Skip to main content
Erschienen in: International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction 1/2018

Open Access 17.10.2017 | Commentary

Conceptual Issues Concerning Internet Addiction and Internet Gaming Disorder: Further Critique on Ryding and Kaye (2017)

verfasst von: Mark D. Griffiths

Erschienen in: International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction | Ausgabe 1/2018

Abstract

The recent commentary paper by Ryding and Kaye Journal of Mental Health and Addiction (doi 10.1007/s11469-017-9811-6, 2017) rightly claimed that “internet addiction” (IA) is a conceptual minefield and raised some important issues for researchers and treatment providers working in the online addiction field. In the present commentary paper, some of the assertions made by Ryding and Kaye are briefly critiqued and extended. More specifically, the present paper (i) examines IA and Internet-based addictions and argues that IA is now a misnomer, (ii) examines IA and its relationship to Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) and argues IA and IGD are two completely different constructs, and that IGD is a sub-type of gaming disorder rather than a sub-type of IA, (iii) argues that the time spent engaging in online activities is not a good criterion for assessing online addictions and that the context of use is far more important criterion, and (iv) argues that those researchers working in the IA field can learn a lot from the problem gambling field in collecting robust data. More specifically, one of the innovative ways forward could be to build strategic partnerships with commercial online companies to analyze their behavioral tracking data.

Internet Addiction vs. Internet-Based Addictions: Some Further Thoughts

The recent commentary paper by Ryding and Kaye (2017) on the concept of Internet addiction (IA) raised a number of theoretical issues that anyone working in the field would recognize as important, and I wholeheartedly agree that the area is (and always has been) a “conceptual minefield.” In this commentary, I briefly critique and extend some of the assertions made by Ryding and Kaye.

Internet Addiction Is Now a Misnomer

Ryding and Kaye began their paper by noting that IA has only “recently been identified as being addictive” (p.1) but then cite references from over 20 years ago on the topic including one of my own (i.e., Griffiths 1996a). In 1995 and 1996, I published a number of papers and articles on “technological addictions” and IA (Griffiths 1995a, b, 1996a, b), all arguing that addictions to and on the Internet were likely to be major areas of concern in the addictive behavior field. In these papers and my other early writings in the field (Griffiths 1998, 1999, 2000), I constantly made the point that there was a difference between what we now term “generalized internet addiction” (i.e., where an individual spends almost all of their waking time online engaged in more than one online activity to the neglect of all other important activities in their life) and “specific internet addictions” (i.e., where an individual spends almost all of their waking time engaged in just online activity, such as gambling or gaming, to the neglect of all other important activities in their life).
Since my writings more than two decades ago, I have come to the view that “internet addiction” and (similarly) “smartphone addiction” (also highlighted by Ryder and Kaye) are misnomers. Internet addicts and smartphone addicts are no more addicted to the Internet or their smartphones than alcoholics are addicted to bottles (Griffiths and Kuss 2017; Griffiths and Pontes 2014; Kim and Kim 2010). Individuals addicted to online gaming, online gambling, online sex, or online shopping are not Internet addicts. They are gambling addicts, gaming addicts, sex addicts, or shopping addicts that are using the medium of the Internet to engage in their addictive behavior. There are of course some activities—such as social networking—that could be argued to be a genuine and “pure” type of “internet addiction” because the activity of social networking can only take place online and there is no offline equivalent. However, the addiction is to an application rather than the Internet itself, and this should be termed “social networking addiction” rather than a type or sub-type of IA.

Internet Addiction and Internet Gaming Disorder Are Two Completely Different Constructs

