Skip to main content
Erschienen in: International Journal for Equity in Health 1/2018

Open Access 01.12.2018 | Research

Does cultural capital contribute to educational inequalities in food consumption in the Netherlands? A cross-sectional analysis of the GLOBE-2011 survey

verfasst von: Carlijn B. M. Kamphuis, Joost Oude Groeniger, Frank J. van Lenthe

Erschienen in: International Journal for Equity in Health | Ausgabe 1/2018

Abstract

Background

The importance of culture for food consumption is widely acknowledged, as well as the fact that culture-based resources (“cultural capital”) differ between educational groups. Since current explanations for educational inequalities in healthy and unhealthy food consumption (e.g. economic capital, social capital) are unable to fully explain this gradient, we aim to investigate a new explanation for educational inequalities in healthy food consumption, i.e. the role of cultural capital.

Methods

Data were obtained cross-sectionally by a postal survey among participants of the GLOBE study in the Netherlands in 2011 (N = 2953; response 67.1%). The survey measured respondents’ highest attained educational level, food-related cultural capital (institutionalised, objectivised and incorporated cultural capital), economic capital (e.g. home ownership, financial strain), social capital (e.g. social support, health-related social leverage, interpersonal relationships), and frequency of consumption of healthy and unhealthy food products. Two general outcomes (overall healthy food consumption, and overall unhealthy food consumption), and seven specific food consumption outcomes were constructed, and prevalence ratios (PR) were estimated in Poisson regression models with robust variance.

Results

Cultural capital was significantly associated with all food outcomes, also when social and economic capital were taken into account. Those with low levels of cultural capital were more likely to have a lower overall healthy food consumption (PR 1.35, 95% CI 1.22–1.49), a lower consumption of whole wheat bread (PR 1.21, 95% CI 1.05–1.38), vegetables (PR 1.55, 95% CI 1.40–1.71), and meat-substitutes and fish (PR 1.74, 95% CI 1.53–1.97), and a higher consumption of fried food (PR 1.59, 95% CI 1.31–1.93). Social capital was positively associated with overall healthy food consumption, whole wheat bread consumption, and the consumption of fish and meat-substitutes, and economic capital with none of the outcomes. The PR of the lowest educational group to have a low overall healthy food consumption decreased from 1.48 (95% CI 1.28–1.73) to 1.22 (95% CI 1.04–1.43) when cultural, social and economic capital were taken into account.

Conclusions

Cultural capital contributed to the explanation of educational inequalities in food consumption in The Netherlands, over and above economic and social capital. The socialisation processes through which cultural capital is acquired could offer new entry-points for the promotion of healthy food consumption among low educational groups.

