Skip to main content
Erschienen in: EcoHealth 1/2021

04.06.2021 | COVID-19 | Original Contribution Zur Zeit gratis

Hesitancy Toward a COVID-19 Vaccine

verfasst von: Linda Thunström, Madison Ashworth, David Finnoff, Stephen C. Newbold

Erschienen in: EcoHealth | Ausgabe 1/2021

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Abstract

The scientific community has come together in a mass mobilization to combat the public health risks of COVID-19, including efforts to develop a vaccine. However, the success of any vaccine depends on the share of the population that gets vaccinated. We designed a survey experiment in which a nationally representative sample of 3,133 adults in the USA stated their intentions to vaccinate themselves and their children for COVID-19. The factors that we varied across treatments were: the stated severity and infectiousness of COVID-19 and the stated source of the risk information (White House or the Centers for Disease Control). We find that 20% of people in the USA intend to decline the vaccine. We find no statistically significant effect on vaccine intentions from the severity of COVID-19. In contrast, we find that the degree of infectiousness of the coronavirus influences vaccine intentions and that inconsistent risk messages from public health experts and elected officials may reduce vaccine uptake. However, the most important determinants of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy seem to be distrust of the vaccine safety (including uncertainty due to vaccine novelty), as well as general vaccine avoidance, as implied by not having had a flu shot in the last two years.
Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
The threshold for herd immunity in the population (i.e., the proportion of the population that needs to be immune, either from a vaccine, previous infections or both, to ensure herd immunity) is typically inferred from the basic reproduction number for COVID-19, R0. R0 estimates vary across multiple dimensions, such as data availability, geographical location and methods used to produce the estimates (Liu et al., 2020). In the beginning of the pandemic, Sanche et al. (2020) used data from outbreaks in China and estimated R0 to be as high as 5.7, implying herd immunity may be reached first when 82.5% of the population is immune (Keeling and Rohani, 2008). Most estimates, across countries and regions, range between 2.5 and 4 (Fontanet and Gauchemez, 2020). Based on that range, the classical formula for herd immunity (1–1/R0) generates a herd immunity threshold within 60 to 75 percent of the population being immune. However, local variations can be large. Sy et al. (2020) estimated R0 for COVID-19 at the county level within the U.S. and found estimates ranging from 0.4 to 12.4, with a median county level R0 of 1.66. Further, the classic formula for herd immunity may be too simplistic to generate a good approximation of the herd immunity threshold (Aguas et al., 2020).
 
2
Our study relates to a rich body of literature on consumer responses to conflicting information, spanning multiple scientific disciplines. While not an exhaustive list, examples of important work in this area are Viscusi and Magat (1992), Magat and Viscusi (1992), Viscusi (1997), Viscusi et al. (1999), Rodgers (1999), Fox et al. (2002), Hoehn and Randall (2002), Cameron (2005), Rousu and Shogren (2006), Kelly et al. (2012), Carpenter et al. (2014) Hämeen-Anttila et al. (2014), Pushkarskaya et al. (2015), and Binder et al. (2016).
 
3
For instance, the few early estimates of the IFR available around the time of data collection for this study (i.e., in March 2020) were based on non-US data, primarily from Asia (see the meta analysis by Meyerowitz-Katz and Merone, 2020), which may not be representative of the USA, given local differences in factors such as public health and healthcare access and quality.
 
4
We kept the disparity in risk communicated by the CDC and the White House constant (at 15 percentage points) across both high and low infection risk treatments. Viscusi (1997) shows that the disparity in the risks communicated by different information sources may affect trust in all information sources, such that a change in the disparity in percentage point probabilities across high and low infection risk treatments could affect trust in both the CDC and the White House.
 
5
While the IFR in our study is high, our data imply that both the 1.5% and 10% IFR indicated in our survey were viewed as plausible by most study participants. After presenting respondents with our chosen design levels for the IFR for the average American (i.e., 1.5% or 10%), participants were asked whether they believed their own risk was lower, about the same, or higher than the indicated level. In all treatments, 65–70% of participants stated that they either considered themselves to be at “about the same” or “higher” risk, and we find no statistically significant (or substantive) difference in those beliefs across respondents in the low- and high-mortality treatments.
 
