained by latent traits. Thus, the symptomatic change appears to be inter-individually rather similar than different comparing patients to each other. This overlap between the studies is surprising, especially regarding the major differences concerning sample structure, treatment settings, and duration of the observation period. This underlying trait aspect may be interpreted as a “common ground” of the assessed symptom dimensions, a structure that is shared by all symptom dimensions. As Hochstrasser et al. [
16] describes it: the disorder always is “part of a ‘whole’ of mutually implicative, interpenetrating experiences, feelings, beliefs, expressions, and actions, influenced by the biographical background of the individual.” (p. 6). Previous studies also support this idea by reporting a strong inter-connectedness between schizophrenia symptoms [
38,
41‐
43]. Moreover, although there is a high heterogeneity between schizophrenia patients, the long-term course of the symptoms appears to be rather stable at an individual level (i.e., symptoms having a high inter-individual stability). Cannon [
44] introduced the idea of schizophrenia being a trait, referring to a long-term vulnerability to psychosis which, in turn, can be seen as a state. A trait aspect that is shared by all symptom dimensions may, therefore, represent the underlying structure of chronic schizophrenia that may become visible in other psychological and (neuro)biological markers [
16]. However, to test this hypothesis, more research is necessary, especially including patients with other psychotic disorders (like schizoaffective disorder or delusional disorder).
This argumentation is somewhat supported by another finding of our study: visually inspected, there were no major differences between the two treatment groups (cariprazine versus placebo) regarding their LST structure. This finding might seem counterintuitive. However, the similarity in their LST structure might display something inherent to the disorder as discussed above. However, there are also other possible explanations for this observation. For example, “treatment” cannot be narrowed down to psychopharmacological medication alone. All patients have been treated in an in-patient psychiatric setting with all its numerous therapeutic effects. This may have led to a fairly similar improvement of symptom scores and similar proportions of traits and states, independently whether patients received antipsychotic treatment or not. It is, therefore, difficult to come to a conclusion regarding this finding.
Further, we found some small differences between the two treatment groups. The group receiving placebo showed a slightly higher proportion of inter-individual stability. Patients receiving the antipsychotic, however, tended to have a larger proportion of error variance. Both findings may be due to the fact that patients differ in their response to their received treatment; patients receiving the placebo tended to exhibit a weaker symptom reduction (their symptoms remain rather inter-individually stable across time), while patients receiving the antipsychotic respond stronger but possibly inter-individually more different to the medication (leading to a higher proportion of unexplained variance). However, the group differences were small, and it remains unclear whether these differences would reach clinical significance.
Finally, the trait components in our study exhibited the lowest values at the beginning, then rising to the maximum value before decreasing again. Changes of the state components behaved in an opposite pattern (U-shape). This bent at weeks 3–4 could be ascribed to the study setting as patients who all have been treated in an in-patient setting at first were allowed to leave the hospital after 3 or 4 weeks, leading to higher differing treatment settings and a, therefore, stronger varying symptom development. However, this remains as the only one potential explanation for this finding, which should be further explored in future research.
One of the main limitations of the analysis is that the data are from four phase II/III clinical trials which all had a rigorous inclusion criterion such as having a score between 80 and 120 on the PANSS, including a score of at least 4 on more than 2 of the PANSS positive symptom items. Therefore, the total variance in PANSS scores is somewhat restricted compared to what could be seen in a real-world clinical practice. Second, it cannot be ruled out that certain psychiatric comorbidities (like personality or affective disorders) or the effects of concomitant medications (like drugs alleviating agitation, or hostility) may have systematically influenced the changes in symptom scores. In addition, the timeframe of the studies was short, which again limits the unfolding of whether a symptom dimension is rather a trait or a state. This makes any conclusion about long-term variability of symptoms impossible, as state aspects of some symptoms might have only become apparent after the 6 weeks long period. Future research should, therefore, include a longer observation period. Finally, those who have dropped out from the trials were excluded from the analysis, which might bias the results in the direction of underestimating the trait components.
In summary, the present analysis sheds light on the notion that symptoms of schizophrenia appear to be rather inter-individually stable than state-like in patients suffering from an acute phase of schizophrenia. This observation was found in both—patients who received antipsychotic medication and patients who did not.