Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Brain Topography 1/2018

Open Access 18.01.2017 | Original Paper

Differences in Cortical Sources of the Event-Related P3 Potential Between Young and Old Participants Indicate Frontal Compensation

verfasst von: R. van Dinteren, R. J. Huster, M. L. A. Jongsma, R. P. C. Kessels, M. Arns

Erschienen in: Brain Topography | Ausgabe 1/2018

Abstract

The event-related P3 potential, as elicited in auditory signal detection tasks, originates from neural activity of multiple cortical structures and presumably reflects an overlap of several cognitive processes. The fact that the P3 is affected by aging makes it a potential metric for age-related cognitive change. The P3 in older participants is thought to encompass frontal compensatory activity in addition to task-related processes. The current study investigates this by decomposing the P3 using group independent component analysis (ICA). Independent components (IC) of young and old participants were compared in order to investigate the effects of aging. Exact low-resolution tomography analysis (eLORETA) was used to compare current source densities between young and old participants for the P3-ICs to localize differences in cortical source activity for every IC. One of the P3-related ICs reflected a different constellation of cortical generators in older participants compared to younger participants, suggesting that this P3-IC reflects shifts in neural activations and compensatory processes with aging. This P3-IC was localized to the orbitofrontal/temporal, and the medio-parietal regions. For this IC, older participants showed more frontal activation and less parietal activation as measured on the scalp. The differences in cortical sources were localized in the precentral gyrus and the parahippocampal gyrus. This finding might reflect compensatory activity recruited from these cortical sources during a signal detection task.
Hinweise
This is one of several papers published together in Brain Topography on the “Special Issue: Multisubject decomposition of EEG-methods and applications”.

Introduction

The P3 (or P300) is a positive-going event-related potential recorded in the electroencephalogram that peaks at approximately 300 milliseconds after stimulus presentation (Sutton et al. 1965). The P3 that is elicited in an auditory signal detection task, i.e. the oddball paradigm (Ritter and Vaughan 1969), presumably reflects overlapping activity of several cognitive processes. It is generated by neural activity of multiple cortical structures (Friedman 2003).

The Aging P3

The multifariousness of the P3 and the fact that it is affected by age-related changes in cognitive capacities, suggests it as a sensitive metric for cognitive performance (Van Dinteren et al. 2014b; Polich 1996). In an earlier study the developmental trajectories of the P3 amplitude across the lifespan were compared for frontal and parietal electrode sites. It was demonstrated that age-related changes were remarkably different between these locations. The parietal P3 amplitude increased in childhood to reach its peak in adolescence, then declined for the rest of the lifespan. In contrast, the frontal P3 amplitude reached its peak at a much older age, 46 years, after which it remained constant for the rest of the lifespan. It was concluded that the P3 wave reflects different mixes of cortical activation depending on the location of measurement and the age of the participant, among other probable factors. The frontal P3 amplitude might reflect compensatory activity from frontal regions that becomes more prominent as people age (Van Dinteren et al. 2014a).
This compensatory activity is possibly originating from the prefrontal cortex (PFC) that is related to higher order cognitive processes involved in regulating attention, working memory, and problem solving. These processes can be lateralized and it has been demonstrated that in older participants this lateralization reduces (this model is referred to as HAROLD, Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Old Adults (Cabeza 2002)). This phenomenon might reflect changes in cognitive strategies used by older adults and/or neuroplastic reorganization in the aging brain (Cabeza et al. 2002). Similarly, the Compensation-related Utilization of Neural Circuits Hypothesis (CRUNCH) by Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell (2008) posits that the aging brain, facing a decline in cognitive performance, can compensate to some degree by increasing activity in alternative connected neural networks (Daffner et al. 2011; Cappell et al. 2010). The P3 potential has a relatively long duration and consists of multiple overlapping slow potentials that reflect the several cognitive processes that are devoted to the task at hand. In line with this thought, there are many neural generators identified for the P3, e.g. the PFC, the temporo-parietal junction, primary auditory cortex, and other sources (Friedman 2003). The P3 might be exceptionally sensitive to pick up subtle changes in the relative contribution of the different cognitive processes that give rise to it. Therefore, it is a likely candidate for capturing compensatory activity that is increasingly allocated as people age. A simple comparison of P3 potentials between older and younger adults would be a first step. However, one has to extract and separate the various underlying sources of information that altogether give rise to the P3 potential in order to compare both age groups thoroughly.
Besides the often reported anterior shift in topography of the P3 in aging (Li et al. 2013; West et al. 2010; Friedman et al. 1997; O’Connell et al. 2012), Frodl et al. (2000) observed two subcomponents of the P3 by dipole source analysis that were differently affected by aging. The temporo-parietal P3 subcomponent was smaller in older adults whereas the frontal P3 subcomponent remained unchanged. These findings seem to explain the noted topographical change of the P3 with aging. Furthermore, there are reports of differences between younger and older adults in activation of the cortical regions that contribute to the P3 wave. For instance, it has been reported that P3s of older adults consist of more activation in prefrontal (O’Connell et al. 2012) and temporal regions compared to younger adults (Tsolaki et al. 2015; O’Connell et al. 2012).