6
Note that we asked participants to suppose that a vaccine that was available today, although we expected participants to understand that a vaccine was in fact not yet available. An alternative would have been to ask about intentions to vaccinate at a future point in time, when a vaccine is more likely to be available. Our choice is based on control over the study environment. Participants may differ in their beliefs about when a vaccine will be available and how the risks of infection and death may evolve over the course of the outbreak—they might expect the pandemic to have concluded before a vaccine is available, herd immunity to be near, or that they personally will already have been infected. The recent polls that have measured COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy (see discussion in Sect. 4) vary in how they have dealt with the timing of the vaccine when asking about the willingness to vaccinate. Like our study, the poll by Pew Research Center (2020) asks about vaccine intentions if the vaccine was available today, while the polls by ABC news/Ipsos (2020) and LX/Morning Consult (2020) ask about willingness to vaccinate when a vaccine becomes available without specifying when that might be.
 
7
All results are adjusted for multiple hypothesis testing using the Bonferroni correction (Bonferroni, 1935) with a family-wise type I error rate of 0.05. Adjusted p-values represent Bonferroni corrected p-values.
 
8
The results reported in Fig. 2 (a) remain highly similar even if we remove subjects stating 50 percent (see figure included in Supplemental online appendix).
 
9
The results from the probit regression are reported in Supplemental online appendix. It is the same model that generates the result in Fig. 4, but with the treatment variables only (i.e., only the top three variables in Fig. 3), given the isolation of the treatment effects relies on the randomization of participants across treatments. The observed treatment effects are very similar to those generated by the model in Fig. 4. For space saving reasons, we therefore refrain from including a separate figure in the main text for the probit regression that has the treatment variables only.
 
10
The particularly low vaccine uptake in the group that received the “low-probability (communicated by both the CDC and the White House) and high-mortality” treatment can be partially explained by our finding that the IFR for the average American (as communicated in our study) has little (if any) influence on participants’ decision to vaccinate, as shown in Fig. 4. The probability of infection and the mixed messages from the White House and the CDC have larger effects on vaccine uptake (as suggested by the results in Fig. 4). Therefore, we should expect the vaccine uptake in this treatment to be lower than that in other treatments. Another factor that could contribute to the low vaccine uptake in this group is differences in participant characteristics between this group and the rest of the sample. As discussed in Sect. 2, we found no “meaningful” differences (Imbens and Rubin, 2015) in participant characteristics and attitudes across any treatment groups. However, there are small differences between this group and the rest of the sample (statistically significant at conventional levels, but uncorrected for multiple hypotheses testing). Specifically, the share of women, share of participants with low trust in government agencies, and share of participants ascribing to neither Democrats nor Republicans are all higher in this group than all other groups in the sample. These differences are in the direction that would suggest a lower than average vaccine uptake for this group, as suggested by our analysis of vaccine uptake for our sample as a whole.
 
11
The dummy variables Vaccine confidence, Vaccine complacency, Vaccine calculation, Vaccine collective responsibility, and Vaccine constraint represent the five key components of the vaccine hesitancy scale developed by Betsch et al. (2018).
 
12
While we measured trust in government agencies as a continuous variable, it is included in the regression as a categorical variable. The reason for this is to avoid problematic multicollinearity. There is high bivariate correlation between the continuous trust measure and the vaccine confidence and the flu shot variables, respectively. If the continuous trust variable is included in the regression, trust (misleadingly) appears to have no effect on vaccine uptake. However, if either vaccine confidence or the flu shot variable is removed from the regression, the continuous trust variable is highly statistically significant. Binning the trust variable into low, medium, and high trust eliminates the problematic multicollinearity.
 
13
We do not know the causality for this relationship—while people might be less inclined to take a vaccine if they perceive the risk of the virus to be lower, it is also possible that people who are hesitant to vaccines are motivated to downplay the risk of the disease (for motivated risk beliefs, see, e.g., Kopetz and Woerner, 2021).
 
