Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Italian Journal of Pediatrics 1/2017

Open Access 01.12.2017 | Review

Conduct disorders and psychopathy in children and adolescents: aetiology, clinical presentation and treatment strategies of callous-unemotional traits

Erschienen in: Italian Journal of Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2017

Abstract

Conduct Disorder (CD) is a psychiatric diagnosis characterized by a repetitive and persistent pattern of behaviour in which the basic rights of others and major age-appropriate social norms or rules are violated. Callous Unemotional (CU) traits are a meaningful specifier in subtyping CD for more severe antisocial and aggressive behaviours in adult psychopathology; they represent the affective dimension of adult psychopathy, but they can be also detected in childhood and adolescence. The CU traits include lack of empathy, sense of guilt and shallow emotion, and their characterization in youth can improve our diagnostic, prognostic and therapeutic abilities. A strong genetic liability, in interaction with parenting and relevant environmental factors, can lead to elevated levels of CU traits in children. We pointed out that CU traits can be detected in early childhood, may remain stable along the adolescence, but a decrease following intensive and specialized treatment is possible. We here provide a narrative review of the available evidences on CU traits in three main domains: aetiology (encompassing genetic liability and environmental risk factors), presentation (early signs and longitudinal trajectories) and treatments.
Abkürzungen
APSD
Antisocial process screening device
CARES
Coaching and rewarding emotional skills
CD
Conduct disorder
CP
Conduct problems
CPTI
Child problematic traits inventory
CU
Callous unemotional
DSM-5
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorder- 5 edition
ERT
Emotion recognition training
ICU
Inventory of callous unemotional traits
LPE
Limited pro-social emotion
PCS
Childhood Psychopathy Scale
PT
Parent training
UNSW system
University of New South Wales system
YPI
Youth psychopathic traits inventory

Background

Conduct Disorder (CD) is a psychiatric diagnosis characterized by a repetitive and persistent pattern of behaviour in which the basic rights of others and major age-appropriate social norms or rules are violated. CD is among the most frequent clinical conditions in child and adolescent mental health [1], with a host of social, emotional, and behavioural problems with high costs for the community. Aggressive behaviours towards other persons or animals or properties, as well as deceitfulness, theft or other severe violations of rules are the core features of this disorder [2]. This category, although necessary for a diagnosis, cannot capture the complexity of the clinical manifestations of CD, which are highly heterogeneous in terms of clinical presentation (high or low level of socialized behaviours, early or late onset, with impulsive or proactive aggression, high or low rate of comorbid affective disorders), outcome (remission or chronicity) and response to treatments (good or poor response to psychoterapy or pharmachotherapy) [3]. The fifth edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health [2] (DSM-5) has defined a new specifier for the CD diagnosis named “with limited prosocial emotions” (LPE); this update should help clinicians and researchers in reducing this heterogeneity, to improve their diagnostic and prognostic abilities [4, 5].
Psychopathic traits, previously considered as a meaningful (negative) specifier for severe antisocial and aggressive behaviours in adult psychopathology, have been re-discovered as a relevant factor in subtyping CD in youth [6]. The current concept of psychopathy comes from Hervey Cleckley’s seminal paper, describing a pattern of personality characterized by low levels of empathy and sense of guilt, arrogance, superficial charm, irresponsible and resulting antisocial behaviours [7]. Subsequent research, based on latent class analysis, better disentangled this issue in three conceptually separated, but inter-correlated domains: an interpersonal domain, consisting of grandiose-manipulative traits; an affective domain, consisting of callous unemotional (CU) traits; and a behavioural domain, consisting of daring-impulsive traits [8]. The grandiose-manipulative domain (also named narcissism) is characterized by verbal and manipulative abilities, superficial charm, egocentricity and glibness. The callous unemotional traits consist of lack of empathy and remorse, with short-lived emotions. The daring-impulsive domain (also named impulsivity or psychopathy-related impulsivity) traits include irresponsibility, proneness to boredom, novelty seeking and antisocial behaviour. Previous research pointed out that these domains are not exclusive of adult psychopathology, but they can also be found in children and adolescents [9], leading to the concept of childhood psychopathy, firstly conceptualized by Forth and colleagues [10], and further supported by Frick [11] and Lynam [12]. Subsequent studies [6, 13] confirmed that the multidimensional structure of adult psychopathy is detectable also in the adolescent populations. Furthermore, these studies on children/adolescents strongly suggested the association between psychopathic personality traits and conduct problems (CP), namely aggression and law violation [1416]. As said before, the, DSM-5 [2] has consolidated this connection, considering the callous unemotional (CU) traits as the LPE specifier for the CD. This specifier, defining a specific subtype of patients with CD, includes symptoms such as lack of remorse or guilt, callous lack of empathy, lack of concern about performance, and shallow or deficient affect. About 12 to 46% of youth with CD show significant CU traits [1720]. The presence of CU traits in children and adolescents with CD defines a subclass of children characterized by a poorer adolescence outcome both in clinical and control samples [21], with increased risk for developing psychopathy in adulthood, with severe and persistent antisocial behaviour [3].
However, these studies raise the issue of translating the measures of CU traits into DSM-5 criteria of CD with LPE. According to DSM-5, CU traits should be present “persistently over at least 12 months and in more than one relationship or setting” [2]. Therefore, it is important to verify whether these features reflect the child’s typical pattern of interpersonal and emotional functioning, and not sporadic occurrences in specific situations.
Disentangling CD patients through an evaluation of the presence of CU traits is a crucial component of the assessment. However, clinical assessment of CU traits may present specific caveats. Firstly, the stability of CU traits is not well established, as not all the children presenting these characteristics at the first evaluation will continue to show them across childhood, and until early adolescence [2224]. Many factors may be implicated in the development and maintenance of the CU traits, although it is hard to separately test the specific contribution of each element, i.e., genetic vs. environmental, to the onset and progression of these traits. Children grow up with their biological parents, thus they receive the genetic heritage, but at the same time, they are exposed to parents’ personality traits as well as to parenting strategies.
In the following sections, we narratively summarize latest findings about the concept of psychopathy in childhood, with a specific focus on their translation into clinical work of health professionals facing with youth with severe behavioural problems. The review will specifically focus on CU traits, given their presence as specifier in DSM-5 criteria of CD. Neuro-cognitive markers, genetic and environmental influences on CU onset will be explored, as well as their implications on developmental trajectories and treatment strategies. We will not cover neurobiological markers of CD and CU traits or ways of assessment, and refer readers to other reviews [9, 25, 26]. See Fig. 1 for a graphical representation of the concept of psychopathy in children, from a personality-based as well as from a DSM-5 oriented perspectives. In the figure have also been resumed the main instruments (self- or parent- report questionnaires) for the evaluation of CU traits in childhood/adolescence; for an extensive review see the work of Masi et al. [26].

Neurocognitive issues

Previous studies showed that altered neuro-cognitive functions (e.g. intelligence, attentional network, processing of external stimuli) may underlie dysfunctional personality disorder features [27]. Similarly, many studies have documented neuro-cognitive correlates of psychopathy in children and adolescent.
In regard to intelligence, it is generally accepted that grandiose manipulative (narcissism) domain is linked to better performances in several cognitive areas, whereas CU traits were negatively associated with verbal intelligence, creativity, practicality and analytic thinking; finally, the daring-impulsive dimension was positively associated with measures of creativity, practicality, and analytic thinking, but not with verbal abilities [28, 29].
In their extensive review Frick et al. [9] pointed out that youth with CU traits show abnormalities in the processing of punishment cues and in the affective empathy (whereas data on cognitive empathy are more controversial); also, there are clear meta analytic evidences that antisocial personality is characterized by impairment in the facial recognition of fear and sadness [30], but also to other emotions and other way of expression (e.g. vocal) [31]. Furthermore, it is evident that high CU traits are associated with a less aversive way to interpret neutral and negative stimuli [32]. It has been suggested that the levels of CU traits are also associated with a generalized impairment in the natural allocation of attention to emotionally salient stimuli that results in cascading errors in recognizing other people’s emotions [33]. All these findings indicate an altered way of processing emotions and external stimuli and may explain why negative reinforcement could fail in the treatment process [32].