The Decomposed P3

Because of the spatio-temporal overlap of activity from different neural sources inherent to multichannel scalp EEG recordings, methods for data decomposition have been brought forward to recover and separate the underlying source activity patterns. Independent component analysis (ICA) or principal component analysis (PCA), as applied to data of single as well as multiple subjects, have become prominent tools to recover neural source patterns from various measurement modalities (e.g., Eichele et al. 2011; Huster et al. 2015). ICA achieves this by decomposing the original recordings such that the resulting sources (usually referred to as components) exhibit maximal statistical independence; PCA, on the other hand, results in merely uncorrelated component activity patterns. PCA for EEG has a somewhat longer tradition, whereas studies applying ICA for the dissociation of P3 subcomponents are still somewhat scarce. Nonetheless, multiple studies have found independent components of this ERP using PCA and ICA that are physiologically plausible. Debener et al. (2005) decomposed concatenated single-trial ERP data from a novelty oddball paradigm and found two independent component clusters that respectively accounted for the novelty P3 and P3b responses. Brown et al. (2015) found a similar dissociation using PCA, and further reported a differential susceptibility of the two P3 subcomponents to drug exposure. Others also reported a dissociation of the frontal and parietal P3s from the succeeding slow wave activity that often appears as prolonged activity associated with the P3 peak (e.g., Steiner et al. 2013; Spencer et al. 2001). Makeig et al. (1999) identified four ICs in the ERP corresponding to a frontoparietal potential, a longer-latency parietal potential, a post-motor potential, and a left frontocentral potential.

Aim

This study aims at unraveling aging effects on cognitive processes that are involved in signal detection tasks by identifying the independent sources that contribute to the P3 wave as observed in the recorded ERP. ICs derived from the complete sample of healthy participants will be reconstructed into single-subject EEG responses for each component. The sample is split in two age-groups, younger participants aged 18–46 years, and older participants, aged 46–82 years. The acquired single-trial EEGs will be compared between younger and older individuals in the sample. Based on our earlier work (Van Dinteren et al. 2014b), it is expected that one or more P3-ICs will emerge that differ between age groups, potentially reflecting compensatory brain activation (i.e. ICs with a more anterior topography). Further P3-ICs are expected that do not differ between age-groups (i.e. ICs with a more posterior topography) and rather reflect general task-related activation. Furthermore, exact low-resolution tomography analysis (eLORETA) (Pascual-Marqui 2007) is used to compare current source densities between younger and older participants for the P3-ICs to localize differences in cortical activity for every IC. The differences in cortical activity are expected to mainly originate from anterior regions, reflecting compensatory frontal activation in older participants.

Methods

Participants

The sample consisted of 99 healthy participants, aged 18 to 82 years, and all recruited and tested at the Research Institute Brainclinics (Nijmegen, The Netherlands). All participants voluntarily gave written informed consent. These data are a subset of the data reported in previous papers (Van Dinteren et al. 2014a, b, 2015). Exclusion criteria were a personal or family history of psychiatric disorders, neurological disorders, brain injuries, addiction or any other serious medical condition. Participants were required to refrain from caffeine, alcohol and nicotine for at least 2 hours prior to electroencephalographic data acquisition. The data collection and study protocol was approved by the local IRB (CMO Region Arnhem Nijmegen; #2002/008).
The sample was divided in a young and an old group with a cut-off at 46 years (Van Dinteren et al. 2014a). Sample demographics are shown in Table 1. The percentage of male/female participants did not significantly differ for the age-groups (Χ 2(1,99) = 0.529, p = .30). Additionally, post-hoc analysis of differences in global field power (GFP) by sex and by age-group revealed no significant differences at any frame in the degree of global cortical activation as reflected in scalp electrode recordings. GFP is a single measure that is developed by Lehmann and Skandries (1980) that is computed as the mean of all absolute potential differences in the scalp field corresponding to a spatial standard deviation, see Skrandies (1990) for more details.
Table 1
Demographics of the used sample
 
N
Mean age (SD)
Males/females
Younger
50
23 (3.5)
19/30
Older
49
59 (9.6)
23/27
Total
99
41 (19.5)
42/57

Experimental Paradigm

As part of a large international database study, participants completed a test battery containing both EEG and behavioral tasks. A description of the full test battery can be found in Williams et al. (2011). The paradigm with the main interest was the oddball paradigm. The oddball paradigm consisted of a quasi-random sequence of 280 frequent background tones (500 Hz) and 60 infrequent target tones (1000 Hz). No two targets could appear consecutively in trials. All stimuli (50 ms; 5 ms rise and fall time) were presented binaurally (via headphones) at a volume of 75dB SPL with an inter-stimulus interval of 1000 ms. Participants were instructed to press two buttons simultaneously (one for each index finger to counterbalance motor effects) when they heard a target tone and to ignore the background tones. Speed and accuracy of responses were both equally stressed in the instructions. Before the actual test, participants were presented with a brief practice run to clarify the distinction between the two tones.
We further analyzed reaction time (RT) data obtained within a continuous performance test (CPT) in which participants were required to press two buttons simultaneously when a letter on the screen was presented twice in a row in a series of 125 letters (B, C, D or G). The inter-stimulus interval was 2500 ms. Additionally, a go/no go paradigm was presented in which participants were presented with the word “push” in either red or green letters. They were required to push two buttons simultaneously when the word was presented in green letters and to withhold from pushing the buttons when the word was presented in red letters. A total of 168 stimuli was presented within the task with an inter-stimulus interval of 2000 ms.