Introduction

Studies consistently find a socioeconomic gradient in healthy dietary intakes [16]. However, interventions to encourage healthy food consumption have only had small effects, and if so, particularly among high socioeconomic groups [7, 8]. Therefore, there is a high need to identify relevant determinants of healthy food consumption in order to find entry points for developing interventions that may increase healthier food consumption, especially among low socioeconomic groups.
Previous studies have identified explanatory factors that partly explain socioeconomic inequalities in diet. Economic resources, such as an adequate food budget, are typically connected to an individual’s socioeconomic position, and to healthy food intakes, as lower-quality diets generally cost less per calorie [1, 9, 10]. Also, social resources, measured through membership in support-providing networks, perceived social support, or perceived social norms, have shown to be associated with healthy food intakes [11, 12], although their contribution to socioeconomic differences in healthy food consumption is less clear [1, 13]. As measures of economic and social resources cannot fully explain the socioeconomic gradient in healthy food consumption, recently, studies have appeared taking a different angle. These studies have linked cultural resources to health inequalities [14, 15], and have argued that culture-based activities, knowledge and perceptions present a unique form of health-relevant ‘capital’.
Culture can be defined as the culture-based resources that shape and influence people’s habits, values, norms, knowledge and preferences, acquired mostly through social learning [14]. Learning conditions vary across socioeconomic groups and milieus, and so culture does as well [14]. Culture, further, is well-known for its important nfluence on food consumption, as it determines what people consider to be acceptable and preferable foods, and what the amount and combinations of food they choose [16, 17]. Although some research has emerged over the last few years [1820], empirical evidence for the role of cultural factors for the explanation of socioeconomic inequalities in food consumption is still limited.
Among the most influential studies regarding the role of culture for daily practices is the work of the French sociologist Pierre Bourdieu (1930–2003) [21]. High socioeconomic groups, over their life courses, acquire more capital and ‘use’ this to develop a taste for specific forms of music, lecture, leisure activities, and foods. Bourdieu defines three types of capital that play a role in this process, namely cultural, economic, and social capital. Cultural capital is a non-material resource that accumulates throughout the life course, acquired through education and life-long socialisation, and includes “the distinctive forms of knowledge and ability that people acquire [...] from their training in the cultural disciplines” [21]. Through available cultural capital in the family, one is more inclined to ‘inherit’ cultural resources that can be mobilised to accumulate incorporated cultural capital [22]. Cultural capital emerges in three different states: incorporated cultural capital (e.g. values, skills, cultural participation), objectivised cultural capital (e.g. books, tools) and institutionalised cultural capital (e.g. educational degrees, professional titles) [21]. Lareau and Weininger ([23], p. 156) refer to incorporated cultural capital as “the legitimate cultural attitudes, preferences and behaviours […] that are internalized during the socialization process”. Incorporated cultural capital, “the form of long-lasting dispositions of the mind and the body”, entails socialisation, personal effort, and time investment ([22], p. 47). In line with reflections by Abel [14], we expect incorporated cultural capital to be more important for educational inequalities in food choices than institutionalised and objectivised cultural capital. Also, incorporated cultural capital has the largest potential to be on the causal chain between socioeconomic position and healthy food choices. It is not possible to convey incorporated cultural capital to someone else, as would be possible with economic capital or objectivised cultural capital.
Besides cultural capital, Bourdieu also acknowledges the importance of economic and social capital. Economic capital comprises all sources of income (including wealth), as well as the security of having a reliable income. Social capital is defined by Bourdieu as “the aggregate of the actual or potential resources which are linked to possession of a durable network of more or less institutionalized relationships of mutual acquaintance or recognition”. Economic, social and cultural capital are correlated and feed on each other. The different forms of capital can be converted as, for example, personal savings (economic capital) can be used to pay for advanced education (cultural capital) [14]. The roles of economic and social capital for inequalities in health and health-behaviours have been studied rather extensively, whereas the role of cultural capital is largely unknown. The aim of this study is to investigate whether cultural capital contributes to the explanation of socioeconomic inequalities in food consumption among adults, over and above social and economic capital.

Methods

Self-reported data were collected by means of a large-scale postal survey in 2011, administered as a new wave of data collection for the longitudinal GLOBE study [24]. The research was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki, and informed consent was obtained from all subjects. No formal approval of the medical ethics committee of the Erasmus University Medical Centre was required for the study. The use of personal data in the GLOBE study is in compliance with the Dutch Personal Data Protection Act and the Municipal Database Act, and has been registered with the Dutch Data Protection Authority (number 1248943).
Of the respondents to the previous GLOBE survey in 2004, which formed a stratified sample of the 25–75 years old population in the city of Eindhoven and surrounding cities in 2004 (N = 4784), n = 249 had died, n = 76 had emigrated, and n = 14 were lost to follow-up (i.e. no correct address information available), which resulted in a sample of N = 4437 that was sent the 2011-survey. For the total of 2983 respondents that returned the survey (response 67.2%), missing values for sex (n = 21), age (n = 24), and educational level (n = 172) could largely be replaced by information from the 2004-questionnaire, resulting in only one case with a missing value for age and 29 cases with missing values for educational level. These 30 respondents were excluded from the analysis, resulting in an analytic sample of N = 2953.

Educational level as indicator of socioeconomic position, and demographic variables

Educational level has traditionally been the most important indicator of social stratification in Dutch society [25, 26]. It is also an appropriate indicator of socioeconomic position to classify both men and women, in contrast to occupational level and income level (as women are more likely than men to not have a paid job) [27]. Respondents indicated their highest attained educational level, which was classified according to the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED): 1 – primary education (ISCED 0–1), 2 – lower secondary education (ISCED 2), 3 – upper secondary education (ISCED 3–4), 4 – tertiary education (ISCED 5–7). All analyses were adjusted for marital status (married, single/divorced/widowed), ethnic background (native Dutch, other), age and sex.