14
Also, the intended vaccine uptake for the treatments with low IFR only (n = 1,597) is 81%, i.e., about the same as the average vaccine uptake for the study as a whole.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Aguas, R., Corder, R. M., King, J. G., Goncalves, G., Ferreira, M. U., and Gomes, M. G. M. (2020). Herd immunity thresholds for SARS-CoV-2 estimated from unfolding epidemics. medRxiv. Aguas, R., Corder, R. M., King, J. G., Goncalves, G., Ferreira, M. U., and Gomes, M. G. M. (2020). Herd immunity thresholds for SARS-CoV-2 estimated from unfolding epidemics. medRxiv.
Zurück zum Zitat Alberini A, Ščasný M (2013) Exploring heterogeneity in the value of a statistical life: Cause of death v. risk perceptions. Ecological Economics 94:143–155CrossRef Alberini A, Ščasný M (2013) Exploring heterogeneity in the value of a statistical life: Cause of death v. risk perceptions. Ecological Economics 94:143–155CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat ASTHO. Communicating Effectively About Vaccines: Summary of a Survey of U.S. Parents and Guardians. Arlington, VA: Association of State and Territorial Health Officials, 2010. ASTHO. Communicating Effectively About Vaccines: Summary of a Survey of U.S. Parents and Guardians. Arlington, VA: Association of State and Territorial Health Officials, 2010.
Zurück zum Zitat Athey S, Imbens GW (2017) The econometrics of randomized experiments. In: Banerjee AV, Duflo E (eds) Handbook of Economic Field Experiments Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland, pp 73–140CrossRef Athey S, Imbens GW (2017) The econometrics of randomized experiments. In: Banerjee AV, Duflo E (eds) Handbook of Economic Field Experiments Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland, pp 73–140CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Betsch, C., Schmid, P., Heinemeier, D., Korn, L., Holtmann, C., and Böhm, R. (2018). Beyond confidence: Development of a measure assessing the 5C psychological antecedents of vaccination. PLoS One, 13(12). Betsch, C., Schmid, P., Heinemeier, D., Korn, L., Holtmann, C., and Böhm, R. (2018). Beyond confidence: Development of a measure assessing the 5C psychological antecedents of vaccination. PLoS One13(12).
Zurück zum Zitat Binder AR, Hillback ED, Brossard D (2016) Conflict or caveats? Effects of media portrayals of scientific uncertainty on audience perceptions of new technologies. Risk Analysis 36(4):831–846PubMedCrossRef Binder AR, Hillback ED, Brossard D (2016) Conflict or caveats? Effects of media portrayals of scientific uncertainty on audience perceptions of new technologies. Risk Analysis 36(4):831–846PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bonferroni CE (1935) Il calcolo delle assicurazioni su gruppi di teste. Rome: Tipografia del Senato Bonferroni CE (1935) Il calcolo delle assicurazioni su gruppi di teste. Rome: Tipografia del Senato
Zurück zum Zitat Breakwell GM (2000) Risk communication: factors affecting impact. British Medical Bulletin 56(1):110–120PubMedCrossRef Breakwell GM (2000) Risk communication: factors affecting impact. British Medical Bulletin 56(1):110–120PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brewer NT, Chapman GB, Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, McCaul KD, Weinstein ND (2007) Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: the example of vaccination. Health Psychology 26(2):136PubMedCrossRef Brewer NT, Chapman GB, Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, McCaul KD, Weinstein ND (2007) Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: the example of vaccination. Health Psychology 26(2):136PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bruine de Bruin W, Fischbeck PS, Stiber NA, Fischhoff B (2002) What number is “fifty-fifty”?: Redistributing excessive 50% responses in elicited probabilities. Risk Analysis: an International Journal 22(4):713–723CrossRef Bruine de Bruin W, Fischbeck PS, Stiber NA, Fischhoff B (2002) What number is “fifty-fifty”?: Redistributing excessive 50% responses in elicited probabilities. Risk Analysis: an International Journal 22(4):713–723CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Calman K, Curtis S (2010) Risk communication and public health. Oxford University Press Calman K, Curtis S (2010) Risk communication and public health. Oxford University Press