Electroencephalographic Data Acquisition

EEG acquisition was performed using a standardized methodology and platform (Brain Resource Ltd., Australia). Participants were seated in a sound and light-attenuated room, controlled at an ambient temperature of 22 °C. EEG data were acquired from 26 channels: Fp1, Fp2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, FC3, FCz, FC4, T3, C3, Cz, C4, T4, CP3, CPz, CP4, T5, P3, Pz, P4, T6, O1, Oz and O2 (Compumedics Quick-Cap and NuAmps amplifier; 10–20 electrode international system) with a ground at AFz. Data were recorded reference-free and offline referenced to averaged mastoids. Horizontal eye movements were recorded with electrodes placed 1.5 cm lateral to the outer canthus of each eye (bipolar). Vertical eye movements were recorded with electrodes placed 3 mm above the middle of the left eyebrow and 1.5 cm below the middle of the left bottom eyelid. Skin resistance was <5 kOhms for all electrodes. A continuous acquisition was employed at a sampling rate of 500 Hz at all channels. A high cut-off filter at 100 Hz was employed prior to digitization.

ERP Scoring

Conventional ERP averages were computed relative to the target and background stimuli for all EEG channels per participant using Brain Vision Analyzer (Brainproducts, Germany). Only segments with a correct target response were included in the target average. Before averaging, the EEG epochs were filtered with a high-pass IIR filter of 0.16 Hz (12 dB/Oct) and a low-pass filter of 40 Hz (24 dB/Oct). Vertical and horizontal ocular correction was applied according to the Gratton algorithm (Gratton et al. 1983). The segments ranged from −200 to 800 ms around stimulus presentation. Segments were DC detrended and baseline corrected for a pre-stimulus interval of −200 to 0 ms. Segments were considered to contain artifacts when there was a difference of 20 μV or more between two subsequent data points, when the difference between the highest and lowest voltage within a 100 ms epoch exceeded 150 μV, when the maximum or minimum amplitude respectively exceeded 100 or −100 μV within −200 to 300 ms, or when activity fluctuations were below 0.5 μV in a 50 ms time period. Rejected segments were not included in the calculation of the target average. There were 55 rejected target segments in the complete sample.

Independent Component Extraction

To decompose the P3 into its constituent components, a group ICA on the multi-subject EEG data was set up (e.g., Eichele et al. 2011; Huster et al. 2015; Bridwell et al. 2014). Hence, after preprocessing of the EEG an adjusted number of background and target trials was randomly selected from each subject’s available data. Note that the number of trials has to be the same for each subject in order to compute a group ICA. The number of background trials for further processing was restricted such that the average noise level of the condition-specific single-subject ERPs, computed as root mean squares of the baseline periods, did not differ between the background and target conditions. This procedure was implemented not to bias the group ICA towards background-related brain responses. These constraints led to a selection of 250 trials (200 background and 50 target trials) from each of the 99 participants that entered the analysis.
Group ICA extracts statistically independent components consistently expressed across participants. In short, each single-subject dataset first undergoes an individual principal component analysis (PCA), thereby extracting most relevant and orthogonal time courses per subject. These first-level principal components then are used as variables in a second, group-level PCA, now estimating most-relevant and orthogonal principal component time courses capturing activity patterns correlated across subjects. These undergo ICA that finally computes the statistically independent component time courses. Importantly, this nested procedure is capable of capturing some of the topographical variability we usually see with EEG events found in each of the single-subject datasets of a multi-subject study. Please refer to Eichele et al. (2011) and Huster et al. (2015), for a more detailed description of the algorithm.
A total of ten independent components were extracted using group ICA, because two procedures suggested this to correspond to the intrinsic dimensionality of the data. First, the inspection of the single-subject PCAs revealed that on average ten components explained about 95% of the variance of the scalp EEG data. Second, the ICASSO software package was used to assess the reliability and stability of the ICA solutions of 100 runs (Himberg et al. 2004). This combination of group ICA and ICASSO was used for evaluating models with 9, 10, and 11 components, and the ten-component solution revealed sufficient reliability and stability. Correspondingly, at each level of this analysis, including single-subject PCAs, group PCA, and group ICA, ten components were extracted. The ten resulting group components are characterized by their topographies and time courses, with estimations aiming at a maximal statistical independence of the latter, and naturally lead to a characterization of the latent data structure at the group level.
Four of the ten ICs were selected for further and more detailed analyses based on their resemblance with the P3. This selection was made after inspection of the topographies and time courses of each component, and reflects the research question directed at the study of the differential frontalization of the P3 in young and old adults. To statistically analyze the differences between younger and older participants and to assess the brain sources underlying the selected independent components, the scalp EEG corresponding to each component was reconstructed for every single subject. Hence, for every subject the matrices estimated during the data reduction and group ICA steps were extracted and applied to the single-subject EEG. By means of these individual demixing matrices, the subject-specific component time courses and topographies were reconstructed that directly relate to the group-level components. Then, the subject-specific EEG for each component was reconstructed by multiplying each single component time course with its corresponding coefficients of the subject-specific mixing matrix. The subject-specific EEG patterns corresponding to certain independent components formed the basis for our statistical analyses. Group independent components as well as the subject-specific mixing and demixing matrices were computed using self-written MATLAB scripts that followed the procedures described in Eichele et al. (2011). In addition, EEGLAB’s implementation of Infomax ICA in runica() was used.