Factors contributing to the onset and development of CU traits

In 2014 Frick et al. [34], reviewed nine publications about the heritability of CU traits and found that genetically accounted variations of CU traits ranged from 42% to 68% [3539], and that a great part of the stability in CU traits during development is genetically driven [36, 40, 41]. These studies also indicate that, in front of a large shared genetic effect between the CD and the CU traits, the coexistence of specific genetic influences for both the constructs suggests the existence of a partially specific genetic aetiology [35, 37, 38, 42, 43].
Other factors that may contribute to the onset and the development of CU traits are represented by parents’ temperament and personality traits and by the parent-child interaction.
Two recent studies involving 561 adopted children and their biological mothers and adoptive families explored the first critical issue [44, 45]. In the first study [44], temperament (fearlessness) of the biological mother predicted CU behaviour of the adopted child at 27 months, via earlier fearlessness measured at 18 months; similarly, low affiliative behaviour of biologic mothers directly predicted child CU behaviours, although without any correlation with child affiliative behaviours tested at 18 months. Furthermore, adoptive mothers with highly positive parenting buffered the risk for developing CU traits via fearlessness. In the second study [45], a specific heritable pathway to CU behaviour was suggested: antisocial behaviour of the biological mother predicted CU behaviours of the child in late toddler period. Furthermore, more positively reinforcing mothers buffered inherited risk for early CU behaviours. These relevant studies strongly suggest that positive parenting has an effect on buffering genetic predisposition to develop CU traits.
The relation between parenting and CU traits development is bidirectional [46]. If the association between some features of dysfunctional parenting and the presence of CU traits in offspring has been supported by previous research, on the other hand children showing high CU traits can induce negative modifications in parenting behaviour. Although few studies specifically explored these mutual influences, preliminary evidences suggest that CU traits may be more predictive of changes in parenting over time than parenting on changes in CU traits [47, 48]. Some studies indicated that the presence of CU traits in children with CD is associated with low warmth in parenting [49, 50], others found harsh, inconsistent, and coercive discipline to be highly associated with CP in youth with normative levels of CU traits [5055]. Viding, Fontaine, Oliver, & Plomin [56], in their study on monozygotic twins, found that negative parental discipline operates as a non-shared environmental risk factor for developing CP during the transition to early adolescence, but not for the development of CU traits.
However, studies investigating the association between quality of parenting and prospective change in CU traits in preschool samples have found that multiple domains of parenting (positive parenting, parental involvement, and poor monitoring/supervision) uniquely predicted changes in CU traits [47], and that high CU traits at ages 3–4 were predicted by parent harshness [57]. Other studies have investigated the direct prediction of CU traits by parenting in middle and late childhood. Loney, Huntenburg, Counts-Allan, and Schmeelk [58] assessed the intergenerational continuity of psychopathic traits, and found that the association was mediated by parenting. Parental-reported corporal punishment and child-reported parental warmth/involvement predicted CU traits, as children with low levels of anxiety who reported low parental warmth showed increased CU features 1 year later [59]. Other studies have related an increase in psychopathic characteristics to parental psychological aggression and inconsistency [60], or to poor supervision, physical punishment, and poor parent-child communication [61], or to maternal reports of harsh parenting at age 4 [62]. On the contrary, Vitacco, Neumann, Ramos, and Roberts [63] found that poor parental monitoring and inconsistent discipline were both related to impulsivity and narcissism, but not to CU traits.
In a recent publication exploring developmental trajectories of CU traits, youth with persisting high CU traits had experienced more maternal harshness, low parental knowledge, and monitoring compared to the moderate or low CU youth groups [64]. More specifically, harshness increased significantly across groups from low to high CU traits, and youth with low CU traits reported higher parental warmth, but there were no differences between the warmth reported by high compared to moderated groups.
In a study about effects of positive and negative parenting practices on the levels of child CU in 126 Italian children treated by a multi-modal approach including parent-training, Muratori et al. [65] found that increased levels of positive parenting predicted a decrease in the levels of CU traits in children. In addition, low levels of CU traits at the intermediate evaluation promoted an increase of positive parenting 1 year later. In contrast to previous studies [56], they found that negative parenting practices are unrelated to the level of CU traits when positive parenting is taken into account. Finally, Waller, Shaw and Hyde [66] observed that the presence of fearlessness at 24 months predicted CU behaviours at 42 months, but only when parents exhibited low levels of positive parenting. Furthermore, they showed that this association had a lasting effect on the presence of CU behaviour at 11–12 years. In summary, all these data suggest that the trajectory of children at high risk for CU behaviours could be modified by high levels of positive parenting [67].
In addition to parenting, other environmental and social factors can influence the development of CU traits and, in turn, parenting itself can be influenced by social context (as it as been theorized in 1984 by Belsky [68]). Recently, Waller et al. [67] applied Belsky’s model in relation to the development of CU behaviour. They found that parental warmth at age 2 uniquely predicted parent-reported CU behaviour at ages 10–12, controlling for both concurrent AB and early contextual risk factors across domains [69]. In addition, there was a direct association between neighborhood impoverishment and higher CU behaviour at ages 10–12 and age 20. Moreover, contextual risk and maternal characteristics were indirectly linked to later child CU behaviour by shaping less warm parenting style. These data strengthen the concept that a negative parenting style may enhance the developing of CU behaviour and/or AB, particularly among families with low socio-economic status [46, 67].
Tuvblad and colleagues’ study [70] on genetic and environmental determinants of the psychopathic personality in a community sample of 5-year-old twins indicated that both genetic and shared environmental influences are of importance for psychopathy personality traits in childhood. Regarding the CU dimension, they found a moderate (25%) genetic influence and a higher (48%) shared environmental influence. Shared environmental risk factors include family related factors (e.g., neglect, parental stressors) and contextual factors in the surrounding community.
A recent study from Kahn, Deatre-Deckard, King-Casas and Kim-Spoon [71] has investigated the influences of both parenting and household environment on the intergenerational similarity in CU traits. They found a mediating role of hostile parenting in the association between parent and adolescent CU traits, especially in the context of high household chaos. They concluded that there is a heightened vulnerability to intergenerational transmission of these traits in a contest of household chaos that may exacerbate the effects of hostile parenting on CU traits during adolescence.
A recent meta-analysis showed that low family socio-economic status is associated with higher levels of children’s antisocial behaviours, and indicated that this relationship is stronger when CU traits are considered as outcome variables [72]. Previous researches [39, 73] consistently found that low family’s socio-economic status is the most important predictor of high CU traits. In the study by Muratori et al. [24] involving children with disruptive behaviour disorder, a higher level of socio-economic status at baseline was related to lower level of CU traits. Markowitz et al. [74] extended their research from the family to the neighborhood socio-economic context, by comparing the associations between high-CU traits and delinquency across adolescents living in high-, medium- and low-income neighborhoods in a non clinical sample. They confirmed the association between CU traits and delinquency; furthermore, they found that for high-CU individuals, living in high-income neighborhood hasn’t a protective effect on the likelihood to engage in delinquency compared to the moderate income, high-CU peer group. They also found that neighborhood context has an effect on the type, rather than the extent, of delinquency in which high-CU individuals are involved. In their sample CU traits were more strongly predictive of violent delinquency in low-income neighborhoods and of instrumental delinquency in high-income neighborhoods. Waller et al. [64], found that higher levels of exposure to violence predicted both moderate and high CU traits trajectories, while no associations were found between neighbourhood disorder and CU traits trajectory.
Previous studies have also investigated the relations between children/adolescents with high CU and peers group (for an extensive review see [34], paragraph “Parenting and Peer Risk Factors”). In Barker and Salekin’s study [75], experience of peer victimization at age 10 predicted CU traits at age 13 in children with a high score on a measure of irritability. Another characteristic of children and adolescents with CD and high CU traits may be a higher inclination to join with antisocial and delinquent peers, and consequently, to commit crimes in groups [76], compared to children and adolescents with CD but without CU traits [77]. However, Kerr et al. [78], suggest that adolescents with high CU traits could be less likely influenced by deviant peers group in putting into effect antisocial behaviours, but conversely, they could have a role in influencing antisocial behaviours of their group.