Zurück zum Zitat Cameron TA (2005) Updating subjective risks in the presence of conflicting information: an application to climate change. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 30:63–97CrossRef Cameron TA (2005) Updating subjective risks in the presence of conflicting information: an application to climate change. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 30:63–97CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Campos-Mercade , Meier A, Schneider F, Wengström E (2020) Prosociality predicts health behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic. University of Zurich, Department of Economics, Working Paper, (346) Campos-Mercade , Meier A, Schneider F, Wengström E (2020) Prosociality predicts health behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic. University of Zurich, Department of Economics, Working Paper, (346)
Zurück zum Zitat Carpenter DM, Elstad EA, Blalock SJ, DeVellis RF (2014) Conflicting medication information: prevalence, sources, and relationship to medication adherence. Journal of Health Communication 19(1):67–81PubMedCrossRef Carpenter DM, Elstad EA, Blalock SJ, DeVellis RF (2014) Conflicting medication information: prevalence, sources, and relationship to medication adherence. Journal of Health Communication 19(1):67–81PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cascella, M., Rajnik, M., Cuomo, A., Dulebohn, S. C., and Di Napoli, R. (2020). Features, evaluation and treatment coronavirus (COVID-19). In Statpearls [internet]. StatPearls Publishing. Cascella, M., Rajnik, M., Cuomo, A., Dulebohn, S. C., and Di Napoli, R. (2020). Features, evaluation and treatment coronavirus (COVID-19). In Statpearls [internet]. StatPearls Publishing.
Zurück zum Zitat Chandler J, Paolacci G (2017) Lie for a dime: When most prescreening responses are honest but most study participants are impostors. Social Psychological and Personality Science 8(5):500–508CrossRef Chandler J, Paolacci G (2017) Lie for a dime: When most prescreening responses are honest but most study participants are impostors. Social Psychological and Personality Science 8(5):500–508CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cross ML, Buddle BM, Aldwell FE (2007) The potential of oral vaccines for disease control in wildlife species. The Veterinary Journal 174:472–480PubMedCrossRef Cross ML, Buddle BM, Aldwell FE (2007) The potential of oral vaccines for disease control in wildlife species. The Veterinary Journal 174:472–480PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat De Serres G, Markowski F, Toth E, Landry M, Auger D, Mercier M, Bélanger P, Turmel B, Arruda H, Boulianne N, Ward BJ (2013) Largest measles epidemic in North America in a decade—Quebec, Canada, 2011: contribution of susceptibility, serendipity, and superspreading events. The Journal of Infectious Diseases 207(6):990–998PubMedCrossRef De Serres G, Markowski F, Toth E, Landry M, Auger D, Mercier M, Bélanger P, Turmel B, Arruda H, Boulianne N, Ward BJ (2013) Largest measles epidemic in North America in a decade—Quebec, Canada, 2011: contribution of susceptibility, serendipity, and superspreading events. The Journal of Infectious Diseases 207(6):990–998PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dubé E, Laberge C, Guay M, Bramadat P, Roy R, Bettinger JA (2013) Vaccine hesitancy: an overview. Human Vaccines & Immunotherapeutics 9(8):1763–1773CrossRef Dubé E, Laberge C, Guay M, Bramadat P, Roy R, Bettinger JA (2013) Vaccine hesitancy: an overview. Human Vaccines & Immunotherapeutics 9(8):1763–1773CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Edwards RD (2008) Health risk and portfolio choice. Journal of Business & Economic Statistics 26(4):472–485CrossRef Edwards RD (2008) Health risk and portfolio choice. Journal of Business & Economic Statistics 26(4):472–485CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Everett, J. A. (2013). The 12 item social and economic conservatism scale (SECS). PLOS ONE, 8(12). Everett, J. A. (2013). The 12 item social and economic conservatism scale (SECS). PLOS ONE8(12).