Ryding and Kaye assert that much “of the recent literature in the realm of IA has focused upon Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD)” (p.3). However, IA and IGD are two completely different constructs as have been shown empirically in a largescale nationally representative study (i.e., Király et al. 2014) and argued theoretically (Griffiths and Pontes 2014). The conflation of these two independent constructs has not been helped by the accompanying text for IGD in the latest (fifth) edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association 2013). More specifically, the DSM-5 asserts that:
There are no well-researched subtypes for Internet gaming disorder to date. Internet gaming disorder most often involves specific Internet games, but it could involve non-Internet computerized games as well, although these have been less researched. It is likely that preferred games will vary over time as new games are developed and popularized, and it is unclear if behaviors and consequence associated with Internet gaming disorder vary by game type…Internet gaming disorder has significant public health importance, and additional research may eventually lead to evidence that Internet gaming disorder (also commonly referred to as Internet use disorder, Internet addiction, or gaming addiction) has merit as an independent disorder (p.796).
Not only do these statements conflate IGD with IA but also claim that IGD can (confusingly) include offline gaming disorder, a situation that my colleagues and I recently described as leading to “chaos and confusion” in the online addiction field (Kuss et al. 2017; p.103). Personally, I would argue that IGD is a sub-type of gaming disorder rather than a sub-type of IA. Ryding and Kaye appear to implicitly agree with this because their paper also asserts:
One means by which researchers could move forward in this regard is to establish the validity of IGD to a wider range of gaming formats…Arguably, there are a range of forms of “online” gaming, including social networking site (SNS) games which are Internet-mediated and thus by definition, would appear under the remit of IGD…given these are often played on mobile devices rather than on more traditional PC or console platforms (p.3).
Even before the advent of online gaming in the early 2000s, I argued that there were many different platforms on which individuals could play videogames including gaming machines in amusement arcades, dedicated home consoles (e.g., Sony PlayStation), personal computers, and handheld devices (e.g., Nintendo GameBoy) (Griffiths 1991, 1993). None of these gaming platforms involved being online, yet gaming on all of these platforms can be potentially addictive for a minority that play videogames on them (Griffiths et al. 2012). Added to this, we now have online gaming via dedicated home consoles, personal computers, laptops, smartphones, tablets, and MP3 players. Given that there as many ways to play videogames offline as online, it makes greater conceptual sense to view IGD as a sub-type of gaming disorder rather than as a sub-type of IA (particularly—as argued above—because online gaming addicts are not addicted to the Internet, they are addicted to the gaming). The World Health Organization’s proposed diagnosis of Gaming Disorder in the beta draft of the (11th edition) International Classification of Diseases (with sub-varieties of online and offline gaming disorders) appears to be much more logical and well thought out than the description of Internet Gaming Disorder found in the DSM-5 (Saunders et al. 2017).

The Context of Online Screen Time Is Far More Important than the Time Spent Online

Another key issue raised by Ryding and Kaye is the time spent gaming (often viewed as a key criterion of addictive gaming when played to excess) and the issue of online “screen time” more generally. While there is clearly a correlation between time spent playing and individuals that have IGD (Griffiths et al. 2016), the reverse is not necessarily the case. This is because the context is far more important than time when it comes to defining key characteristics of IGD and because there are many individuals who are excessive gamers but very few who would endorse five or more of the DSM-5’s criteria for IGD diagnosis (Griffiths 2010). Given that many of us spend hours a day online for work purposes (myself included) clearly demonstrates that how the medium of the Internet is used is much more important than the time spent in the medium when defining IA. Even when the excessive use of the Internet is work-based and causing long-term negatively detrimental effects on the individual, such individuals would be defined and conceptualized as work addicts rather than Internet addicts (Quinones and Griffiths 2017; Quinones et al. 2016).

Researchers in the Online Addiction Field Can Learn a Lot from Those in the Problem Gambling Field

One of the major weaknesses in the IA field is that the majority of the data is self-report and based on non-representative epidemiological self-selected samples. However, there is an increasing amount of studies using neuroimaging techniques (see Pontes et al. (2017) for a recent review), a small number of studies using clinical samples of those undergoing treatment for IGD or IA (e.g., Beranuy et al. 2013). Many of the methodological weaknesses in the IA field are similar to those found in the gambling study fields a few decades ago. However, the growth of research into problem gambling has led to the collection of high-quality data from many different sources, and the IA field could learn a lot from the types of research and methodologies used by researchers in the gambling study field.
One of the major innovations in gambling is the access to (and the publishing of studies using) online behavioral tracking data. For example, over the past few years, Dr. Michael Auer and I have been given access to online gambling datasets from companies such as win2day, Norsk Tipping, and Kindred Gaming. We have used these data to develop new measures of “gambling intensity” based on actual gambling data (Auer and Griffiths 2014a, 2015a), evaluate the efficacy of responsible gambling tools such as limit setting (Auer and Griffiths 2013), pop-up messaging (Auer et al. 2014; Auer and Griffiths 2015b), and personalized feedback (Auer and Griffiths 2014b, 2015c, 2016), as well as directly comparing what gamblers say they have done online with what they have actually done online (Auer and Griffiths 2017a), and test classic psychological theories such as cognitive dissonance using subjective and objective measures from the same gamblers (Auer and Griffiths 2017b). Other researchers have used such data to identify behavioral markers of high-risk online gambling (Braverman and Shaffer 2012; Braverman et al. 2013; Gray et al. 2012) and to compare online gamblers who self-exclude with those that do not (Dragicevic et al. 2015).
In relation to IA and IGD, the use of behavioral tracking data may lead to innovative ways forward in the online addiction field, as well as new theoretical insights. This is because every click and transaction of every customer on every online site is automatically stored in a database and provides very high-data quality for those who know what to do with it. This is very much in contrast to traditional surveys where such data have to be gathered at high costs, with lots of manual work involved, and may be prone to error. Online video-gaming sites and social networking sites typically have (i) personal details provided by a user (e.g., name, email address, telephone number, address, gender,) and (ii) usage data collected automatically as the user clicks from one webpage to another (e.g., how users access the site, type of web browser used, time spent logged onto the site, what activities are engaged in, who they are engaging with,). If academics can form strategic collaborations with such commercial companies, they may be able to start identifying behavioral markers of excessive online users, gamers, and/or social networkers (that will include a small minority of problematic users). However, other types of research are also needed including longitudinal studies so that people can be tracked over time (either via behavioral tracking and/or surveys), and more detailed qualitative work (interviews and focus groups) with different player or user types, and those who seek treatment for their problematic use of online activities.