Early signs and developmental trajectories

Although most of the studies about CU traits associated to CD are focused on children and adolescents, more recently the interest has been expanded to early childhood and toddler age, in order to detect early sings of CU behaviours. The possibility of detecting the origins of CU traits early in childhood is given by the emerging knowledge about individual differences in empathy and conscience as early as at the age of 2 or 3 years [79]. Consequently, several measures were developed or adapted to specifically assess possible precursors of CU traits (for a review see [80]). Aim of these studies is a timely identification of children at high risk of developing CD or antisocial behaviours, in a developmental phase with more malleable behaviour, when early interventions can be more effective [81]. In this earlier age, “CU behaviours” is a more adequate definition rather than “CU traits”, given the lesser evidences about their stability over time [67].
Higher CU behaviours in children as early as 3 year-old are related to lower guilt and empathy, more proactive aggression [67], and, perspectively, to CU traits in late childhood [67, 82, 83].
In his seminal work about the neurobiological basis of psychopathy, Blair [84] proposed that CU traits and behaviours emerge from a fearless temperament expressed through low arousal to others’ distress and punishment, leading to reduced learning about the consequences of harmful behaviours, and increasing the risk of CU behaviours. The measure of CU behaviours in children focuses on the presence of lack of empathy and guilt, in addition to a reduced emotional responsivity to others’ feelings or cues [21]. As a possible early indicator of CU behaviours in the adoption study mentioned above, Waller et al. [44] showed that the experimental observation of higher fearlessness and low affiliative behaviours at 18 months were related to higher CU behaviours referred by adoptive mothers at 27 months.
Recently, Goffin et al. [85] published the results of a long-term longitudinal study exploring early antecedents of CU traits and their evolution in typically developing children from toddler age to 12. They use observational measures to score guilt (in breaking a valued object situation), empathy (hurting the parent during play) and fearlessness in toddlers and preschoolers, and related these findings tolater parent-rated measures of CU traits and externalizing behaviours during scholar and pre-adolescent age. They found that children who were unconcerned to transgression (either breaking objects or hurting others) early in development exhibited higher CU traits in middle childhood. However, those links were moderated by child’s early temperament, being significant only in children high on fearlessness and not in fearful ones.
The developmental stability of CU traits has been investigated in several studies. In their review, Frick et al. [34] report a relative stability of CU traits from the age of 3–4 years-old according to parent rating. Although the CU trait levels tend to decreases across childhood and adolescence in a large number of children, those showing an elevated level of CU traits are at higher risk for keeping them in older ages. Other longitudinal studies have suggested that CU traits are relatively stable across childhood, and from childhood to adolescence. Dadds and colleagues [22] found moderate 1-year stability estimates for features of callousness (r = .55) in a community sample of young Australian children. Frick and colleagues [86] reported high 4-year stability estimates for parent ratings of callousness (interclass correlation = .71) from late childhood to middle adolescence. A more recent study of longitudinal invariance found that the rank-order stability of CU was moderate (r = .50, p < .001) during an 8-year period from childhood into adolescence among boys [87]. Fontaine, McCrory, Boivin, Moffitt, and Viding [73], using a person-centered approach (growth mixture modeling), showed that in children substantial decreases in CU traits across development were more common than substantial increases (see also Frick et al., [86]).
Also, Klingzell et al. [88] investigated joint trajectories of CU traits and conduct problems during early childhood in a community sample and found a close relationship between them (e.g. CU and conduct problems tend to decrease together or remain stable together) and with fearlessness and psychopathic traits. Despite the differences in stability across studies, these estimates of stability are comparable to those reported for others psychopathological constructs [89].

Treatment

Conduct problems are usually highly impairing social, familial, and school functioning, and a management of this impairment is needed. Some specifiers, such as CU traits (or LPE DSM-5 specifier), as well as early age of onset of symptoms and comorbidities, should promote more timely referrals, assessments, and possibly an intensive treatment, and follow-ups.
The treatment of CP among children and adolescents with CU traits has been explored in depth, but so far, neither a proven psychological nor a definitive psychopharmacological treatment is available [90]. A recent systematic review by Hawes, Price, and Dadds [91] showed that CU traits were associated with increased risk for poor post-treatment outcomes. However, recent research has also suggested that children and adolescents with elevated CU traits are not “untreatable” and that they can improve with intensive treatments, tailored to the unique emotional, cognitive, and motivational styles [92].
Some recent and promising treatments specifically focused in ameliorating CU traits in the context of behavioural problems will be presented below and summarized in Table 1.
Table 1
Currently available psychotherapeutic treatments for children with conduct disorder and high CU traits
Name
Age range
Treatment target
Time range
Directed to (children, parents, teachers etc.)
Main references
CARES Module
3.5–8
Improvement of emotion recognition and labelling; enhancement of pro-social and empathic behaviour; increase of child’s frustration tolerance.
6 weeks
Children
Datyner et al., 2016
ERT
6–16
Enhancement of emotion recognition and interpretation; improvements of empathic abilities.
4 sessions (90 min each)
Children
Dadds et al., [94]
Mental Models
Adolescents
Increase positive emotion and reduce negative affect; improvement of decision-making skills; reduction of psychopathic features.
12 weeks
Children
Salekin et al., [96]
CP Program
7–14
Improvement of emotion recognition, especially anger; increase of child’s ability to cope with anger arousal; enhancement of perspective taking ability and problem solving skills; improvement of parenting skills.
12 months
Children and parents
Lochman and Wells, [97]; Muratori et al., [98]
Legend: CARES coaching and rewarding emotional skills, ERT emotion recognition training, CP coping power
Coaching and Rewarding Emotional Skills (CARES) Module is a brief emotional training program for assisting empathy and emotional development in young children with CP and CU traits. The CARES module was designed to be delivered to children between the ages of 3.5 and 8 years with non-normative levels of CU traits immediately after completion of parent management training, and when CP have been reduced to below clinical significance. CARES is the first targeted treatment designed for young children to ameliorate empathy-related deficits in processing negative emotions central to CU traits. The key treatment objectives of CARES are: (a) to enhance attention to critical facial cues signalling distress in child, parents and others, to improve emotion recognition and labelling; (b) improve emotional understanding by linking emotion to context, and by identifying contexts and situations that elicit child anger and frustration; (c) teach prosocial and empathic behaviour through social stories, parent modelling, and role play; (d) increase emotional labelling and prosocial behaviour through positive reinforcement; (e) and increase child’s frustration tolerance through modelling, role-playing, and reinforcing child’s use of learned cognitive-behavioural strategies to decrease the incidence of aggressive behaviours. Datyner et al. [93], in a case study, provided initial support for the short-term effectiveness of a brief adjunctive module to a PT intervention, to address empathy and emotions recognition deficits, and CU traits in children with CP + CU.
Emotion recognition training (ERT) [94] associated an emotion recognition training (ERT) and a PT intervention (Family Intervention for Child Conduct Problems). The authors showed that this association can lead to significant improvement in empathy and behavioural problems. Dadds et al. [94] pointed out that ERT could be added to usual intervention for clinically referred children with high CU traits. The ERT was partly based on the MindReading [95] program originally developed to train children with autism to accurately identify and interpret emotional expressions in interpersonal contexts. Mind Reading is a reference framework covering the entire spectrum of human emotions. Using the software, people can explore over 400 emotions, seeing and hearing each one performed by six different people. It includes an Emotion Library, a Learning Centre and a Game Zone. Studies showed that ERT produced significant improvements in affective empathy and CP in children with CD and high CU traits [94]. The association of ERT and PT seems to increase the level of reciprocated eye contact [91], and this should lead to enhancement of parent-child relationship.
Salekin et al. [96] tested a positive psychological intervention, named Mental Models, to reduce the behaviour problems in youth with higher CU traits. Using a combination of motivational techniques, cognitive behaviour training, and instruction on positive emotion, youth received 12 didactic sessions. The intervention was group-focused and included group sizes of four to 24 (mode = 6). The first group included all youth (n = 24), and the group size was then reduced to six individuals per group. The intervention involved a motivational component, whereby youth were motivated to participate in treatment. This component included some discussions on brain development and the new neural connections that can result through the process of active learning. Each week, many of the exercises were geared toward increasing positive emotions and positive ways of interacting with other individuals. Some exercises included writing assignments, entailing youth communicating their thoughts on strengths, ways to problem-solve, identification of emotions, and so forth. Youth were provided ways of thinking about these goals (models) and were also asked to generate their own ideas for setting goals and making plans for the future, to verbalize goals internally, and to mentally visualize the steps to accomplish a given goal, or to a better decision-making (including improving their choices, behaviour and citizenship within the institution). According to Salekin et al. [96], the intervention was effective at reducing interpersonal CU traits in youth, as well as improving their amenability to treatment.
Although these new interventions specifically developed for children with CD and elevated CU traits are promising, other more traditional treatments addressed for behavioural problems are available [25]. The Coping Power Program is a multi-component treatment program, delivered in a group setting, and has been developed using a contextual social–cognitive model as a conceptual framework for identifying intervention objectives [97]. The contextual social–cognitive model focuses on the contextual parenting processes, and on children’s sequential cognitive processing in the development and escalation of children’s behavioural problems. The CopingPower-child component includes 36 group sessions delivered over 12 months. In addition, 16 parent sessions are delivered over the same period. Parents also participate in small group sessions of five families; usually one parent per family attends the sessions. The child and his/her parent receive the treatment on the same day.
A recent study [98] found that the Coping Power program can lead to a significant decrease in CU traits, and this reduction was maintained at the 1-year follow-up evaluation. Authors hypothesized that the specific intervention on parenting practices promoted the decrease in CU traits in children; and that Coping Power group therapy can improve emotion sharing with peers, children’s understanding of other people’s emotions, and consequently has a beneficial influence on their empathy as well [99].
Regarding medications, a recent review [100] suggests that methylphenidate and risperidone present the largest effects on aggression, while other antipsychotics show clinical efficacy on CD, but this evidence is mainly supported by open label trials. There is some low quality evidence to support a small effect of mood stabilizers and other agents. Nevertheless, antipsychotic drugs are associated with meaningful adverse events (namely metabolic disturbances such as obesity, diabetes, dyslipidemia [101, 102] that requires great caution in their use in childhood. No drug treatments specifically work for CU traits, and they may be predictors of poor response to pharmacological treatments in CD, although data are still inconclusive [100, 103]. We have recently shown that a pharmacotherapy may constitute an important added value to psychosocial interventions in DBD, improving emotional dysregulation but not CU traits [104].