Zurück zum Zitat Ferguson, N. M., Laydon, D., Nedjati-Gilani, G., Imai, N., Ainslie, K., Baguelin, M., Bhatia, S., Boonyasiri, A., Cucunubá, Z., Cuomo-Dannenburg, G., Dighe, A., Dorigatti, I., Fu, H., Gaythorpe, K., Green, W., Hamlet, A., Hinsley, W., Okell, L. C., van Elsland, S., Thompson, H., Verity, R., Volz, E., Wang, H., Wang, Y., Walker, P. G. T., Walters, C., Winskill, P. Whittaker, C., Donnelly, A., Riley, S., Ghani, A. C. (2020) Report 9: Impact of non-pharmaceutical interventions (NPIs) to reduce COVID19 mortality and healthcare demand. Imperial College Response Team, http://hdl.handle.net/10044/1/77482. Ferguson, N. M., Laydon, D., Nedjati-Gilani, G., Imai, N., Ainslie, K., Baguelin, M., Bhatia, S., Boonyasiri, A., Cucunubá, Z., Cuomo-Dannenburg, G., Dighe, A., Dorigatti, I., Fu, H., Gaythorpe, K., Green, W., Hamlet, A., Hinsley, W., Okell, L. C., van Elsland, S., Thompson, H., Verity, R., Volz, E., Wang, H., Wang, Y., Walker, P. G. T., Walters, C., Winskill, P. Whittaker, C., Donnelly, A., Riley, S., Ghani, A. C. (2020) Report 9: Impact of non-pharmaceutical interventions (NPIs) to reduce COVID19 mortality and healthcare demand. Imperial College Response Team, http://​hdl.​handle.​net/​10044/​1/​77482.
Zurück zum Zitat Fine PEM, Clarkson JA (1986) Individual versus public priorities in the determination of optimal vaccination policies. American Journal of Epidemiology 124(6):1012–1020PubMedCrossRef Fine PEM, Clarkson JA (1986) Individual versus public priorities in the determination of optimal vaccination policies. American Journal of Epidemiology 124(6):1012–1020PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fine P, Eames K, Heymann DL (2011) “Herd immunity”: a rough guide. Clinical Infectious Diseases 52(7):911–916PubMedCrossRef Fine P, Eames K, Heymann DL (2011) “Herd immunity”: a rough guide. Clinical Infectious Diseases 52(7):911–916PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Finkelstein, A., Luttmer, E. F., and Notowidigdo, M. J. (2013). What good is wealth without health? The effect of health on the marginal utility of consumption. Journal of the European Economic Association, 11(suppl_1), 221–258. Finkelstein, A., Luttmer, E. F., and Notowidigdo, M. J. (2013). What good is wealth without health? The effect of health on the marginal utility of consumption. Journal of the European Economic Association11(suppl_1), 221–258.
Zurück zum Zitat Fox JA, Hayes DJ, Shogren JF (2002) Consumer preferences for food irradiation: how favorable and unfavorable descriptions affect preferences for irradiated pork in experimental auctions. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 24(1):75–95CrossRef Fox JA, Hayes DJ, Shogren JF (2002) Consumer preferences for food irradiation: how favorable and unfavorable descriptions affect preferences for irradiated pork in experimental auctions. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 24(1):75–95CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Freed GL, Clark SJ, Butchart AT, Singer DC, Davis MM (2011) Sources and perceived credibility of vaccine-safety information for parents. Pediatrics 127(Supplement 1):S107–S112PubMedCrossRef Freed GL, Clark SJ, Butchart AT, Singer DC, Davis MM (2011) Sources and perceived credibility of vaccine-safety information for parents. Pediatrics 127(Supplement 1):S107–S112PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Frewer LJ, Howard C, Hedderley D, Shepherd R (1996) What determines trust in information about food-related risks? Underlying Psychological Constructs. Risk Analysis 16(4):473–486PubMedCrossRef Frewer LJ, Howard C, Hedderley D, Shepherd R (1996) What determines trust in information about food-related risks? Underlying Psychological Constructs. Risk Analysis 16(4):473–486PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gerking S, Adamowicz W, Dickie M, Veronesi M (2017) Baseline risk and marginal willingness to pay for health risk reduction. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 55(2–3):177–202PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Gerking S, Adamowicz W, Dickie M, Veronesi M (2017) Baseline risk and marginal willingness to pay for health risk reduction. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 55(2–3):177–202PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hammitt JK, Haninger K (2010) Valuing fatal risks to children and adults: Effects of disease, latency, and risk aversion. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 40(1):57–83CrossRef Hammitt JK, Haninger K (2010) Valuing fatal risks to children and adults: Effects of disease, latency, and risk aversion. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 40(1):57–83CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hoehn JP, Randall A (2002) The effect of resource quality information on resource injury perceptions and contingent values. Resource and Energy Economics 24(1–2):13–31CrossRef Hoehn JP, Randall A (2002) The effect of resource quality information on resource injury perceptions and contingent values. Resource and Energy Economics 24(1–2):13–31CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Imbens GW, Rubin DB (2015) Causal inference in statistics, social, and biomedical sciences. Cambridge University PressCrossRef Imbens GW, Rubin DB (2015) Causal inference in statistics, social, and biomedical sciences. Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jung SM, Akhmetzhanov AR, Hayashi K, Linton NM, Yang Y, Yuan B, Kobayashi T, Kinoshita R, Nishiura H (2020) Real-time estimation of the risk of death from novel coronavirus (COVID-19) infection: inference using exported cases. Journal of Clinical Medicine 9(2):523PubMedCentralCrossRef Jung SM, Akhmetzhanov AR, Hayashi K, Linton NM, Yang Y, Yuan B, Kobayashi T, Kinoshita R, Nishiura H (2020) Real-time estimation of the risk of death from novel coronavirus (COVID-19) infection: inference using exported cases. Journal of Clinical Medicine 9(2):523PubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Keeling MJ, Rohani P (2008) Modeling infectious diseases in humans and animals. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University PressCrossRef Keeling MJ, Rohani P (2008) Modeling infectious diseases in humans and animals. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University PressCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kelly DL, Letson D, Nelson F, Nolan DS, Solís D (2012) Evolution of subjective hurricane risk perceptions: A Bayesian approach. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 81(2):644–663CrossRef Kelly DL, Letson D, Nelson F, Nolan DS, Solís D (2012) Evolution of subjective hurricane risk perceptions: A Bayesian approach. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 81(2):644–663CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kopetz C, Woerner JI (2021) People Downplay Health Risks to Fulfill Their Goals: A Motivational Framework for Guiding Behavioral Policy. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8(1):92–100CrossRef Kopetz C, Woerner JI (2021) People Downplay Health Risks to Fulfill Their Goals: A Motivational Framework for Guiding Behavioral Policy. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8(1):92–100CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Larson HJ (2018) The state of vaccine confidence. The Lancet 392(10161):2244–2246CrossRef Larson HJ (2018) The state of vaccine confidence. The Lancet 392(10161):2244–2246CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lee C, Whetten K, Omer S, Pan W, Salmon D (2016) Hurdles to herd immunity: Distrust of government and vaccine refusal in the US, 2002–2003. Vaccine 34(34):3972–3978PubMedCrossRef Lee C, Whetten K, Omer S, Pan W, Salmon D (2016) Hurdles to herd immunity: Distrust of government and vaccine refusal in the US, 2002–2003. Vaccine 34(34):3972–3978PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat MacDonald NE (2015) Vaccine hesitancy: Definition, scope and determinants. Vaccine 33(34):4161–4164PubMedCrossRef MacDonald NE (2015) Vaccine hesitancy: Definition, scope and determinants. Vaccine 33(34):4161–4164PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Magat, W. A. and Viscusi, W. K. (1992). Informational approaches to regulation (Vol. 19). MIT press. Magat, W. A. and Viscusi, W. K. (1992). Informational approaches to regulation (Vol. 19). MIT press.