Concluding Comments

The present paper summarized and extended some of the issues raised by Ryding and Kaye. In conclusion, it is argued that (i) Internet addiction (IA) is now a misnomer, (ii) IA and Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) are two completely different constructs and that IGD is a sub-type of gaming disorder rather than a sub-type of IA, (iii) the time spent engaging in online activities is not a good criterion for assessing online addictions and that the context of use is a far more important criterion, and (iv) researchers working in the online addiction field can learn a lot from the problem gambling field in collecting robust data, and that one of the innovative ways forward could be to build strategic partnerships with commercial online companies to analyze their behavioral tracking data.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The author has received funding for a number of research projects in the area of gambling education for young people, social responsibility in gambling and gambling treatment from GambleAware (formerly the Responsibility in Gambling Trust), a charitable body which funds its research program based on donations from the gambling industry. The author undertakes consultancy for various gaming companies in the area of social responsibility in gambling. In the next three years, the author will be receiving research funding from Norsk Tipping, the government-owned gaming company to evaluate responsible gambling features on the Norsk Tipping website.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of University’s Research Ethics Board and with the 1975 Helsinki Declaration.
N/A

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0), which permits use, duplication, adaptation, distribution, and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Psychiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der psychiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRef American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). Voluntary limit setting and player choice in most intense online gamblers: an empirical study of gambling behaviour. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29, 647–660.CrossRefPubMed Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). Voluntary limit setting and player choice in most intense online gamblers: an empirical study of gambling behaviour. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29, 647–660.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2014a). An empirical investigation of theoretical loss and gambling intensity. Journal of Gambling Studies, 30, 879–887.CrossRefPubMed Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2014a). An empirical investigation of theoretical loss and gambling intensity. Journal of Gambling Studies, 30, 879–887.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2014b). Personalised feedback in the promotion of responsible gambling: a brief overview. Responsible Gambling Review, 1, 27–36. Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2014b). Personalised feedback in the promotion of responsible gambling: a brief overview. Responsible Gambling Review, 1, 27–36.
Zurück zum Zitat Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2015a). Theoretical loss and gambling intensity (revisited): a response to Braverman et al (2013). Journal of Gambling Studies, 31, 921–931.CrossRefPubMed Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2015a). Theoretical loss and gambling intensity (revisited): a response to Braverman et al (2013). Journal of Gambling Studies, 31, 921–931.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017a). Self-reported losses versus actual losses in online gambling: an empirical study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33, 795–806.CrossRefPubMed Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017a). Self-reported losses versus actual losses in online gambling: an empirical study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33, 795–806.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017b). Cognitive dissonance, personalized feedback, and online gambling behavior: an exploratory study using objective tracking data and subjective self-report. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-017-9808-1. Auer, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017b). Cognitive dissonance, personalized feedback, and online gambling behavior: an exploratory study using objective tracking data and subjective self-report. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s11469-017-9808-1.
Zurück zum Zitat Auer, M., Malischnig, D., & Griffiths, M. D. (2014). Is ‘pop-up’ messaging in online slot machine gambling effective? An empirical research note. Journal of Gambling Issues, 29, 1–10.CrossRef Auer, M., Malischnig, D., & Griffiths, M. D. (2014). Is ‘pop-up’ messaging in online slot machine gambling effective? An empirical research note. Journal of Gambling Issues, 29, 1–10.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Beranuy, M., Carbonell, X., & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). A qualitative analysis of online gaming addicts in treatment. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 11, 149–161.CrossRef Beranuy, M., Carbonell, X., & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). A qualitative analysis of online gaming addicts in treatment. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 11, 149–161.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Braverman, J., LaPlante, D. A., Nelson, S. E., & Shaffer, H. J. (2013). Using cross-game behavioral markers for early identification of high-risk Internet gamblers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27, 868–877.CrossRefPubMed Braverman, J., LaPlante, D. A., Nelson, S. E., & Shaffer, H. J. (2013). Using cross-game behavioral markers for early identification of high-risk Internet gamblers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27, 868–877.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Braverman, J., & Shaffer, H. J. (2012). How do gamblers start gambling: identifying behavioral markers for high-risk Internet gambling. European Journal of Public Health., 22, 273–278.CrossRefPubMed Braverman, J., & Shaffer, H. J. (2012). How do gamblers start gambling: identifying behavioral markers for high-risk Internet gambling. European Journal of Public Health., 22, 273–278.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Dragicevic, S., Percy, C., Kudic, A., & Parke, J. (2015). A descriptive analysis of demographic and behavioral data from internet gamblers and those who self-exclude from online gambling platforms. Journal of Gambling Studies, 31, 105–132.CrossRefPubMed Dragicevic, S., Percy, C., Kudic, A., & Parke, J. (2015). A descriptive analysis of demographic and behavioral data from internet gamblers and those who self-exclude from online gambling platforms. Journal of Gambling Studies, 31, 105–132.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gray, H. M., LaPlante, D. A., & Shaffer, H. J. (2012). Behavioral characteristics of Internet gamblers who trigger corporate responsible gambling interventions. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 26, 527–535.CrossRefPubMed Gray, H. M., LaPlante, D. A., & Shaffer, H. J. (2012). Behavioral characteristics of Internet gamblers who trigger corporate responsible gambling interventions. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 26, 527–535.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D. (1991). Amusement machine playing in childhood and adolescence: A comparative analysis of video games and fruit machines. Journal of Adolescence, 14, 53–73.CrossRefPubMed Griffiths, M. D. (1991). Amusement machine playing in childhood and adolescence: A comparative analysis of video games and fruit machines. Journal of Adolescence, 14, 53–73.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D. (1993). Are computer games bad for children? The Psychologist: Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 6, 401–407. Griffiths, M. D. (1993). Are computer games bad for children? The Psychologist: Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 6, 401–407.
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D. (1995a). Technological addictions. Clinical Psychology Forum, 76, 14–19. Griffiths, M. D. (1995a). Technological addictions. Clinical Psychology Forum, 76, 14–19.
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D. (1995b). Netties anonymous: are some people addicted to the internet? Times Higher Education Supplement, April 17, p. 17. Griffiths, M. D. (1995b). Netties anonymous: are some people addicted to the internet? Times Higher Education Supplement, April 17, p. 17.
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D. (1996a). Gambling on the internet: a brief note. Journal of Gambling Studies, 12, 471–474.CrossRefPubMed Griffiths, M. D. (1996a). Gambling on the internet: a brief note. Journal of Gambling Studies, 12, 471–474.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D. (1996b). Internet addiction: an issue for clinical psychology? Clinical Psychology Forum, 97, 32–36. Griffiths, M. D. (1996b). Internet addiction: an issue for clinical psychology? Clinical Psychology Forum, 97, 32–36.
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D. (1998). Internet addiction: does it really exist? In J. Gackenbach (Ed.), Psychology and the Internet: intrapersonal, interpersonal and transpersonal applications (pp. 61–75). New York: Academic Press. Griffiths, M. D. (1998). Internet addiction: does it really exist? In J. Gackenbach (Ed.), Psychology and the Internet: intrapersonal, interpersonal and transpersonal applications (pp. 61–75). New York: Academic Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D. (1999). Internet addiction: Internet fuels other addictions. Student British Medical Journal, 7, 428–429. Griffiths, M. D. (1999). Internet addiction: Internet fuels other addictions. Student British Medical Journal, 7, 428–429.
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D. (2000). Internet addiction—time to be taken seriously? Addiction Research, 8, 413–418.CrossRef Griffiths, M. D. (2000). Internet addiction—time to be taken seriously? Addiction Research, 8, 413–418.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D. (2010). The role of context in online gaming excess and addiction: some case study evidence. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 8, 119–125.CrossRef Griffiths, M. D. (2010). The role of context in online gaming excess and addiction: some case study evidence. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 8, 119–125.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D., & Kuss, D. J. (2017). Adolescent social media addiction (revisited). Education and Health, 35, 59–62. Griffiths, M. D., & Kuss, D. J. (2017). Adolescent social media addiction (revisited). Education and Health, 35, 59–62.
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., & King, D. L. (2012). Video game addiction: past, present and future. Current Psychiatry Reviews, 8, 308–318.CrossRef Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., & King, D. L. (2012). Video game addiction: past, present and future. Current Psychiatry Reviews, 8, 308–318.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., & Pontes, H. (2016). A brief overview of Internet Gaming Disorder and its treatment. Australian Clinical Psychologist, 2(1), 20108. Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., & Pontes, H. (2016). A brief overview of Internet Gaming Disorder and its treatment. Australian Clinical Psychologist, 2(1), 20108.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim, M. G., & Kim, J. (2010). Cross-validation of reliability, convergent and discriminant validity for the problematic online game use scale. Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 389–398.CrossRef Kim, M. G., & Kim, J. (2010). Cross-validation of reliability, convergent and discriminant validity for the problematic online game use scale. Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 389–398.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kuss, D. J., Griffiths, M. D., & Pontes, H. M. (2017). Chaos and confusion in DSM-5 diagnosis of Internet Gaming Disorder: issues, concerns, and recommendations for clarity in the field. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6, 103–109.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kuss, D. J., Griffiths, M. D., & Pontes, H. M. (2017). Chaos and confusion in DSM-5 diagnosis of Internet Gaming Disorder: issues, concerns, and recommendations for clarity in the field. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6, 103–109.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Király, O., Griffiths, M. D., Urbán, R., Farkas, J., Kökönyei, G., Elekes, Z., Domokos, T., & Demetrovics, Z. (2014). Problematic internet use and problematic online gaming are not the same: findings from a large nationally representative adolescent sample. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 17, 749–754.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Király, O., Griffiths, M. D., Urbán, R., Farkas, J., Kökönyei, G., Elekes, Z., Domokos, T., & Demetrovics, Z. (2014). Problematic internet use and problematic online gaming are not the same: findings from a large nationally representative adolescent sample. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 17, 749–754.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Pontes, H. M., Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Psychometric assessment of Internet Gaming Disorder in neuroimaging studies: a systematic review. In C. Montag & M. Reuter (Eds.), Internet addiction neuroscientific approaches and therapeutical implications (pp. 181–208). New York: Springer. Pontes, H. M., Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Psychometric assessment of Internet Gaming Disorder in neuroimaging studies: a systematic review. In C. Montag & M. Reuter (Eds.), Internet addiction neuroscientific approaches and therapeutical implications (pp. 181–208). New York: Springer.
Zurück zum Zitat Quinones, C., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). The impact of daily emotional demands, job resources and emotional effort on intensive internet use during and after work. Computers in Human Behavior, 76, 561–575.CrossRef Quinones, C., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). The impact of daily emotional demands, job resources and emotional effort on intensive internet use during and after work. Computers in Human Behavior, 76, 561–575.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Quinones, C., Griffiths, M. D., & Kakabadse, N. (2016). Compulsive Internet use and workaholism: an exploratory two-wave longitudinal study. Computers in Human Behavior, 60, 492–499.CrossRef Quinones, C., Griffiths, M. D., & Kakabadse, N. (2016). Compulsive Internet use and workaholism: an exploratory two-wave longitudinal study. Computers in Human Behavior, 60, 492–499.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Saunders, J. B., Hao, W., Long, J., King, D. L., Mann, K., et al. (2017). Gaming disorder: its delineation as an important condition for diagnosis, management and prevention. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6, 271–279.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Saunders, J. B., Hao, W., Long, J., King, D. L., Mann, K., et al. (2017). Gaming disorder: its delineation as an important condition for diagnosis, management and prevention. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6, 271–279.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Conceptual Issues Concerning Internet Addiction and Internet Gaming Disorder: Further Critique on Ryding and Kaye (2017)
verfasst von
Mark D. Griffiths
Publikationsdatum
17.10.2017
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction / Ausgabe 1/2018
Print ISSN: 1557-1874
Elektronische ISSN: 1557-1882
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-017-9818-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.