Discussion

The aim of this paper was to provide a brief review of the main features of childhood psychopathy, and specifically on the CU traits, which represent its affective dimension (e.g. which encompasses multiple deficits in emotional processing). Paediatricians manage the health of children and adolescents since their birth, including behaviour and mental health issues, and, usually, they are the primary care specialists who provide the first contact for children and their families. Therefore, they could represent the first step in identifying the presence of CU traits, and the possible developmental pathways to antisocial behaviours, and in promoting early interventions. Additionally, considering the caveats in clinical assessment, paediatricians may help to identify the “persistently over at least 12 months and in more than one relationship or setting” DMS-5 criterion.
This and other reviews [21, 26, 46, 72, 79] clearly demonstrate that CU traits are highly heritable, that a timely identification is possible, since early years, and that they are associated with identifiable environmental risk factors. Also, they can be targeted with specific interventions, and, to some extent, partially modifiable. Well designed longitudinal studies, both naturalistic and intervention, mainly in clinical samples, are needed to further delineate developmental trajectories, their predictors, and how CU traits alone or interacting with behavioural symptoms predict the outcomes.
Implications are straightforward. A thorough knowledge of family interactions, easily available for paediatricians, is crucial for early identification as well for a therapeutic alliance with parents and children. Some of the existing instruments can be possibly implemented in paediatric settings [26], even if an easier and quicker questionnaire, more suitable for a population based screen, is still lacking. Further researches should fill this gap.
The identification of behavioural problems and CU traits during childhood may be helpful to promote early interventions. As pointed out in the paper, there are currently available some promising treatment programs, specifically addressed for children and adolescents with CD and high level of CU traits (e.g., Coping Power Program, CARES, ERT, Mental Models), whose main targets are the improvement of emotion recognition, the enhancement of prosocial and empathic behaviour, and the improvement of parenting skills. Well designed, randomized trials should be the next step for evidence based treatments directly targeting CU traits.

Conclusion

As a summary, we conclude that a strong genetic liability, in interaction with parenting and relevant environmental factors, can lead to elevated levels of CU traits; they can be detected in early childhood, may remain stable along the adolescence, but a possible decrease following intensive and specialized treatment is possible. We maintain that a strict collaboration among paediatricians, child psychiatrists, clinical psychologists and all other professionals devoted to child mental and physical health is needed to early detection and management of these severely impaired patients.

Acknowledgments

Not applicable.

Funding

Not applicable.

Availability of data and materials

Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.