Inverse Modeling

The subject-specific EEG/ERPs were reconstructed for every independent component and then compared between younger and older subjects. The data was entered in the eLORETA software (Pascual-Marqui 2002; Pascual-Marqui et al. 1994) that analyzes GFP of both groups at every time frame and produces a t-value for each time frame. Those time frames where the t-value exceeds the significance level (p < .01) comprise the ERP segments where the groups differ significantly from each other in GFP. These time frames were used in a spatial analysis. The software corrects for multiple testing by means of a randomization procedure.
The validated (Olbrich et al. 2009) eLORETA software yields images of current source density differences across the cortex. Current source density is calculated by squaring the weighed sum of scalp potentials for each voxel (A/m2). The computations are made in a realistic head model (Fuchs et al. 2002), using the MNI152 template (Mazziotta et al. 2001) with standard electrode positions displayed on the scalp (Jurcak et al. 2007; Oostenveld and Praamstra 2001). The three-dimensional solution space is restricted to cortical gray matter, as determined by the probabilistic Talairach atlas (Lancaster et al. 2000).
The eLORETA software solves the inverse problem under the assumption that the smoothest of all possible solutions (meaning that neighboring voxels should demonstrate maximally similar activation) is the most plausible one. This comes at the cost of low spatial resolution. Averaged current source density values for the two age groups were log-transformed and statistically compared. The software compares the groups on a voxel basis and identifies those voxels with t-values that exceed a significance level.

Behavioral Analysis

Post-hoc, relationships between independent components and behavioral data from the oddball, CPT and go/no go paradigms were investigated by correlational analysis. To do so, peak amplitudes of the reconstructed independent component ERPs correlations with reaction times in the three paradigms were statistically assessed. A Bonferroni corrected alpha of 0.02 (initial alpha level of 0.05, three tests) was employed.

Results

ERP Time Frames

In Fig. 1 the grand average ERPs for younger and older participants at midline sites are shown. Figure 2 shows the head maps corresponding to the 200–520 ms post-stimulus time interval. The time frames when the difference in global field power between averaged group ERPs exceeded the significance level were at 54–74 and 182–580 ms (t > 4.953, p < .01).

Independent Components

ICs are ordered according to their contribution to the variance of the EEG signal. Figures 3 and 4 depict the first four ICs for the complete sample. Using eLORETA the current source densities were calculated for localization purposes. IC1 was localized to an orbitofrontal/temporal as well as a medioparietal region; IC2 was localized to the precuneus/cingulate gyrus area; IC3 was localized to an area around the precuneus of the right parietal lobe; IC4 was localized to the posterior cingulate gyrus, see Fig. 4. Based on the ERP latency of IC4, this component seems to be related to earlier processing stages and is less likely related to the later higher order cognitive processes that the P3 represents.

IC Group Comparisons

The ICs were subjected to a time-domain analysis using t tests to compare the two groups. For IC1, groups differed significantly from each other at 276–492 ms post-stimulus (t > 4.282, p < .01). For IC2, the groups differed significantly from each other at 208–250 ms post-stimulus (t > 4.266, p < .01). For IC3, the groups differed significantly from each other at 238–316 ms post-stimulus (t > 4.190, p < .01). Finally, for IC4, the groups differed significantly at 58–70 ms post-stimulus (t > 4.408, p < .01).
To test where in the brain these differences would be located, the specified time frames were segmented into 20 ms epochs in order to reduce the number of tests that were run per analysis. For every IC separately, segments were analyzed for group differences after source localization and by using voxel-wise t tests. Note that corrections for multiple comparisons are done by the eLORETA software. For IC1, older participants had significantly more activation in the precentral gyrus and the parahippocampal gyrus (PHG). This was true for the early part of IC1, i.e. 276–296 ms (t > 3.364, p < .01), 298–316 ms (t > 3.351, p < .01) and 318–338 ms (t > 3.499, p < .01). In the 318–338 ms epoch there were differences in cortical activations, besides the precentral gyrus and PHG, localized to the insula, cingulate gyrus, uncus, subcallosal gyrus, inferior parietal lobule and post-central gyrus. Figure 5a, b show the back-constructed ERPs of IC1 for both groups at Fz, Cz and Pz. It can be seen that IC1 displays a shift towards anterior electrodes in older participants. Figure 6 shows the differences between both groups in cortical activations for IC1. For IC2, IC3 and IC4, there were no brain regions indicating statistically significant differences between younger and older participants.