Zurück zum Zitat Meyerowitz-Katz, G., and Merone, L. (2020). A systematic review and meta-analysis of published research data on COVID-19 infection-fatality rates. International Journal of Infectious Diseases, 101, 138–148. Mochiri, N. (2020) Coronavirus seems to mutate much slower than seasonal flu. https://www.livescience.com/coronavirus-mutation-rate.html. Retrieved April 28 2020. Meyerowitz-Katz, G., and Merone, L. (2020). A systematic review and meta-analysis of published research data on COVID-19 infection-fatality rates. International Journal of Infectious Diseases, 101, 138–148. Mochiri, N. (2020) Coronavirus seems to mutate much slower than seasonal flu. https://​www.​livescience.​com/​coronavirus-mutation-rate.​html. Retrieved April 28 2020.
Zurück zum Zitat Mutz DC, Pemantle R, Pham P (2017) The perils of balance testing in experimental design: messy analyses of clean data. The American Statistician 73(1):32–42CrossRef Mutz DC, Pemantle R, Pham P (2017) The perils of balance testing in experimental design: messy analyses of clean data. The American Statistician 73(1):32–42CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Olive, J. K., Hotez, P. J., Damania, A., and Nolan, M. S. (2018). The state of the antivaccine movement in the United States: A focused examination of nonmedical exemptions in states and counties. PLoS Medicine, 15(6). Olive, J. K., Hotez, P. J., Damania, A., and Nolan, M. S. (2018). The state of the antivaccine movement in the United States: A focused examination of nonmedical exemptions in states and counties. PLoS Medicine15(6).
Zurück zum Zitat Onder G, Rezza G, Brusaferro S (2020) Case-fatality rate and characteristics of patients dying in relation to COVID-19 in Italy. JAMA 323(18):1775–1776PubMed Onder G, Rezza G, Brusaferro S (2020) Case-fatality rate and characteristics of patients dying in relation to COVID-19 in Italy. JAMA 323(18):1775–1776PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Rodgers W (1999) The influences of conflicting information on novices and loan officers’ actions. Journal of Economic Psychology 20(2):123–145CrossRef Rodgers W (1999) The influences of conflicting information on novices and loan officers’ actions. Journal of Economic Psychology 20(2):123–145CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rousu, M. C., and Shogren, J. F. (2006). Valuing conflicting public information about a new technology: the case of irradiated foods. Journal of Agricultural and Resource Economics, 642–652. Rousu, M. C., and Shogren, J. F. (2006). Valuing conflicting public information about a new technology: the case of irradiated foods. Journal of Agricultural and Resource Economics, 642–652.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanche S., Lin, Y.T., Xu, C., Romero-Severson, E., Hengartner, N., and Ke, R. (2020). High contagiousness and rapid spread of severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2. Emerging Infectious Disease. 2020 Jul [11 April 2020]. https://doi.org/10.3201/eid2607.200282 Sanche S., Lin, Y.T., Xu, C., Romero-Severson, E., Hengartner, N., and Ke, R. (2020). High contagiousness and rapid spread of severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2. Emerging Infectious Disease. 2020 Jul [11 April 2020]. https://​doi.​org/​10.​3201/​eid2607.​200282
Zurück zum Zitat Sarkar S, Zlojutro A, Khan K, Gardner L (2019) Measles resurgence in the USA: how international travel compounds vaccine resistance. The Lancet Infectious Diseases 19(7):684–686PubMedCrossRef Sarkar S, Zlojutro A, Khan K, Gardner L (2019) Measles resurgence in the USA: how international travel compounds vaccine resistance. The Lancet Infectious Diseases 19(7):684–686PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sharpe Wessling K, Huber J, Netzer O (2017) MTurk character misrepresentation: Assessment and solutions. Journal of Consumer Research 44(1):211–230CrossRef Sharpe Wessling K, Huber J, Netzer O (2017) MTurk character misrepresentation: Assessment and solutions. Journal of Consumer Research 44(1):211–230CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Shereen, M. A., Khan, S., Kazmi, A., Bashir, N., and Siddique, R. (2020). COVID-19 infection: origin, transmission, and characteristics of human coronaviruses. Journal of Advanced Research. Shereen, M. A., Khan, S., Kazmi, A., Bashir, N., and Siddique, R. (2020). COVID-19 infection: origin, transmission, and characteristics of human coronaviruses. Journal of Advanced Research.