Ethics approval

Not applicable.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

GM was in the advisory boards for Eli Lilly, Shire and Angelini, has received research grants from Eli Lilly, Shire, and Lundbeck, and has been speaker for FB, Eli Lilly, Shire, Lundbeck, and Otsuka. All the other authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Gritti A, Bravaccio C, Signoriello S, Salerno F, Pisano S, Catone G, et al. Epidemiological study on behavioural and emotional problems in developmental age: prevalence in a sample of Italian children, based on parent and teacher reports. Ital J Pediatr. 2014;40:19.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gritti A, Bravaccio C, Signoriello S, Salerno F, Pisano S, Catone G, et al. Epidemiological study on behavioural and emotional problems in developmental age: prevalence in a sample of Italian children, based on parent and teacher reports. Ital J Pediatr. 2014;40:19.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
2.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorder - 5th ed. Washington, DC: Author; 2013.CrossRef American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorder - 5th ed. Washington, DC: Author; 2013.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, Viding E. Antisocial behavior from a developmental psychopathology perspective. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21:1111–31.CrossRefPubMed Frick PJ, Viding E. Antisocial behavior from a developmental psychopathology perspective. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21:1111–31.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Jambroes T, Jansen LM, Vermeiren RR, Doreleijers TA, Colins OF, Popma A. The clinical usefulness of the new LPE specifier for subtyping adolescents with conduct disorder in the DSM 5. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;25:891–902.CrossRefPubMed Jambroes T, Jansen LM, Vermeiren RR, Doreleijers TA, Colins OF, Popma A. The clinical usefulness of the new LPE specifier for subtyping adolescents with conduct disorder in the DSM 5. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;25:891–902.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Vanwoerden S, Reuter T, Sharp C. Exploring the clinical utility of the DSM-5 conduct disorder specifier of 'with limited prosocial emotions' in an adolescent inpatient sample. Compr Psychiatry. 2016;69:116–31.CrossRefPubMed Vanwoerden S, Reuter T, Sharp C. Exploring the clinical utility of the DSM-5 conduct disorder specifier of 'with limited prosocial emotions' in an adolescent inpatient sample. Compr Psychiatry. 2016;69:116–31.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, Barry CT, Bodin SD. Applying the concept of psychopathy to children: implication for the assessment of antisocial in youth. In: Gacono CB, editor. The clinical and forensic assessment of psychopaty: a practitioner's guide. Mahwah: Erlbaum; 2000. p. 3–24. Frick PJ, Barry CT, Bodin SD. Applying the concept of psychopathy to children: implication for the assessment of antisocial in youth. In: Gacono CB, editor. The clinical and forensic assessment of psychopaty: a practitioner's guide. Mahwah: Erlbaum; 2000. p. 3–24.
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Cleckley H. The mask of sanity (5th ed). St. Louis: Mosby; 1976. Cleckley H. The mask of sanity (5th ed). St. Louis: Mosby; 1976.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Hare RD. Manual for the revised Psychopathy checklist. Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. 1991/2003. Hare RD. Manual for the revised Psychopathy checklist. Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. 1991/2003.
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, White SF. Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49:359–75.CrossRefPubMed Frick PJ, White SF. Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49:359–75.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Forth AE, Hart SD, Hare RD. Assessment of psychopathy in male young offenders. Psychol Assess. 1990;2:342–4.CrossRef Forth AE, Hart SD, Hare RD. Assessment of psychopathy in male young offenders. Psychol Assess. 1990;2:342–4.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, O'Brien BS, Wootton JM, McBurnett K. Psychopathy and conduct problem in children. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:700–7.CrossRefPubMed Frick PJ, O'Brien BS, Wootton JM, McBurnett K. Psychopathy and conduct problem in children. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:700–7.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Lynam DR. Pursuing the psychopath: capturing the fledgling psychopath in a nomological net. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106:425–38.CrossRefPubMed Lynam DR. Pursuing the psychopath: capturing the fledgling psychopath in a nomological net. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106:425–38.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Kotler JS, McMahon RG. Child psychopathy: theories, measurement, and relation with the development and persistence of conduct problems. Clin Child Fam Psychol. 2005;8:291–325.CrossRef Kotler JS, McMahon RG. Child psychopathy: theories, measurement, and relation with the development and persistence of conduct problems. Clin Child Fam Psychol. 2005;8:291–325.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Asscher JJ, van Vugt ES, Stams GJ, Deković M, Eichelsheim VI, Yousfi S. The relationship between juvenile psychopathic traits, delinquency and (violent) recidivism: a meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:1134–43.CrossRefPubMed Asscher JJ, van Vugt ES, Stams GJ, Deković M, Eichelsheim VI, Yousfi S. The relationship between juvenile psychopathic traits, delinquency and (violent) recidivism: a meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:1134–43.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Lynam DR, Charnigo R, Moffitt TE, Raine A, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M. The stability of psychopathy across adolescence. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21:1133–53.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lynam DR, Charnigo R, Moffitt TE, Raine A, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M. The stability of psychopathy across adolescence. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21:1133–53.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Lopez-Romero L, Romero E, Luengo MA. Disentangling the role of psychopathic traits and externalizing behavior in predicting conduct problems from childhood to adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2012;41:1397–408.CrossRefPubMed Lopez-Romero L, Romero E, Luengo MA. Disentangling the role of psychopathic traits and externalizing behavior in predicting conduct problems from childhood to adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2012;41:1397–408.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Fanti KA. Individual, social, and behavioral factors associated with co-occurring conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013;41:811–24.CrossRefPubMed Fanti KA. Individual, social, and behavioral factors associated with co-occurring conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013;41:811–24.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahn RE, Frick PJ, Youngstrom E, Findling RL, Youngstrom JK. The effects of including a callous-unemotional specifier for the diagnosis of conduct disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:271–82.CrossRefPubMed Kahn RE, Frick PJ, Youngstrom E, Findling RL, Youngstrom JK. The effects of including a callous-unemotional specifier for the diagnosis of conduct disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:271–82.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Pardini D, Stepp S, Hipwell A, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Loeber R. The clinical utility of the proposed DSM-5 callous-unemotional subtype of conduct disorder in young girls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51:62–73.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pardini D, Stepp S, Hipwell A, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Loeber R. The clinical utility of the proposed DSM-5 callous-unemotional subtype of conduct disorder in young girls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51:62–73.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Rowe R, Costello EJ, Angold A, Copeland WE, Maughan B. Developmental pathways in oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010;119:726.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rowe R, Costello EJ, Angold A, Copeland WE, Maughan B. Developmental pathways in oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010;119:726.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Annual research review: a developmental psychopathology approach to understanding callous-unemotional traits in children and adolescents with serious conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55:532–48.CrossRefPubMed Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Annual research review: a developmental psychopathology approach to understanding callous-unemotional traits in children and adolescents with serious conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55:532–48.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Dadds MR, Fraser J, Frost A, Hawes DJ. Disentangling the underlying dimensions of psychopathy and conduct problems in childhood: a community study. J Consult Clinical Psychol. 2005;73:400.CrossRef Dadds MR, Fraser J, Frost A, Hawes DJ. Disentangling the underlying dimensions of psychopathy and conduct problems in childhood: a community study. J Consult Clinical Psychol. 2005;73:400.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, Cornell AH, Barry CT, Bodin SD, Dane HE. Callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in the prediction of conduct problem severity, aggression, and self-report of delinquency. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2003;31:457–70.CrossRefPubMed Frick PJ, Cornell AH, Barry CT, Bodin SD, Dane HE. Callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in the prediction of conduct problem severity, aggression, and self-report of delinquency. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2003;31:457–70.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Muratori P, Lochman JE, Manfredi A, Milone A, Nocentini A, Pisano S, et al. Callous unemotional traits in children with disruptive behavior disorder: predictors of developmental trajectories and adolescent outcomes. Psychiatry Res. 2016;236:35–41.CrossRefPubMed Muratori P, Lochman JE, Manfredi A, Milone A, Nocentini A, Pisano S, et al. Callous unemotional traits in children with disruptive behavior disorder: predictors of developmental trajectories and adolescent outcomes. Psychiatry Res. 2016;236:35–41.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Casagrande M, Marotta A, Canepone V, Spagna A, Rosa C, Dimaggio G, et al. Dysfunctional personality traits in adolescence: effects on alerting, orienting and executive control of attention. Cogn Process. 2017;18:183–93.CrossRefPubMed Casagrande M, Marotta A, Canepone V, Spagna A, Rosa C, Dimaggio G, et al. Dysfunctional personality traits in adolescence: effects on alerting, orienting and executive control of attention. Cogn Process. 2017;18:183–93.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Salekin RT, Neumann CS, Leistico AM, Zalot AA. Psychopathy in youth and intelligence: an investigation of Cleckley’s hypothesis. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004;33:731–42.CrossRefPubMed Salekin RT, Neumann CS, Leistico AM, Zalot AA. Psychopathy in youth and intelligence: an investigation of Cleckley’s hypothesis. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004;33:731–42.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Feilhauer J, Cima M. Youth psychopathy: differential correlates of callous-unemotional traits, narcissism, and impulsivity. Forensic Sci Int. 2013;224:1–7.CrossRefPubMed Feilhauer J, Cima M. Youth psychopathy: differential correlates of callous-unemotional traits, narcissism, and impulsivity. Forensic Sci Int. 2013;224:1–7.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Marsh AA, Blair RJ. Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;32:454–65.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Marsh AA, Blair RJ. Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;32:454–65.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Dawel A, O'Kearney R, McKone E, Palermo R. Not just fear and sadness: meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:2288–304.CrossRefPubMed Dawel A, O'Kearney R, McKone E, Palermo R. Not just fear and sadness: meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:2288–304.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Masi G, Milone A, Pisano S, Lenzi F, Muratori P, Gemo I, et al. Emotional reactivity in referred youth with disruptive behavior disorders: the role of the callous-unemotional traits. Psychiatry Res. 2014;220:426–32.CrossRefPubMed Masi G, Milone A, Pisano S, Lenzi F, Muratori P, Gemo I, et al. Emotional reactivity in referred youth with disruptive behavior disorders: the role of the callous-unemotional traits. Psychiatry Res. 2014;220:426–32.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Dadds MR, Perry Y, Hawes DJ, Merz S, Riddell AC, Haines DJ, et al. Attention to the eyes and fear-recognition deficits in child psychopathy. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:280–1.CrossRefPubMed Dadds MR, Perry Y, Hawes DJ, Merz S, Riddell AC, Haines DJ, et al. Attention to the eyes and fear-recognition deficits in child psychopathy. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:280–1.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol Bull. 2014;140:1–57.CrossRefPubMed Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol Bull. 2014;140:1–57.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA, Lynam DR. Psychopathic personality in children: genetic and environmental contributions. Psychol Med. 2011;41:589–600.CrossRefPubMed Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA, Lynam DR. Psychopathic personality in children: genetic and environmental contributions. Psychol Med. 2011;41:589–600.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG. Continuity and change in psychopathic traits as measured via normal-range personality: a longitudinal-biometric study. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115:85–95.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG. Continuity and change in psychopathic traits as measured via normal-range personality: a longitudinal-biometric study. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115:85–95.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Larsson H, Andershed H, Lichtenstein P. A genetic factor explains most of the variation in the psychopathic personality. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115:221–30.CrossRefPubMed Larsson H, Andershed H, Lichtenstein P. A genetic factor explains most of the variation in the psychopathic personality. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115:221–30.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor J, Loney BR, Bobadilla L, Iacono WG, McGue M. Genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy trait dimensions in a community sample of male twins. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2003;31:633–45.CrossRefPubMed Taylor J, Loney BR, Bobadilla L, Iacono WG, McGue M. Genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy trait dimensions in a community sample of male twins. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2003;31:633–45.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Viding E, Blair RJ, Moffitt TE, Plomin R. Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46:592–7.CrossRefPubMed Viding E, Blair RJ, Moffitt TE, Plomin R. Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46:592–7.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Fontaine NM, Rijsdijk FV, McCrory EJ, Viding E. Etiology of different developmental trajectories of callous-unemotional traits. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:656–64.PubMed Fontaine NM, Rijsdijk FV, McCrory EJ, Viding E. Etiology of different developmental trajectories of callous-unemotional traits. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:656–64.PubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Forsman M, Lichtenstein P, Andershed H, Larsson H. Genetic effects explain the stability of psychopathic personality from mid- to late adolescence. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008;117:606–17.CrossRefPubMed Forsman M, Lichtenstein P, Andershed H, Larsson H. Genetic effects explain the stability of psychopathic personality from mid- to late adolescence. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008;117:606–17.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Viding E, Frick PJ, Plomin R. Aetiology of the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;190(Suppl):33–8.CrossRef Viding E, Frick PJ, Plomin R. Aetiology of the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;190(Suppl):33–8.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Waldman ID, Tackett JL, Van Hulle CA, Applegate B, Pardini D, Frick PJ, et al. Child and adolescent conduct disorder substantially shares genetic influences with three socioemotional dispositions. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011;120:57–70.CrossRefPubMed Waldman ID, Tackett JL, Van Hulle CA, Applegate B, Pardini D, Frick PJ, et al. Child and adolescent conduct disorder substantially shares genetic influences with three socioemotional dispositions. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011;120:57–70.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Waller R, Trentacosta CJ, Shaw DS, Neiderhiser JM, Ganiban JM, Reiss D, et al. Heritable temperament pathways to early callous-unemotional behaviour. Br J Psychiatry. 2016;209:275–82.CrossRef Waller R, Trentacosta CJ, Shaw DS, Neiderhiser JM, Ganiban JM, Reiss D, et al. Heritable temperament pathways to early callous-unemotional behaviour. Br J Psychiatry. 2016;209:275–82.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Hyde LW, Waller R, Trentacosta CJ, Shaw DS, Neiderhiser JM, Ganiban JM, et al. Heritable and nonheritable pathways to early callous-unemotional behaviors. Am J Psychiatry. 2016;173:903–10.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hyde LW, Waller R, Trentacosta CJ, Shaw DS, Neiderhiser JM, Ganiban JM, et al. Heritable and nonheritable pathways to early callous-unemotional behaviors. Am J Psychiatry. 2016;173:903–10.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Waller R, Gardner F, Hyde LW. What are the associations between parenting, callous- unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior in youth? A systematic review of evidence. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33:593–608.CrossRefPubMed Waller R, Gardner F, Hyde LW. What are the associations between parenting, callous- unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior in youth? A systematic review of evidence. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33:593–608.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Hawes DJ, Dadds MR, Frost ADJ, Hasking PA. Do childhood callous-unemotional traits drive change in parenting practice? J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011;40:507–18.CrossRefPubMed Hawes DJ, Dadds MR, Frost ADJ, Hasking PA. Do childhood callous-unemotional traits drive change in parenting practice? J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011;40:507–18.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Muñoz LC, Pakalniskiene V, Frick PJ. Parental monitoring and youth behavior problems: moderation by callous-unemotional traits over time. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;20:261–9.CrossRefPubMed Muñoz LC, Pakalniskiene V, Frick PJ. Parental monitoring and youth behavior problems: moderation by callous-unemotional traits over time. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;20:261–9.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Kroneman LM, Hipwell AE, Loeber R, Knoot HM, Pardini DA. Contextual risk factors as predictors of disruptive behavior disorder trajectories in girls: the moderating effect of callous-unemotional features. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:167–75.CrossRefPubMed Kroneman LM, Hipwell AE, Loeber R, Knoot HM, Pardini DA. Contextual risk factors as predictors of disruptive behavior disorder trajectories in girls: the moderating effect of callous-unemotional features. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:167–75.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Pasalich DS, Dadds MR, Hawes DJ, Brennan J. Do callous-unemotional traits moderate the relative importance of parental coercion versus warmth in child conduct problems? An observational study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:1308–15.CrossRefPubMed Pasalich DS, Dadds MR, Hawes DJ, Brennan J. Do callous-unemotional traits moderate the relative importance of parental coercion versus warmth in child conduct problems? An observational study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:1308–15.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Hipwell AE, Pardini DA, Loeber R, Sembower M, Keenan K, Stouthamer-Loeber M. Callous-unemotional behaviours in young girls: shared and unique effects relative to conduct problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2007;36:293–304.CrossRefPubMed Hipwell AE, Pardini DA, Loeber R, Sembower M, Keenan K, Stouthamer-Loeber M. Callous-unemotional behaviours in young girls: shared and unique effects relative to conduct problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2007;36:293–304.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Edens JF, Skopp NA, Cahill MA. Psychopathic features moderate the relationship between harsh and inconsistent parental discipline and adolescent antisocial behavior. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2008;37:472–6.CrossRefPubMed Edens JF, Skopp NA, Cahill MA. Psychopathic features moderate the relationship between harsh and inconsistent parental discipline and adolescent antisocial behavior. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2008;37:472–6.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Oxford M, Cavell TA, Huges JN. Callous/unemotional traits moderate the relation between ineffective parenting and child externalizing problems: a partial replication and extension. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2003;32:577–85.CrossRefPubMed Oxford M, Cavell TA, Huges JN. Callous/unemotional traits moderate the relation between ineffective parenting and child externalizing problems: a partial replication and extension. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2003;32:577–85.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Wootton JM, Frick PJ, Shelton KK, Silverthorn P. Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct problems: the moderating role of callous-unemotional traits. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1997;65:301–8.CrossRefPubMed Wootton JM, Frick PJ, Shelton KK, Silverthorn P. Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct problems: the moderating role of callous-unemotional traits. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1997;65:301–8.CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Yeh MY, Chen P, Raine A, Baker LA, Jacobson KC. Child psychopathic traits moderate relationships between parental affect and child aggression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50:1054–64.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Yeh MY, Chen P, Raine A, Baker LA, Jacobson KC. Child psychopathic traits moderate relationships between parental affect and child aggression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50:1054–64.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Viding E, Fontaine NMG, Oliver BR, Plomin R. Negative parental discipline, conduct problems, and callous-unemotional traits: monozygotic twin differenes study. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195:414–9.CrossRefPubMed Viding E, Fontaine NMG, Oliver BR, Plomin R. Negative parental discipline, conduct problems, and callous-unemotional traits: monozygotic twin differenes study. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195:414–9.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Waller R, Gardner F, Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN. Do harsh and positive parenting predict parent reports of deceitful-callous behavior in early childhood? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:946–53.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Waller R, Gardner F, Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN. Do harsh and positive parenting predict parent reports of deceitful-callous behavior in early childhood? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:946–53.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Loney BR, Huntenburg A, Counts-Allan C, Schmeelk KM. A preliminary examination of the intergenerational continuity of maternal psychopathic features. Aggress Behav. 2007;33:14–25.CrossRefPubMed Loney BR, Huntenburg A, Counts-Allan C, Schmeelk KM. A preliminary examination of the intergenerational continuity of maternal psychopathic features. Aggress Behav. 2007;33:14–25.CrossRefPubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Pardini DA, Lochman JE, Powell N. The development of callous-unemotional traits and antisocial behavior in children: are there shared and/or unique predictors? J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2007;36:319–33.CrossRefPubMed Pardini DA, Lochman JE, Powell N. The development of callous-unemotional traits and antisocial behavior in children: are there shared and/or unique predictors? J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2007;36:319–33.CrossRefPubMed
60.
Zurück zum Zitat McDonald R, Dodson MC, Rosenfield D, Jouriles EN. Effects of a parenting intervention on features of psychopathy in children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011;39:1013–23.CrossRefPubMed McDonald R, Dodson MC, Rosenfield D, Jouriles EN. Effects of a parenting intervention on features of psychopathy in children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011;39:1013–23.CrossRefPubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Pardini DA, Loeber R. Interpersonal callousness trajectories across adolescence: early social influences and adult outcomes. Crim Justice Behav. 2008;35:173–96.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pardini DA, Loeber R. Interpersonal callousness trajectories across adolescence: early social influences and adult outcomes. Crim Justice Behav. 2008;35:173–96.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Barker ED, Oliver BR, Viding E, Salekin RT, Maughan B. The impact of prenatal maternal risk, fearless temperament, and early parenting on adolescent callous-unemotional traits: a 14-years longitudinal investigation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:878–88.CrossRefPubMed Barker ED, Oliver BR, Viding E, Salekin RT, Maughan B. The impact of prenatal maternal risk, fearless temperament, and early parenting on adolescent callous-unemotional traits: a 14-years longitudinal investigation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:878–88.CrossRefPubMed
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Vitacco MJ, Neumann CS, Ramos V, Roberts MK. Ineffective parenting: a precursor to psychopathic traits and delinquency in Hispanic females. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1008:300–3.CrossRefPubMed Vitacco MJ, Neumann CS, Ramos V, Roberts MK. Ineffective parenting: a precursor to psychopathic traits and delinquency in Hispanic females. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1008:300–3.CrossRefPubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Waller R, Baskin-Sommers AR, Hyde LW. Examining predictors of callous unemotional traits trajectories across adolescence among high-risk males. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2016;22:1–14.CrossRef Waller R, Baskin-Sommers AR, Hyde LW. Examining predictors of callous unemotional traits trajectories across adolescence among high-risk males. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2016;22:1–14.CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Muratori P, Lochman JE, Lai E, Milone A, Nocentini A, Pisano S, et al. Which dimension of parenting predicts the change of callous unemotional traits in children with disruptive behavior disorder? Compr Psychiatry. 2016;69:202–10.CrossRefPubMed Muratori P, Lochman JE, Lai E, Milone A, Nocentini A, Pisano S, et al. Which dimension of parenting predicts the change of callous unemotional traits in children with disruptive behavior disorder? Compr Psychiatry. 2016;69:202–10.CrossRefPubMed
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Waller R, Shaw DS, Hyde LW. Observed fearlessness and positive parenting interact to predict childhood callous-unemotional behaviors among low-income boys. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58:282–91.CrossRefPubMed Waller R, Shaw DS, Hyde LW. Observed fearlessness and positive parenting interact to predict childhood callous-unemotional behaviors among low-income boys. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58:282–91.CrossRefPubMed
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Waller R, Shaw DS, Neiderhiser JM, Ganiban JM, Natsuaki MN, Reiss D, et al. Toward an understanding of the role of the environment in the development of early callous behaviour. J Pers. 2017;85:90–103.CrossRefPubMed Waller R, Shaw DS, Neiderhiser JM, Ganiban JM, Natsuaki MN, Reiss D, et al. Toward an understanding of the role of the environment in the development of early callous behaviour. J Pers. 2017;85:90–103.CrossRefPubMed
68.
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Waller R, Shaw DS, Forbes EE, Hyde LW. Understanding early contextual and parental risk factors for the development of limited prosocial emotions. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2015;43:1025–39.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Waller R, Shaw DS, Forbes EE, Hyde LW. Understanding early contextual and parental risk factors for the development of limited prosocial emotions. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2015;43:1025–39.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Tuvblad C, Fanti KA, Andershed H, Colins OF, Larsson H. Psychopathic personality traits in 5 years old twins: the importance of genetic and shared environmental influences. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;26:469–79.CrossRefPubMed Tuvblad C, Fanti KA, Andershed H, Colins OF, Larsson H. Psychopathic personality traits in 5 years old twins: the importance of genetic and shared environmental influences. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;26:469–79.CrossRefPubMed
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahn RE, Deater-Deckard K, King-Casas B, Kim-Spoon J. Intergenerational similarity in callous-unemotional traits: contributions of hostile parenting and household chaos during adolescence. Psychiatry Res. 2016;246:815–20.CrossRefPubMed Kahn RE, Deater-Deckard K, King-Casas B, Kim-Spoon J. Intergenerational similarity in callous-unemotional traits: contributions of hostile parenting and household chaos during adolescence. Psychiatry Res. 2016;246:815–20.CrossRefPubMed
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Piotrowska PJ, Stride CB, Croft SE, Rowe R. Socioeconomic status and antisocial behaviour among children and adolescent: a sistematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015;35:47–55.CrossRefPubMed Piotrowska PJ, Stride CB, Croft SE, Rowe R. Socioeconomic status and antisocial behaviour among children and adolescent: a sistematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015;35:47–55.CrossRefPubMed
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Fontaine NM, McCrory EJ, Boivin M, Moffitt TE, Viding E. Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011;120:730–42.CrossRefPubMed Fontaine NM, McCrory EJ, Boivin M, Moffitt TE, Viding E. Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011;120:730–42.CrossRefPubMed
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Markowitz AJ, Ryan RM, Marsh AA. Neighborhood income and the expression of callous–unemotional traits. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;24:1103–18.CrossRefPubMed Markowitz AJ, Ryan RM, Marsh AA. Neighborhood income and the expression of callous–unemotional traits. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;24:1103–18.CrossRefPubMed
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Barker ED, Salekin RT. Irritable oppositional defiance and callous unemotional traits: is the association partially explained by peer victimization? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:1167–75.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Barker ED, Salekin RT. Irritable oppositional defiance and callous unemotional traits: is the association partially explained by peer victimization? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:1167–75.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldweber A, Dmitrieva J, Cauffman E, Piquero AR, Steinberg L. The development of criminal style in adolescence and young adulthood: separating the lemmings from the loners. J Youth Adolesc. 2011;40:332–46.CrossRefPubMed Goldweber A, Dmitrieva J, Cauffman E, Piquero AR, Steinberg L. The development of criminal style in adolescence and young adulthood: separating the lemmings from the loners. J Youth Adolesc. 2011;40:332–46.CrossRefPubMed
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Kimonis ER, Frick PJ, Barry CT. Callous-unemotional traits and delinquent peer affiliation. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2004;72:956–66.CrossRefPubMed Kimonis ER, Frick PJ, Barry CT. Callous-unemotional traits and delinquent peer affiliation. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2004;72:956–66.CrossRefPubMed
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Kerr M, Van Zalk M, Stattin H. Psychopathic traits moderate peer influence on adolescent delinquency. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:826–35.CrossRefPubMed Kerr M, Van Zalk M, Stattin H. Psychopathic traits moderate peer influence on adolescent delinquency. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:826–35.CrossRefPubMed
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Waller R, Hyde LW. Callous-unemotional behaviors in early childhood: measurement, meaning, and the influence of parenting. Child Dev Perspect 2017; 0: 1-7. Waller R, Hyde LW. Callous-unemotional behaviors in early childhood: measurement, meaning, and the influence of parenting. Child Dev Perspect 2017; 0: 1-7.
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Waller R, Hyde LW. Callous-unemotional behaviors in early childhood: the development of empathy and prosociality gone awry. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;20:11–6.CrossRefPubMed Waller R, Hyde LW. Callous-unemotional behaviors in early childhood: the development of empathy and prosociality gone awry. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;20:11–6.CrossRefPubMed
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Reid MJ, Webster-Stratton C, Baydar N. Halting the development of conduct problems in head start children: the effects of parent training. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004;33:279–91.CrossRefPubMed Reid MJ, Webster-Stratton C, Baydar N. Halting the development of conduct problems in head start children: the effects of parent training. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004;33:279–91.CrossRefPubMed
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Waller R, Gardner F, Viding E, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN, et al. Bidirectional association between parental warmth, callous unemotional behavior and behavior problems in high-risk preschoolers. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42:1275–85.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Waller R, Gardner F, Viding E, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN, et al. Bidirectional association between parental warmth, callous unemotional behavior and behavior problems in high-risk preschoolers. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42:1275–85.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Dadds MR, Allen JL, McGregor K, Woolgar M, Viding E, Scott S. Callous-unemotional traits in children and mechanism of impaired eye contact during expression of love: a treatment target? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55:771–8.CrossRefPubMed Dadds MR, Allen JL, McGregor K, Woolgar M, Viding E, Scott S. Callous-unemotional traits in children and mechanism of impaired eye contact during expression of love: a treatment target? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55:771–8.CrossRefPubMed
84.
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Goffin KC, Boldt LJ, Kim S, Kochanska G. A unique path to callous-unemotional traits for children who are temperamentally fearless and unconcerned about transgressions: a longitudinal study of typically developing children from age 2 to 12. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2017; https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0317-2. Goffin KC, Boldt LJ, Kim S, Kochanska G. A unique path to callous-unemotional traits for children who are temperamentally fearless and unconcerned about transgressions: a longitudinal study of typically developing children from age 2 to 12. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2017; https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10802-017-0317-2.
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, Kimonis ER, Dandreaux DM, Farell JM. The 4 year stability of psychopathic traits in non-referred youth. Behav Sci Law. 2003;21:713–36.CrossRefPubMed Frick PJ, Kimonis ER, Dandreaux DM, Farell JM. The 4 year stability of psychopathic traits in non-referred youth. Behav Sci Law. 2003;21:713–36.CrossRefPubMed
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Obradović J, Pardini DA, Long JD, Loeber R. Measuring interpersonal callousness in boys from childhood to adolescence: an examination of longitudinal invariance and temporal stability. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2007;36:276–92.CrossRefPubMed Obradović J, Pardini DA, Long JD, Loeber R. Measuring interpersonal callousness in boys from childhood to adolescence: an examination of longitudinal invariance and temporal stability. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2007;36:276–92.CrossRefPubMed
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Klingzell I, Fanti KA, Colins OF, Frogner L, Andershed AK, Andershed H. Early childhood trajectories of conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits: the role of fearlessness and psychopathic personality dimensions. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2016;47:236–47.CrossRefPubMed Klingzell I, Fanti KA, Colins OF, Frogner L, Andershed AK, Andershed H. Early childhood trajectories of conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits: the role of fearlessness and psychopathic personality dimensions. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2016;47:236–47.CrossRefPubMed
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Loeber R, Burke J, Pardini DA. Perspectives on oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and psychopathic features. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50:133–42.CrossRefPubMed Loeber R, Burke J, Pardini DA. Perspectives on oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and psychopathic features. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50:133–42.CrossRefPubMed
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Bakker MJ, Greven CU, Buitelaar JK, Glennon JC. Practitioner review: psychological treatments for children and adolescents with conduct disorder problems - a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58:4–18.CrossRefPubMed Bakker MJ, Greven CU, Buitelaar JK, Glennon JC. Practitioner review: psychological treatments for children and adolescents with conduct disorder problems - a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58:4–18.CrossRefPubMed
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Hawes DJ, Price MJ, Dadds MR. Callous-unemotional traits and the treatment of conduct problems in childhood and adolescence: a comprehensive review. Clin Child Family Psychol Rev. 2014;17:248–67.CrossRef Hawes DJ, Price MJ, Dadds MR. Callous-unemotional traits and the treatment of conduct problems in childhood and adolescence: a comprehensive review. Clin Child Family Psychol Rev. 2014;17:248–67.CrossRef
92.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilkinson S, Waller R, Viding E. Practitioner review: involving young people with callous unemotional traits in treatment--does it work? A systematic review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57:552–65.CrossRefPubMed Wilkinson S, Waller R, Viding E. Practitioner review: involving young people with callous unemotional traits in treatment--does it work? A systematic review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57:552–65.CrossRefPubMed
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Datyner A, Kimonis ER, Hunt E, Armstrong K. Using a novel emotional skills module to enhance empathic responding for a child with conduct disorder with limited Prosocial emotions. Clin Case Stud. 2016;15:35–52.CrossRef Datyner A, Kimonis ER, Hunt E, Armstrong K. Using a novel emotional skills module to enhance empathic responding for a child with conduct disorder with limited Prosocial emotions. Clin Case Stud. 2016;15:35–52.CrossRef
94.
Zurück zum Zitat Dadds MR, Cauchi AJ, Wimalaweera S, Hawes DJ, Brennan J. Outcomes, moderators, and mediators of empathic-emotion recognition training for complex conduct problems in childhood. Psychiatry Res. 2012;199:201–7.CrossRefPubMed Dadds MR, Cauchi AJ, Wimalaweera S, Hawes DJ, Brennan J. Outcomes, moderators, and mediators of empathic-emotion recognition training for complex conduct problems in childhood. Psychiatry Res. 2012;199:201–7.CrossRefPubMed
95.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron-Cohen S, Golan O, Wheelwright S, Hill JJ. Mind reading: the interactive guide to emotions. London: Jessica Kingsley Limited; 2004. Baron-Cohen S, Golan O, Wheelwright S, Hill JJ. Mind reading: the interactive guide to emotions. London: Jessica Kingsley Limited; 2004.
96.
Zurück zum Zitat Salekin RT, Tippey JG, Allen AD. Treatment of conduct problem youth with interpersonal callous traits using mental models: measurement of risk and change. Behav Sci Law. 2012;30:470–86.CrossRefPubMed Salekin RT, Tippey JG, Allen AD. Treatment of conduct problem youth with interpersonal callous traits using mental models: measurement of risk and change. Behav Sci Law. 2012;30:470–86.CrossRefPubMed
97.
Zurück zum Zitat Lochman JE, Wells KC. Contextual social-cognitive mediators and child outcome: a test of the theoretical model in the coping power program. Dev Psychopathol. 2002;14:945–67.CrossRefPubMed Lochman JE, Wells KC. Contextual social-cognitive mediators and child outcome: a test of the theoretical model in the coping power program. Dev Psychopathol. 2002;14:945–67.CrossRefPubMed
98.
Zurück zum Zitat Muratori P, Milone A, Manfredi A, Polidori L, Ruglioni L, Lambruschi F, et al. Evaluation of improvement in externalizing behaviors and callous-unemotional traits in children with disruptive behavior disorder: a 1-year follow up clinic-based study. Admin Pol Ment Health. 2015; https://doi.org/10.1007/s10488-015-0660-y. Muratori P, Milone A, Manfredi A, Polidori L, Ruglioni L, Lambruschi F, et al. Evaluation of improvement in externalizing behaviors and callous-unemotional traits in children with disruptive behavior disorder: a 1-year follow up clinic-based study. Admin Pol Ment Health. 2015; https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10488-015-0660-y.
99.
Zurück zum Zitat Salekin RT, Worley C, Grimes RD. Treatment of psychopathy: a review and brief introduction to the mental model approach for psychopathy. Behav Sci Law. 2010;28:235–66.CrossRefPubMed Salekin RT, Worley C, Grimes RD. Treatment of psychopathy: a review and brief introduction to the mental model approach for psychopathy. Behav Sci Law. 2010;28:235–66.CrossRefPubMed
101.
Zurück zum Zitat Pisano S, Catone G, Veltri S, Lanzara V, Pozzi M, Clementi E, et al. Update on the safety of second generation antipsychotics in youths: a call for collaboration among paediatricians and child psychiatrists. Ital J Pediatr. 2016;42:51.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pisano S, Catone G, Veltri S, Lanzara V, Pozzi M, Clementi E, et al. Update on the safety of second generation antipsychotics in youths: a call for collaboration among paediatricians and child psychiatrists. Ital J Pediatr. 2016;42:51.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
102.
Zurück zum Zitat Pisano S, Gritti A, Catone G, Pascotto A. Antipsychotic-induced dyslipidemia treated with omega 3 fatty acid supplement in an 11-year-old psychotic child: a 1-year follow-up. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2013;23:139–41.CrossRefPubMed Pisano S, Gritti A, Catone G, Pascotto A. Antipsychotic-induced dyslipidemia treated with omega 3 fatty acid supplement in an 11-year-old psychotic child: a 1-year follow-up. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2013;23:139–41.CrossRefPubMed
103.
Zurück zum Zitat Masi G, Milone A, Canepa G, Millepiedi S, Mucci M, Muratori F. Olanzapine treatment in adolescents with severe conduct disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2006;21:51–7.CrossRefPubMed Masi G, Milone A, Canepa G, Millepiedi S, Mucci M, Muratori F. Olanzapine treatment in adolescents with severe conduct disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2006;21:51–7.CrossRefPubMed
104.
Zurück zum Zitat Masi G, Milone A, Manfredi A, Brovedani P, Pisano S, Muratori P. Combined pharmacotherapy-multimodal psychotherapy in children with disruptive behavior disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2016;238:8–13.CrossRefPubMed Masi G, Milone A, Manfredi A, Brovedani P, Pisano S, Muratori P. Combined pharmacotherapy-multimodal psychotherapy in children with disruptive behavior disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2016;238:8–13.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Conduct disorders and psychopathy in children and adolescents: aetiology, clinical presentation and treatment strategies of callous-unemotional traits
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2017
Erschienen in
Italian Journal of Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2017
Elektronische ISSN: 1824-7288
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-017-0404-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2017

Italian Journal of Pediatrics 1/2017 Zur Ausgabe

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.