Relation with Behavioral Data

Associations between individual peak amplitudes of the reconstructed IC1-ERP (at Pz) and RTs in the oddball paradigm, CPT, and the go/no go task were analyzed by non-parametrical correlational analysis.
There were no significant correlations between peak amplitudes and reaction times in the go/no go or oddball paradigms. However, peak amplitudes of the reconstructed IC1-ERPs correlated significantly with reaction times in the CPT (r = −252, p = .014, R2 = 0.06). Higher amplitudes were associated with faster reaction times, see Fig. 7.
Age group comparisons on the behavioral data using the Mann–Whitney U test demonstrate that median reaction times do not differ in the oddball and CPT tasks between younger and older participants. Median reaction times do significantly differ between younger and older participants in the go/no go task (Mann–Whitney U = 738, N young  = 49, N old  = 46, p = .004). The median reaction time of younger and older participants was 277 and 291 ms, respectively.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to unravel aging effects on oddball ERPs, and especially focusing on the P3 elicited in an auditory oddball paradigm. This was attempted by identifying the independent sources that contribute to the P3 and comparing these P3-ICs between younger and older participants. Furthermore, current source density reconstructions of the P3-ICs were compared between both groups to study differences in cortical activity. It was expected that some ICs reflect compensatory activation in older subjects, whereas other ICs reflect general task-related processes.
The ERPs of younger and older participants differed significantly in global field power in the P3 time frame, and to a lower degree also in an earlier time window. Based on their topography and temporal activity profiles, three ICs were identified as sources underlying the P3 wave (IC1, IC2 and IC3). Based on its ERP time course, IC4 appeared to be less likely P3-related, but because of its parieto-central topography it was also included in the initial analyses. For the whole group, IC1 was localized to an orbitofrontal/temporal and a medioparietal region; IC2 was localized to the precuneus/cingulate gyrus region; and IC3 was localized to a region around the right precuneus, see Fig. 4. Our results further corroborate earlier reports showing that the scalp P3 actually reflects a mixture of several statistically independent neural processes (Makeig et al. 1999).
Individual ERPs were back-reconstructed based on the ICs. The three IC-ERPs of the two age groups differed significantly in the time domain. In the source domain, only IC1 showed significant differences in cortical activity between older and younger participants. Here, older participants exhibited stronger activation than younger participants of precentral and parahippocampal regions. For the later part of the IC1 potential other cortical structures, i.e. the insula, cingulate gyrus, uncus, subcallosal gyrus, post-central gyrus and inferior parietal lobule, demonstrated significant differences in activation as well.
In sum, older participants use their available brain capacity differently from younger participants when they are performing the same oddball task. IC1 reflects a different mix of cortical sources in older participants compared to younger participants and therefore, this IC seems to be the most obvious P3-IC that partly might be reflecting compensatory processes in older age [i.e. the frontal P3 component we described earlier in Van Dinteren et al. (2014a)]. Differences in cortical activations were spread over multiple sources indicating there are possibly multiple neural networks involved. IC1 was localized to orbitofrontal and medial regions in the whole sample, which supports the idea that compensatory activation at least partially comes from frontal regions.
When the peaks from the reconstructed IC1 ERPs were compared with reaction times on the included tests a significant correlation was observed between IC1-ERP peak amplitudes and CPT reaction times. CPT performance has long been thought of as a test that assesses working memory but its construct validity has been questioned. The n-back task (CPT) typically demands more recognition where participants discriminate the target stimuli from similar stimuli, e.g. a specific letter among other letters in an array of stimuli (Kane et al. 2007). N-back task accuracy has been found to correlate with the Trail Making Test A, an information processing speed task (Miller et al. 2009). Therefore, IC1 may also be a component extracted from the ERP associated with speed of recognition processes. This altogether supports the assumption that this IC reflects the expected frontal compensation mechanisms, which are increasingly drawn upon during ageing.
There are some limitations to this study. Although, the oddball paradigm is very suitable for research with participants at different ages because of its simplicity, a more difficult paradigm may be even better suited to test for compensatory effects in older ages. A paradigm that allows for manipulation of the task load would be a valuable addition in future research. Another limitation is that a 26-channel EEG is relatively limited for inverse modelling purposes. Although eLORETA has been validated in multiple studies and the standard 10/20 EEG system employing electrode montages with for example 25 electrodes has been proven sufficient for source localization (Pascual-Marqui et al. 2002), ideally more channels should be used to increase the accuracy of localizations (Lantz et al. 2003; Srinivasan et al.; Michel et al. 2004).
In conclusion, the P3 can be separated into three independent components. All of these components differ regarding the strength of activity between younger and older participants when assessed in the electrode space. Despite the significant differences found in the time-domain the effects were not present in the source localization for all ICs, which may of course result from the necessity to correct test statistics for multiple comparisons However, the cortical sources of one component differed between younger and older participants and source analyses of the back-reconstructed ERPs indicated more frontal activation and lesser parietal activation in older participants compared to younger participants. This IC possibly reflects compensatory brain activation that is increasingly recruited as people age. In favor of CRUNCH, as people age the sources of IC1 are shifting whereby frontal contributions are increasing and parietal contributions are declining.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Bridwell DA, Kiehl KA, Pearlson GD, Calhoun VD (2014) Patients with schizophrenia demonstrate reduced cortical sensitivity to auditory oddball regularities. Schizophr Res 158:189–194CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bridwell DA, Kiehl KA, Pearlson GD, Calhoun VD (2014) Patients with schizophrenia demonstrate reduced cortical sensitivity to auditory oddball regularities. Schizophr Res 158:189–194CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Brown SB, van der Wee NJ, van Noorden MS, Giltay EJ, Nieuwenhuis S (2015) Noradrenergic and cholinergic modulation of late ERP responses to deviant stimuli. Psychophysiology 52(12):1620–1631CrossRefPubMed Brown SB, van der Wee NJ, van Noorden MS, Giltay EJ, Nieuwenhuis S (2015) Noradrenergic and cholinergic modulation of late ERP responses to deviant stimuli. Psychophysiology 52(12):1620–1631CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cabeza R (2002) Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol Aging 17:85–100CrossRefPubMed Cabeza R (2002) Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol Aging 17:85–100CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cabeza R, Anderson ND, Locantore JK, McIntosh AR (2002) Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. Neuroimage 17:1394–1402CrossRefPubMed Cabeza R, Anderson ND, Locantore JK, McIntosh AR (2002) Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. Neuroimage 17:1394–1402CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cappell KA, Gmeindl L, Reuter-Lorenz PA (2010) Age differences in prefontal recruitment during verbal working memory maintenance depend on memory load. Cortex 46:462–473CrossRefPubMed Cappell KA, Gmeindl L, Reuter-Lorenz PA (2010) Age differences in prefontal recruitment during verbal working memory maintenance depend on memory load. Cortex 46:462–473CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Daffner KR, Chong H, Sun X, Tarbi EC, Riis JL, McGinnis SM, Holcomb PJ (2011) Mechanisms underlying age- and performance-related differences in working memory. J Cogn Neurosci 23:1298–1314CrossRefPubMed Daffner KR, Chong H, Sun X, Tarbi EC, Riis JL, McGinnis SM, Holcomb PJ (2011) Mechanisms underlying age- and performance-related differences in working memory. J Cogn Neurosci 23:1298–1314CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Debener S, Makeig S, Delorme A, Engel AK (2005) What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related potential as revealed by independent component analysis. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 22:309–321CrossRefPubMed Debener S, Makeig S, Delorme A, Engel AK (2005) What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related potential as revealed by independent component analysis. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 22:309–321CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Eichele T, Rachakonda S, Brakedal B, Eikeland R, Calhoun VD (2011) EEGIFT: group independent component analysis for event-related EEG data. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011:129365CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Eichele T, Rachakonda S, Brakedal B, Eikeland R, Calhoun VD (2011) EEGIFT: group independent component analysis for event-related EEG data. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011:129365CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Friedman D (2003) Cognition and aging: a highly selective overview of event-related potential (ERP) data. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 25:702–720CrossRefPubMed Friedman D (2003) Cognition and aging: a highly selective overview of event-related potential (ERP) data. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 25:702–720CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Friedman D, Kazmerski V, Fabiani M (1997) An overview of age-related changes in the scalp distribution of P3b. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 104:498–513CrossRefPubMed Friedman D, Kazmerski V, Fabiani M (1997) An overview of age-related changes in the scalp distribution of P3b. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 104:498–513CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Frodl T, Juckel G, Gallinat J, Bottlender R, Riedel M, Preuss U, Möller HJ, Hegerl U (2000) Dipole localization of P300 and normal aging. Brain Topogr 13:3–9CrossRefPubMed Frodl T, Juckel G, Gallinat J, Bottlender R, Riedel M, Preuss U, Möller HJ, Hegerl U (2000) Dipole localization of P300 and normal aging. Brain Topogr 13:3–9CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Fuchs M, Kastner J, Wagner M, Hawes S, Ebersole JS (2002) A standardized boundary element method volume conductor model. Clin Neurophysiol 113:702–712CrossRefPubMed Fuchs M, Kastner J, Wagner M, Hawes S, Ebersole JS (2002) A standardized boundary element method volume conductor model. Clin Neurophysiol 113:702–712CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gratton G, Coles MG, Donchin E (1983) A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 55:468–484CrossRefPubMed Gratton G, Coles MG, Donchin E (1983) A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 55:468–484CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Himberg J, Hyvärinen A, Esposito F (2004) Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization. Neuroimage 22:1214–1222CrossRefPubMed Himberg J, Hyvärinen A, Esposito F (2004) Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization. Neuroimage 22:1214–1222CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Jurcak V, Tsuzuki D, Dan I (2007) 10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems. Neuroimage 34:1600–1611CrossRefPubMed Jurcak V, Tsuzuki D, Dan I (2007) 10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems. Neuroimage 34:1600–1611CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kane MJ, Conway AR, Miura TK, Colflesh GJ (2007) Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: a question of construct validity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 33:615–622CrossRefPubMed Kane MJ, Conway AR, Miura TK, Colflesh GJ (2007) Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: a question of construct validity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 33:615–622CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, Kochunov PV, Nickerson D, Mikiten SA, Fox PT (2000) Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum Brain Mapp 10:120–131CrossRefPubMed Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, Kochunov PV, Nickerson D, Mikiten SA, Fox PT (2000) Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum Brain Mapp 10:120–131CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Lantz G, Grave de Peralta R, Spinelli L, Seeck M, Michel CM (2003) Epileptic source localization with high density EEG: how many electrodes are needed? Clin Neurophysiol 114:63–69CrossRefPubMed Lantz G, Grave de Peralta R, Spinelli L, Seeck M, Michel CM (2003) Epileptic source localization with high density EEG: how many electrodes are needed? Clin Neurophysiol 114:63–69CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Lehmann D, Skrandies W (1980) Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential fields. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 48:609–621CrossRefPubMed Lehmann D, Skrandies W (1980) Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential fields. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 48:609–621CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Li L, Gratton C, Fabiani M, Knight RT (2013) Age-related frontoparietal changes during the control of bottom-up and top-down attention: an ERP study. Neurobiol Aging 34:477–488CrossRefPubMed Li L, Gratton C, Fabiani M, Knight RT (2013) Age-related frontoparietal changes during the control of bottom-up and top-down attention: an ERP study. Neurobiol Aging 34:477–488CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Makeig S, Westerfield M, Jung TP, Covington J, Townsend J, Sejnowski TJ, Courchesne E (1999) Functionally independent components of the late positive event-related potential during visual spatial attention. J Neurosci 19:2665–2680PubMed Makeig S, Westerfield M, Jung TP, Covington J, Townsend J, Sejnowski TJ, Courchesne E (1999) Functionally independent components of the late positive event-related potential during visual spatial attention. J Neurosci 19:2665–2680PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Mazziotta J, Toga A, Evans A, Fox P, Lancaster J, Zilles K, Woods R, Paus T, Simpson G, Pike B (2001) A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain: International Consortium for brain mapping (ICBM). Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 356:1293–1322CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Mazziotta J, Toga A, Evans A, Fox P, Lancaster J, Zilles K, Woods R, Paus T, Simpson G, Pike B (2001) A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain: International Consortium for brain mapping (ICBM). Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 356:1293–1322CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Michel CM, Murray MM, Lantz G, Gonzalez S, Spinelli L, de Peralta RG (2004) EEG source imaging. Clin Neurophysiol 115:2195–2222CrossRefPubMed Michel CM, Murray MM, Lantz G, Gonzalez S, Spinelli L, de Peralta RG (2004) EEG source imaging. Clin Neurophysiol 115:2195–2222CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Miller KM, Price CC, Okun MS, Montijo H, Bowers D (2009) Is the n-back task a valid neuropsychological measure for assessing working memory? Arch Clin Neuropsychol 24:711–717CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Miller KM, Price CC, Okun MS, Montijo H, Bowers D (2009) Is the n-back task a valid neuropsychological measure for assessing working memory? Arch Clin Neuropsychol 24:711–717CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat O’Connell RG, Balsters JH, Kilcullen SM, Campbell W, Bokde AW, Lai R, Upton N, Robertson IH (2012) A simultaneous ERP/fMRI investigation of the P300 aging effect. Neurobiol Aging 33:2448–2461CrossRefPubMed O’Connell RG, Balsters JH, Kilcullen SM, Campbell W, Bokde AW, Lai R, Upton N, Robertson IH (2012) A simultaneous ERP/fMRI investigation of the P300 aging effect. Neurobiol Aging 33:2448–2461CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Olbrich S, Mulert C, Karch S, Trenner M, Leicht G, Pogarell O, Hegerl U (2009) EEG-vigilance and BOLD effect during simultaneous EEG/fMRI measurement. Neuroimage 45:319–332CrossRefPubMed Olbrich S, Mulert C, Karch S, Trenner M, Leicht G, Pogarell O, Hegerl U (2009) EEG-vigilance and BOLD effect during simultaneous EEG/fMRI measurement. Neuroimage 45:319–332CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Oostenveld R, Praamstra P (2001) The five percent electrode system for high-resolution EEG and ERP measurements. Clin Neurophysiol 112:713–719CrossRefPubMed Oostenveld R, Praamstra P (2001) The five percent electrode system for high-resolution EEG and ERP measurements. Clin Neurophysiol 112:713–719CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pascual-Marqui RD (2002) Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol 24:5–12PubMed Pascual-Marqui RD (2002) Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol 24:5–12PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pascual-Marqui RD (2007) Discrete, 3D distributed, linear imaging methods of electric neuronal activity. Part 1: exact, zero error localization. arXiv preprint arXiv:0710.3341 Pascual-Marqui RD (2007) Discrete, 3D distributed, linear imaging methods of electric neuronal activity. Part 1: exact, zero error localization. arXiv preprint arXiv:0710.3341
Zurück zum Zitat Pascual-Marqui RD, Michel CM, Lehmann D (1994) Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. Int J Psychophysiol 18:49–65CrossRefPubMed Pascual-Marqui RD, Michel CM, Lehmann D (1994) Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. Int J Psychophysiol 18:49–65CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pascual-Marqui RD, Esslen M, Kochi K, Lehmann D (2002) Functional imaging with low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA): review, new comparisons, and new validation. Jpn J Clin Neurophysiol 30:81–94 Pascual-Marqui RD, Esslen M, Kochi K, Lehmann D (2002) Functional imaging with low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA): review, new comparisons, and new validation. Jpn J Clin Neurophysiol 30:81–94
Zurück zum Zitat Reuter-Lorenz PA, Cappell KA (2008) Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 17:177–182CrossRef Reuter-Lorenz PA, Cappell KA (2008) Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 17:177–182CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ritter W, Vaughan HG (1969) Averaged evoked responses in vigilance and discrimination: a reassessment. Science 164:326–328CrossRefPubMed Ritter W, Vaughan HG (1969) Averaged evoked responses in vigilance and discrimination: a reassessment. Science 164:326–328CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Spencer KM, Dien J, Donchin E (2001) Spatiotemporal analysis of the late ERP responses to deviant stimuli. Psychophysiology 38:343–358CrossRefPubMed Spencer KM, Dien J, Donchin E (2001) Spatiotemporal analysis of the late ERP responses to deviant stimuli. Psychophysiology 38:343–358CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Srinivasan R, Tucker DM, Murias M (1998) Estimating the spatial Nyquist of the human EEG. Behav Res Method Instrum Comput 30:8–19CrossRef Srinivasan R, Tucker DM, Murias M (1998) Estimating the spatial Nyquist of the human EEG. Behav Res Method Instrum Comput 30:8–19CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Steiner GZ, Barry RJ, Gonsalvez CJ (2013) Can working memory predict target-to-target interval effects in the P300? Int J Psychophysiol 89:399–408CrossRefPubMed Steiner GZ, Barry RJ, Gonsalvez CJ (2013) Can working memory predict target-to-target interval effects in the P300? Int J Psychophysiol 89:399–408CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Sutton S, Braren M, Zubin J, John ER (1965) Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science 150:1187–1188CrossRefPubMed Sutton S, Braren M, Zubin J, John ER (1965) Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science 150:1187–1188CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Tsolaki A, Kosmidou V, Hadjileontiadis L, Kompatsiaris IY, Tsolaki M (2015) Brain source localization of MMN, P300 and N400: aging and gender differences. Brain Res 1603:32–49CrossRefPubMed Tsolaki A, Kosmidou V, Hadjileontiadis L, Kompatsiaris IY, Tsolaki M (2015) Brain source localization of MMN, P300 and N400: aging and gender differences. Brain Res 1603:32–49CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Van Dinteren R, Arns M, Jongsma M, Kessels R (2014a) Combined frontal and parietal P300 amplitudes indicate compensated cognitive processing across the lifespan. Front Aging Neurosci 6:294PubMedPubMedCentral Van Dinteren R, Arns M, Jongsma M, Kessels R (2014a) Combined frontal and parietal P300 amplitudes indicate compensated cognitive processing across the lifespan. Front Aging Neurosci 6:294PubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Van Dinteren R, Arns M, Jongsma MLA, Kessels RPC (2014b) P300 development across the lifespan: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 9:e87347CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Van Dinteren R, Arns M, Jongsma MLA, Kessels RPC (2014b) P300 development across the lifespan: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 9:e87347CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Van Dinteren R, Arns M, Kenemans L, Jongsma ML, Kessels RP, Fitzgerald P, Fallahpour K, Debattista C, Gordon E, Williams LM (2015) Utility of event-related potentials in predicting antidepressant treatment response: an iSPOT-D report. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 25(11):1981–1990CrossRefPubMed Van Dinteren R, Arns M, Kenemans L, Jongsma ML, Kessels RP, Fitzgerald P, Fallahpour K, Debattista C, Gordon E, Williams LM (2015) Utility of event-related potentials in predicting antidepressant treatment response: an iSPOT-D report. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 25(11):1981–1990CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat West R, Schwarb H, Johnson BN (2010) The influence of age and individual differences in executive function on stimulus processing in the oddball task. Cortex 46:550–563CrossRefPubMed West R, Schwarb H, Johnson BN (2010) The influence of age and individual differences in executive function on stimulus processing in the oddball task. Cortex 46:550–563CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Williams LM, Rush AJ, Koslow SH, Wisniewski SR, Cooper NJ, Nemeroff CB, Schatzberg AF, Gordon E (2011) International study to predict optimized treatment for depression (iSPOT-D), a randomized clinical trial: rationale and protocol. Trials 12:4. doi:10.1186/1745-6215-12-4 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Williams LM, Rush AJ, Koslow SH, Wisniewski SR, Cooper NJ, Nemeroff CB, Schatzberg AF, Gordon E (2011) International study to predict optimized treatment for depression (iSPOT-D), a randomized clinical trial: rationale and protocol. Trials 12:4. doi:10.​1186/​1745-6215-12-4 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Differences in Cortical Sources of the Event-Related P3 Potential Between Young and Old Participants Indicate Frontal Compensation
verfasst von
R. van Dinteren
R. J. Huster
M. L. A. Jongsma
R. P. C. Kessels
M. Arns
Publikationsdatum
18.01.2017
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Brain Topography / Ausgabe 1/2018
Print ISSN: 0896-0267
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6792
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-016-0542-y

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

Brain Topography 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.