Zurück zum Zitat Siegrist M, Cvetkovich G, Roth C (2000) Salient value similarity, social trust, and risk=benefit perception. Risk Analysis 20(3):353–362PubMedCrossRef Siegrist M, Cvetkovich G, Roth C (2000) Salient value similarity, social trust, and risk=benefit perception. Risk Analysis 20(3):353–362PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Siegrist M, Cvetkovich GT, Gutscher H (2001) Shared values, social trust, and the perception of geographic cancer clusters. Risk Analysis 21(6):1047–1053PubMedCrossRef Siegrist M, Cvetkovich GT, Gutscher H (2001) Shared values, social trust, and the perception of geographic cancer clusters. Risk Analysis 21(6):1047–1053PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Simonov, A., Sacher, S. K., Dubé, J. P. H., and Biswas, S. (2020). The persuasive effect of fox news: non-compliance with social distancing during the covid-19 pandemic (No. w27237). National Bureau of Economic Research. Simonov, A., Sacher, S. K., Dubé, J. P. H., and Biswas, S. (2020). The persuasive effect of fox news: non-compliance with social distancing during the covid-19 pandemic (No. w27237). National Bureau of Economic Research.
Zurück zum Zitat Sloan FA, Viscusi WK, Chesson HW, Conover CJ, Whetten-Goldstein K (1998) Alternative approaches to valuing intangible health losses: the evidence for multiple sclerosis. Journal of Health Economics 17(4):475–497PubMedCrossRef Sloan FA, Viscusi WK, Chesson HW, Conover CJ, Whetten-Goldstein K (1998) Alternative approaches to valuing intangible health losses: the evidence for multiple sclerosis. Journal of Health Economics 17(4):475–497PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Smith VK, Desvousges WH (1987) An empirical analysis of the economic value of risk changes. Journal of Political Economy 95(1):89–114CrossRef Smith VK, Desvousges WH (1987) An empirical analysis of the economic value of risk changes. Journal of Political Economy 95(1):89–114CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Viscusi WK (1997) Alarmist decisions with divergent risk information. The Economic Journal 107(445):1657–1670CrossRef Viscusi WK (1997) Alarmist decisions with divergent risk information. The Economic Journal 107(445):1657–1670CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Viscusi WK, Aldy JE (2003) The value of a statistical life: a critical review of market estimates throughout the world. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 27(1):5–76CrossRef Viscusi WK, Aldy JE (2003) The value of a statistical life: a critical review of market estimates throughout the world. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 27(1):5–76CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Viscusi, W. K., and Evans, W. N. (1990). Utility functions that depend on health status: estimates and economic implications. The American Economic Review, 353–374. Viscusi, W. K., and Evans, W. N. (1990). Utility functions that depend on health status: estimates and economic implications. The American Economic Review, 353–374.
Zurück zum Zitat Viscusi WK, Magat WA (1992) Bayesian decisions with ambiguous belief aversion. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 5(4):371–387CrossRef Viscusi WK, Magat WA (1992) Bayesian decisions with ambiguous belief aversion. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 5(4):371–387CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Viscusi WK, Magat WA, Huber J (1999) Smoking status and public responses to ambiguous scientific risk evidence. Southern Economic Journal 66(2):250–270 Viscusi WK, Magat WA, Huber J (1999) Smoking status and public responses to ambiguous scientific risk evidence. Southern Economic Journal 66(2):250–270
Metadaten
Titel
Hesitancy Toward a COVID-19 Vaccine
verfasst von
Linda Thunström
Madison Ashworth
David Finnoff
Stephen C. Newbold
Publikationsdatum
04.06.2021
Verlag
Springer US
Schlagwort
COVID-19
Erschienen in
EcoHealth / Ausgabe 1/2021
Print ISSN: 1612-9202
Elektronische ISSN: 1612-9210
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10393-021-01524-0

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

EcoHealth 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe