Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Cognitive Therapy and Research 4/2019

17.01.2019 | Original Article

Different Patterns of Attention Bias in Worry and Rumination

verfasst von: Juyoen Hur, Kelly Gaul, Howard Berenbaum

Erschienen in: Cognitive Therapy and Research | Ausgabe 4/2019

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Abstract

In two studies with college student participants, we explored the ways in which worry and rumination may be similar or distinct. Towards that end, as part of our research, we developed new laboratory measures of worry and rumination. In Study 1, we examined how the new lab instruments we developed differentiate worry versus rumination and initiation versus termination. We did so by comparing them with two other measurement methods of worry and rumination, ecological momentary assessment and questionnaires. We found that the laboratory measures were reasonably able to differentiate worry from rumination, but not initiation from termination. In Study 2, we further examined the relationship between attentional biases to threat/danger and loss/failure (using the dot probe task) and both worry and rumination to explore the nature of distinctions between worry and rumination. We found different patterns of attentional bias associated with worry and rumination. Worry was associated with bias away from threat. In contrast, rumination was associated with bias toward loss/failure. The results of the two studies suggest that there are some meaningful differences between worry and rumination. The implications of the current findings are discussed.
Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
Using Meng’s et al. (1992) analytic approach, only one of the eight pairs of correlations—i.e., the correlations of lab rumination initiation with PSWQ versus RRQ—was significantly different, z = − 2.19, p = .03, whereas the other pairs of correlations were not significantly different, ps > .1. The absence of statistical significance is likely a reflection of the small sample size (n = 46).
 
2
See footnote 1.
 
3
Extending Dunn and Clark’s (1969) work, Meng’s et al. (1992) data analytic approach utilizes the Fisher z transformation to test for the significance of the difference between two correlation coefficients.
 
4
For the dot-probe task, we did not use the types of threat stimuli Koster et al. (2006) defined as highly threatening (e.g., mutilated face), but rather used threat stimuli that corresponded to what they defined as mildly threatening (e.g., man with knife).
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1.CrossRef Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Beck, A. T. (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(8), 969–977.CrossRefPubMed Beck, A. T. (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(8), 969–977.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Steer, R. A., Eidelson, J. I., & Riskind, J. H. (1987). Differentiating anxiety and depression: A test of the cognitive content-specificity hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96(3), 179.CrossRefPubMed Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Steer, R. A., Eidelson, J. I., & Riskind, J. H. (1987). Differentiating anxiety and depression: A test of the cognitive content-specificity hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96(3), 179.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Beck, A. T., & Haigh, E. A. (2014). Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: The generic cognitive model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10, 1–24.CrossRefPubMed Beck, A. T., & Haigh, E. A. (2014). Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: The generic cognitive model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10, 1–24.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Berenbaum, H., Chow, P. I., Flores Jr, L. E., Schoenleber, M., Thompson, R. J., & Most, S. B. (2018). A test of the initiation–termination model of worry. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 9(1), 2043808718778965.CrossRef Berenbaum, H., Chow, P. I., Flores Jr, L. E., Schoenleber, M., Thompson, R. J., & Most, S. B. (2018). A test of the initiation–termination model of worry. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 9(1), 2043808718778965.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Borkovec, T. D. (1994). The nature, functions, and origins of worry. In G. Davey & F. Tallis (Eds.), Worrying: Perspectives on theory, assessment and treatment (pp. 5–33). Oxford: Wiley. Borkovec, T. D. (1994). The nature, functions, and origins of worry. In G. Davey & F. Tallis (Eds.), Worrying: Perspectives on theory, assessment and treatment (pp. 5–33). Oxford: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Borkovec, T. D., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., & DePree, J. A. (1983). Preliminary exploration of worry: Some characteristics and processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21(1), 9–16.CrossRefPubMed Borkovec, T. D., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., & DePree, J. A. (1983). Preliminary exploration of worry: Some characteristics and processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21(1), 9–16.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Falla, S. J., & Hamilton, L. R. (1998). Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: Manipulation of stimulus duration. Cognition & Emotion, 12(6), 737–753.CrossRef Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Falla, S. J., & Hamilton, L. R. (1998). Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: Manipulation of stimulus duration. Cognition & Emotion, 12(6), 737–753.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59.CrossRef Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Brown, G. (1989). Cognitive mediation in general psychiatric outpatients: A test of the content-specificity hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(6), 958.CrossRefPubMed Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Brown, G. (1989). Cognitive mediation in general psychiatric outpatients: A test of the content-specificity hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(6), 958.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Dillon, D. G., Rosso, I. M., Pechtel, P., Killgore, W. D., Rauch, S. L., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2014). Peril and pleasure: An RDOC-inspired examination of threat responses and reward processing in anxiety and depression. Depression and Anxiety, 31(3), 233–249.CrossRefPubMed Dillon, D. G., Rosso, I. M., Pechtel, P., Killgore, W. D., Rauch, S. L., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2014). Peril and pleasure: An RDOC-inspired examination of threat responses and reward processing in anxiety and depression. Depression and Anxiety, 31(3), 233–249.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Dunn, O. J., & Clark, V. (1969). Correlation coefficients measured on the same individuals. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 64(325), 366–377.CrossRef Dunn, O. J., & Clark, V. (1969). Correlation coefficients measured on the same individuals. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 64(325), 366–377.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Engels, A. S., Heller, W., Mohanty, A., Herrington, J. D., Banich, M. T., Webb, A. G., & Miller, G. A. (2007). Specificity of regional brain activity in anxiety types during emotion processing. Psychophysiology, 44(3), 352–363.CrossRefPubMed Engels, A. S., Heller, W., Mohanty, A., Herrington, J. D., Banich, M. T., Webb, A. G., & Miller, G. A. (2007). Specificity of regional brain activity in anxiety types during emotion processing. Psychophysiology, 44(3), 352–363.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Eysenck, M. W. (1984). Anxiety and the worry process. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22(6), 545–548.CrossRef Eysenck, M. W. (1984). Anxiety and the worry process. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22(6), 545–548.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gibb, B. E., McGeary, J. E., & Beevers, C. G. (2016). Attentional biases to emotional stimuli: Key components of the RDoC constructs of sustained threat and loss. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 171(1), 65–80.CrossRef Gibb, B. E., McGeary, J. E., & Beevers, C. G. (2016). Attentional biases to emotional stimuli: Key components of the RDoC constructs of sustained threat and loss. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 171(1), 65–80.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goodwin, H., Eagleson, C., Mathews, A., Yiend, J., & Hirsch, C. (2017). Automaticity of attentional bias to threat in high and low worriers. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41(3), 479–488.CrossRefPubMed Goodwin, H., Eagleson, C., Mathews, A., Yiend, J., & Hirsch, C. (2017). Automaticity of attentional bias to threat in high and low worriers. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41(3), 479–488.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Goodwin, H., Yiend, J., & Hirsch, C. R. (2017). Generalized anxiety disorder, worry and attention to threat: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 54, 107–122.CrossRefPubMed Goodwin, H., Yiend, J., & Hirsch, C. R. (2017). Generalized anxiety disorder, worry and attention to threat: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 54, 107–122.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Grafton, B., Southworth, F., Watkins, E., & MacLeod, C. (2016). Stuck in a sad place: Biased attentional disengagement in rumination. Emotion, 16(1), 63.CrossRefPubMed Grafton, B., Southworth, F., Watkins, E., & MacLeod, C. (2016). Stuck in a sad place: Biased attentional disengagement in rumination. Emotion, 16(1), 63.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hankin, B. L., Gibb, B. E., Abela, J. R., & Flory, K. (2010). Selective attention to affective stimuli and clinical depression among youths: Role of anxiety and specificity of emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(3), 491.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hankin, B. L., Gibb, B. E., Abela, J. R., & Flory, K. (2010). Selective attention to affective stimuli and clinical depression among youths: Role of anxiety and specificity of emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(3), 491.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Kellough, J. L., Beevers, C. G., Ellis, A. J., & Wells, T. T. (2008). Time course of selective attention in clinically depressed young adults: An eye tracking study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(11), 1238–1243.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kellough, J. L., Beevers, C. G., Ellis, A. J., & Wells, T. T. (2008). Time course of selective attention in clinically depressed young adults: An eye tracking study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(11), 1238–1243.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Kendall, P. C., & Ingram, R. E. (1989). Cognitive-behavioral perspectives: Theory and research on depression and anxiety. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kendall, P. C., & Ingram, R. E. (1989). Cognitive-behavioral perspectives: Theory and research on depression and anxiety. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Zurück zum Zitat Kirkegaard Thomsen, D. (2006). The association between rumination and negative affect: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 20(8), 1216–1235.CrossRef Kirkegaard Thomsen, D. (2006). The association between rumination and negative affect: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 20(8), 1216–1235.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Attention to threat in anxiety-prone individuals: Mechanisms underlying attentional bias. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30(5), 635–643.CrossRef Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Attention to threat in anxiety-prone individuals: Mechanisms underlying attentional bias. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30(5), 635–643.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, 39–58. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, 39–58.
Zurück zum Zitat MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Anxiety and the allocation of attention to threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 40(4), 653–670.CrossRef MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Anxiety and the allocation of attention to threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 40(4), 653–670.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Martin, L. L., Tesser, A., & McIntosh, W. D. (1993). Wanting but not having: The effects of unattained goals on thoughts and feelings. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Martin, L. L., Tesser, A., & McIntosh, W. D. (1993). Wanting but not having: The effects of unattained goals on thoughts and feelings. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Zurück zum Zitat Meng, X. L., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Comparing correlated correlation coefficients. Psychological bulletin, 111(1), 172–175.CrossRef Meng, X. L., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Comparing correlated correlation coefficients. Psychological bulletin, 111(1), 172–175.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mennin, D. S., & Fresco, D. M. (2013). What, me worry and ruminate about DSM-5 and RDoC? The importance of targeting negative self-referential processing. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 20(3), 258–267. Mennin, D. S., & Fresco, D. M. (2013). What, me worry and ruminate about DSM-5 and RDoC? The importance of targeting negative self-referential processing. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 20(3), 258–267.
Zurück zum Zitat Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(6), 487–495.CrossRefPubMed Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(6), 487–495.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Dixon, C., Fisher, S., Twelftree, H., & McWilliams, A. (2000). Trait anxiety, defensiveness and selective processing of threat: An investigation using two measures of attentional bias. Personality and Individual Differences, 28(6), 1063–1077.CrossRef Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Dixon, C., Fisher, S., Twelftree, H., & McWilliams, A. (2000). Trait anxiety, defensiveness and selective processing of threat: An investigation using two measures of attentional bias. Personality and Individual Differences, 28(6), 1063–1077.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews, A. (1993). Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(2), 304.CrossRefPubMed Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews, A. (1993). Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(2), 304.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 638.CrossRef Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 638.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). The other end of the continuum: The costs of rumination. Psychological Inquiry, 9(3), 216–219.CrossRef Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). The other end of the continuum: The costs of rumination. Psychological Inquiry, 9(3), 216–219.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424.CrossRefPubMed Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Oathes, D. J., Siegle, G. J., & Ray, W. J. (2011). Chronic worry and the temporal dynamics of emotional processing. Emotion, 11(1), 101.CrossRefPubMed Oathes, D. J., Siegle, G. J., & Ray, W. J. (2011). Chronic worry and the temporal dynamics of emotional processing. Emotion, 11(1), 101.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Paulesu, E., Sambugaro, E., Torti, T., Danelli, L., Ferri, F., Scialfa, G.,.. . Sassaroli, S. (2010). Neural correlates of worry in generalized anxiety disorder and in normal controls: A functional MRI study. Psychological Medicine, 40(1), 117–124.CrossRefPubMed Paulesu, E., Sambugaro, E., Torti, T., Danelli, L., Ferri, F., Scialfa, G.,.. . Sassaroli, S. (2010). Neural correlates of worry in generalized anxiety disorder and in normal controls: A functional MRI study. Psychological Medicine, 40(1), 117–124.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Rapee, R. M. (1993). The utilisation of working memory by worry. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31(6), 617–620.CrossRefPubMed Rapee, R. M. (1993). The utilisation of working memory by worry. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31(6), 617–620.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Sass, S. M., Heller, W., Stewart, J. L., Silton, R. L., Edgar, J. C., Fisher, J. E., & Miller, G. A. (2010). Time course of attentional bias in anxiety: Emotion and gender specificity. Psychophysiology, 47(2), 247–259.CrossRefPubMed Sass, S. M., Heller, W., Stewart, J. L., Silton, R. L., Edgar, J. C., Fisher, J. E., & Miller, G. A. (2010). Time course of attentional bias in anxiety: Emotion and gender specificity. Psychophysiology, 47(2), 247–259.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Schmukle, S. C. (2005). Unreliability of the dot probe task. European Journal of Personality, 19(7), 595–605.CrossRef Schmukle, S. C. (2005). Unreliability of the dot probe task. European Journal of Personality, 19(7), 595–605.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime: User’s guide. Sharpsburg: Psychology Software Incorporated. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime: User’s guide. Sharpsburg: Psychology Software Incorporated.
Zurück zum Zitat Sheehan, D., Janavus, J., Baker, R., Harnett-Sheehan, K., Knapp, E., & Sheehan, M. (2001). MINI plus-mini international neuropsychiatric interview. Brazilian version 5.0. 0. DSM IV. Sheehan, D., Janavus, J., Baker, R., Harnett-Sheehan, K., Knapp, E., & Sheehan, M. (2001). MINI plus-mini international neuropsychiatric interview. Brazilian version 5.0. 0. DSM IV.
Zurück zum Zitat Southworth, F., Grafton, B., MacLeod, C., & Watkins, E. (2017). Heightened ruminative disposition is associated with impaired attentional disengagement from negative relative to positive information: Support for the “impaired disengagement” hypothesis. Cognition and Emotion, 31(3), 422–434.CrossRefPubMed Southworth, F., Grafton, B., MacLeod, C., & Watkins, E. (2017). Heightened ruminative disposition is associated with impaired attentional disengagement from negative relative to positive information: Support for the “impaired disengagement” hypothesis. Cognition and Emotion, 31(3), 422–434.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Stokes, C., & Hirsch, C. R. (2010). Engaging in imagery versus verbal processing of worry: Impact on negative intrusions in high worriers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(5), 418–423.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Stokes, C., & Hirsch, C. R. (2010). Engaging in imagery versus verbal processing of worry: Impact on negative intrusions in high worriers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(5), 418–423.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Stone, A., Shiffman, S., Atienza, A., & Nebeling, L. (2007). The science of real-time data capture: Self-reports in health research. Oxford University Press. Stone, A., Shiffman, S., Atienza, A., & Nebeling, L. (2007). The science of real-time data capture: Self-reports in health research. Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(2), 284.CrossRefPubMed Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(2), 284.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wanke, M., & Schmid, J. (1996). Rumination: When AII EIse FaiIs. Ruminative Thoughts, 9, 177. Wanke, M., & Schmid, J. (1996). Rumination: When AII EIse FaiIs. Ruminative Thoughts, 9, 177.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilson, E., & MacLeod, C. (2003). Contrasting two accounts of anxiety-linked attentional bias: Selective attention to varying levels of stimulus threat intensity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(2), 212.CrossRefPubMed Wilson, E., & MacLeod, C. (2003). Contrasting two accounts of anxiety-linked attentional bias: Selective attention to varying levels of stimulus threat intensity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(2), 212.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Different Patterns of Attention Bias in Worry and Rumination
verfasst von
Juyoen Hur
Kelly Gaul
Howard Berenbaum
Publikationsdatum
17.01.2019
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Cognitive Therapy and Research / Ausgabe 4/2019
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-018-09993-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2019

Cognitive Therapy and Research 4/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.