Food consumption

With a Food Frequency Questionnaire (based on existing questionnaires [2830]) that was part of the GLOBE 2011-survey, we obtained self-reported information on the frequency with which 26 specific food groups were consumed. Participants indicated the number of days per week a certain food product was consumed. This number was converted to an indicator for ‘average daily frequency’ by the following formula [31]: never: 0; less than once a week: 0.10; 1–2 days per week: 0.20; 3–4 days per week: 0.50; 5–6 days per week: 0.80; every day: 1.
A ‘healthy foods’ score was constructed as the sum of the consumption of fruit, cooked vegetables, raw vegetables, whole wheat bread, skimmed milk, low fat cheese, chicken, fish, and meat-substitutes (like tofu). To calculate this score, the average daily frequencies (ranging from 0 till 1, as detailed above) for each of these products were summed. Similarly, an ‘unhealthy foods’ score was constructed as the sum of the frequencies of consumption of fried food, candy, white bread, soft drinks, whole milk, high fat cheese, and red meat (beef, pork, lamb, mince, and burgers). These indices were considered useful as general measures of healthy and unhealthy eating [31], since the specific food products included in these measures are also recognized as typically healthy or unhealthy by international authorities, such as the American Heart Foundation, the British Nutrition Foundation and the Netherlands Nutrition Centre [3234]. For detailed analyses, seven specific food outcomes were analysed, representing four typically healthy food groups (whole wheat bread, fruits, vegetables, and meat-substitutes & fish) and three typically unhealthy food groups (red meat (beef, pork, mince, and burgers), fried food, and soft drinks). Means and standard deviations for the raw daily frequency scores by educational level are presented in Additional file 1. As these scores had skewed distributions, the variables were dichotomised with the median as cut-off point, i.e. half of the sample was categorised as having a ‘low’ consumption, and half of the sample as ‘high’ consumption.

Cultural, social and economic capital

We generated composite variables of cultural, social and economic capital based on the scores of the constituent items chosen for capturing each type of capital. Table 1 lists the variables for cultural, social and economic capital, their categorisation for the analyses, and the items that comprised each variable. To construct the variables, several items were combined by means of a factor analyses or a mean score, and these were further divided in tertiles. A detailed description of the measurement and construction of each variable is available in Additional file 2.
Table 1
Measurement and construction of the variables for cultural, social and economic capital
Variables
Measurement in the survey
Categorisation of the variable for the analyses
Family institutionalised cultural capital
Educational level of the respondent’s father, mother and partner
1 = low, 2 = mid, 3 = high (tertiles of mean score)
Objectivised cultural capital
Number of cooking-related possessions, i.e. a stove, cook book(s), set of knives, kitchen scale, and juicer (yes/no)
1 = low, 2 = mid, 3 = high (tertiles of sum score)
Incorporated cultural capital
Participation, cooking skills, grocery shopping skills, information seeking and processing skills, nutrition knowledge
1 = low, 2 = mid, 3 = high (tertiles of mean score)
Total cultural capital
Mean score of the variables for family institutionalised, objectivised, and total incorporated cultural capital
1 = low, 2 = mid, 3 = high (tertiles of mean score)
Total social capital
Social support, health-related social leverage, interpersonal relationship network, social participation, perceptions of trust, perceived neighbourhood social capital
1 = low, 2 = mid, 3 = high (tertiles of mean score)
Total economic capital
Household equivalent income, home ownership, crowding, financial strain
1 = low, 2 = mid, 3 = high (tertiles of mean score)
Note: Detailed information on measurement and construction of the variables is available in Additional file 2. Information on the development of the cultural capital questionnaire is described elsewhere [5].
In short, we used an existing questionnaire to measure the three forms of cultural capital in relation to food [5]. This questionnaire has been developed based on a systematic review to identify existing indicators of cultural capital. The indicators that have been used most often in the literature, were translated to food-related indicators [5]. Objectivised cultural capital was consistently measured in the literature by cultural possessions (e.g. art, books) and we translated this to a list of possessions related to food choice behaviour. In the survey, participants reported whether they owned a list of cooking-related possessions, e.g. cook books, kitchen scale, juicer. Scores were summed and the sum score was divided in tertiles (low, medium, high objectivised cultural capital). Incorporated cultural capital was operationalised by items on participation, cooking skills, grocery shopping skills, information seeking skills, and nutrition knowledge. Scores on the different items were summed and the sum score was divided in tertiles (low, medium, high incorporated cultural capital). Institutionalised cultural capital appeared to be most often operationalised by educational level of the respondent [5]. However, since we were interested in understanding educational inequalities (i.e. we used own education level as indicator of socioeconomic position), we focussed on the socialisation processes in which acquisition of cultural capital takes place, and therefore used educational level of the father, mother and partner of the respondent as indicators of institutionalised cultural capital. Scores were summed and the sum score was divided in tertiles (low, medium, high institutionalised cultural capital). The three types of cultural capital were analysed as separate variables. A mean score of these three variables was used as indicator of total cultural capital, which was divided into tertiles (low, medium, high total cultural capital).
For social capital, indicators of six dimensions of social capital (e.g. social support, health-related social leverage, interpersonal relationship network) [35] were combined in one score for total social capital, which was divided into tertiles (low, medium, high social capital). Economic capital was measured by four commonly used indicators (e.g. home ownership, financial strain) [36] and their mean score was divided into tertiles (low, medium, high economic capital).

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted in SPSS 20.0. Since our outcomes are not rare (i.e. greater than 10%), we follow recommendations to calculate prevalence ratios (PR’s) as measure of association, instead of odds ratios (OR’s), as the interpretation of the OR is difficult and often mistakenly interpreted as PR [37, 38]. In Poisson regression models with robust variance, PR’s with 95% confidence intervals were calculated for each of the outcomes by educational level, adjusted for age, sex, ethnicity, and marital status. Further, PR’s with 95% confidence intervals were calculated for each of the outcomes by each type of capital in separate models, adjusted for educational level, age, sex, ethnicity, and marital status level. In multivariate Poisson regression models, we included all capital variables simultaneously to observe which types of capital remained significantly associated with food consumption when mutually adjusted, and to observe whether the PR’s for the low educational group would attenuate after inclusion of the capital variables, compared to the model with only confounders. This reduction in PR’s was interpreted as the contribution of the capital variables to the explanation of educational inequalities in food consumption.

Results

The mean age of the sample was 56.4 years (SD 13.0) and 56.7% was female (Table 2). In general, educational inequalities in healthy food consumption were larger than those in unhealthy food consumption (Table 3). Low educated were more likely to report a low overall healthy food consumption (PR 1.48, 95% CI 1.28–1.73), low whole wheat bread consumption (PR 1.38, 95% CI 1.08–1.76), low vegetable consumption (PR 1.46, 95% CI 1.23–1.73) and a low consumption of meat-substitutes and fish (PR 1.66, 95% CI 1.37–2.02) than high educated. Regarding unhealthy food outcomes, low educational groups were about twice as likely to have a high fried food consumption (PR 2.03, 95% CI 1.44–2.86), but no significant inequalities in overall unhealthy food, red meat, or soft drink consumption were observed. Outcomes for which no educational inequalities were found, were not further analysed in multivariate models.
Table 2
Study sample characteristics: demographic factors, and cultural, social and economic capital by educational level
  
Educational level
Total
1-low
2-midlow
3-midhigh
4-high
(N = 2953)a
(n = 263)a
(n = 1041)a
(n = 678)a
(n = 971)a
%b
%b
%b
%b
%b
Sex
 Men
43.3
41.4
32.9
44.5
51.8
 Women
56.7
58.6
67.1
55.5
48.2
Marital status
 Married, registered partnership
74.9
63.2
72.4
78.6
76.5
 Single, divorced, widowed
24.6
34.6
27.2
21.0
23.2
 Missing
0.5
2.2
0.4
0.4
0.3
Ethnic backgroundc
 Dutch
84.5
64.9
85.4
87.5
84.9
 Other
10.4
17.8
8.5
10.2
10.9
 Missing
5.1
17.3
6.0
2.3
4.2
Age, mean, in years (SD)
56.4 (13.0)
66.0 (12.3)
61.8 (10.7)
52.9 (12.2)
52.6 (12.9)
Total cultural capital
 Low
39.1
81.6
53.6
38.3
19.6
 Mid
24.4
7.6
27.0
24.2
25.1
 High
36.4
10.3
19.3
37.5
55.2
 Missing
0.1
0.5
0.1
0.0
0.1
Institutionalised cultural capital
 Low
39.4
53.5
60.9
36.2
20.3
 Mid
20.1
1.6
11.4
26.8
26.0
 High
30.3
9.2
12.5
30.1
49.8
 Missing
10.2
35.7
15.1
6.9
4.0
Objectivised cultural capital
 Low
19.1
52.7
22.2
16.2
12.6
 Mid
23.7
17.2
25.8
24.8
22.1
 High
56.0
24.2
50.
58.2
64.7
 Missing
1.3
5.9
1.6
0.8
0.6
Incorporated cultural capital
 Low
29.9
59.7
35.5
28.4
21.0
 Mid
38.4
26.9
42.1
40.4
35.7
 High
31.5
12.9
22.2
31.3
43.1
 Missing
0.2
0.5
0.2
0.0
0.3
Total social capital
 Low
32.0
53.0
35.0
31.4
26.2
 Mid
32.6
28.6
34.0
31.3
33.1
 High
35.4
18.4
31.0
37.0
40.8
 Missing
0.0
0.0
0.0
0.0
0.0
Total economic capital
 Low
36.5
80.0
50.2
36.7
16.8
 Mid
35.3
14.6
31.4
36.2
41.6
 High
27.9
5.4
17.8
27.1
41.3
 Missing
0.3
0.0
0.5
0.0
0.3
aAll numbers (N) are unweighted and reflect the actual numbers of participants in the dataset
bAll percentages (%) are weighted and thereby represent the prevalence rates as they existed in the population of Eindhoven of 2004, which is the source population. The weight factors were calculated from the distribution of the characteristics in a random sample drawn from the municipal registry in Eindhoven, October 2004
cDutch: both parents of the respondent were born in the Netherlands (definition by Statistics Netherlands). Other: at least one parent of the respondent was not born in the Netherlands
Table 3
Separate poisson regression models for educational level, total cultural capital, three specific types of cultural capital, total social capital, and total economic capital in their association with food consumption (adjusted for confoundersa)
 
Sum scores
Specific food outcomes
Low overall healthy food consumption
High overall unhealthy food consumption
Low whole wheat bread
Low fruit
Low vegetables
Low fish/meat substitute
High fried food
High red meat
High soft drink
(n = 2947) b
(n = 2938) b
(n = 2900) b
(n = 2902) b
(n = 2934) b
(n = 2822) b
(n = 2853) b
(n = 2906) b
(n = 2747) b
PR
(95% CI)
PR
(95% CI)
PR
(95% CI)
PR
(95% CI)
PR
(95% CI)
PR
(95% CI)
PR
(95% CI)
PR
(95% CI)
PR
(95% CI)
Educational level
 1 High
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
 2
1.18***
(1.07–1.30)
1.01
(0.92–1.12)
1.32***
(1.16–1.49)
1.09*
(1.01–1.17)
1.32***
(1.23–1.73)
1.33***
(1.18–1.50)
1.59***
(1.33–1.90)
1.03
(0.94–1.14)
1.00
(0.91–1.10)
 3
1.24***
(1.12–1.37)
1.00
(0.90–1.12)
1.40***
(1.22–1.61)
1.04
(0.95–1.14)
1.38***
(1.25–1.52)
1.36***
(1.21–1.54)
1.60***
(1.30–1.97)
0.93
(0.83–1.03)
1.05
(0.95–1.16)
 4 Low
1.48***
(1.28–1.73)
0.88
(0.71–1.10)
1.38**
(1.08–1.76)
1.12
(0.93–1.34)
1.46***
(1.23–1.73)
1.66***
(1.37–2.02)
2.03***
(1.44–2.86)
1.11
(0.92–1.35)
1.16
(0.96–1.41)
Total cultural capital
 High
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
 Mid
1.24***
(1.12–.139)
1.09
(0.97–1.22)
1.14
(0.99–1.31)
1.13*
(1.03–1.24)
1.13**
(1.03–1.24)
1.33***
(1.16–1.52)
1.29*
(1.05–1.58)
0.96
(0.86–1.07)
1.14**
(1.02–1.27)
 Low
1.38***
(1.25–1.53)
1.19**
(1.07–1.32)
1.27***
(1.11–1.45)
1.28***
(1.18–1.40)
1.28***
(1.18–1.40)
1.77***
(1.56–2.00)
1.58***
(1.31–1.91)
1.06
(0.96–1.18)
1.30***
(1.18–1.44)
Institutionalised cultural capital
 High
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
 Mid
1.11
(0.99–1.25)
0.97
(0.86–1.09)
1.03
(0.89–1.20)
1.14**
(1.05–1.25)
1.10
(0.97–1.23)
0.98
(0.85–1.13)
1.24
(1.00–1.53)
1.07
(0.96–1.19)
1.05
(0.95–1.17)
 Low
1.20***
(1.08–1.34)
0.95
(0.85–1.07)
1.26***
(1.09–1.45)
1.12*
(1.02–1.23)
1.36***
(1.23–1.52)
1.21**
(1.07–1.38)
1.31*
(1.07–1.62)
1.00
(0.90–1.11)
1.09
(0.98–1.21)
Objectivised cultural capital
 High
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
 Mid
1.18***
(1.07–1.29)
1.00
(0.90–1.11)
1.04
(0.92–1.18)
1.10*
(1.02–1.19)
1.15**
(1.05–1.25)
1.33***
(1.19–1.48)
0.97
(0.81–1.16)
1.01
(0.93–1.12)
1.00
(0.91–1.10)
 Low
1.21***
(1.10–1.34)
0.97
(0.86–1.09)
1.20**
(1.06–1.37)
1.18***
(1.08–1.29)
1.11*
(1.01–1.23)
1.38***
(1.23–1.56)
0.91
(0.74–1.12)
0.88*
(0.78–0.99)
1.01
(0.91–1.13)
Incorporated cultural capital
 High
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
 Mid
1.25***
(1.13–1.39)
1.25***
(1.12–1.39)
1.15*
(1.01–1.31)
1.14**
(1.05–1.24)
1.26***
(1.14–1.40)
1.33***
(1.17–1.51)
1.42***
(1.16–1.73)
1.11*
(1.01–1.23)
1.18**
(1.06–1.31)
 Low
1.35***
(1.22–1.51)
1.18**
(1.05–1.33)
1.14
(0.99–1.31)
1.30***
(1.19–1.42)
1.52***
(1.37–1.68)
1.74***
(1.53–1.98)
1.76***
(1.44–2.14)
1.06
(0.95–1.19)
1.37***
(1.23–1.52)
Total social capital
 High
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
 Mid
1.03
(0.93–1.13)
0.99
(0.90–1.10)
0.99
(0.87–1.13)
1.04
(0.96–1.13)
0.98
(0.89–1.07)
1.08
(0.96–1.21)
0.94
(0.79–1.12)
1.03
(0.94–1.13)
1.00
(0.91–1.09)
 Low
1.17***
(1.06–1.28)
1.04
(0.94–1.16)
1.27***
(1.12–1.44)
1.16***
(1.07–1.26)
1.08
(0.99–1.18)
1.30***
(1.16–1.46)
0.93
(0.77–1.12)
0.89**
(0.80–0.99)
1.05
(0.96–1.16)
Total economic capital
 High
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
 Mid
1.00
(0.90–1.10)
1.01
(0.91–1.12)
1.12
(0.99–1.28)
0.99
(0.91–1.07)
0.96
(0.87–1.06)
0.96
(0.85–1.08)
1.03
(0.86–1.24)
0.90*
(0.81–0.99)
1.02
(0.93–1.12)
 Low
1.13*
(1.02–1.25)
0.99
(0.88–1.12)
1.16*
(1.00–1.34)
1.10*
(1.01–1.20)
1.06
(0.95–1.17)
1.05
(0.93–1.19)
1.07
(0.87–1.31)
0.85**
(0.76–0.95)
0.98
(0.88–1.09)
* = p < .050, ** = p ≤ .010, *** = p ≤ .001; OR odds ratio. a All models included education and the following confounders: age, sex, ethnic background and marital status
bVarying sample sizes due to missing values on the food outcomes
In univariate models (as presented in Table 3), lower levels of cultural, social and economic capital were in general related to lower healthy food consumption and higher unhealthy food consumption (except for red meat). Incorporated cultural capital was most consistently and strongest associated with the outcomes, compared to institutionalised and objectivised cultural capital. Those with low social capital were more likely to report a low overall healthy food consumption (PR 1.17, 95% CI 1.06–1.28), and a low consumption of whole wheat bread (PR 1.27, 95% CI 1.12–1.44), fruit (PR 1.16, 95% CI 1.07–1.26), and meat-substitutes and fish (PR 1.30, 95% CI 1.16–1.46). A low level of economic capital was associated with low overall healthy food consumption, low whole-wheat bread consumption, low fruit consumption, and low red meat consumption.
In multivariate models including educational level, and cultural, social and economic capital, cultural capital remained significantly associated with all outcomes (Table 4). Having less cultural capital was related to a lower overall healthy food consumption (PR 1.35, 95% CI 1.22–1.49), lower consumption of whole wheat bread (PR 1.21, 95% CI 1.05–1.38), vegetables (PR 1.55, 95% CI 1.40–1.71), and meat-substitutes & fish (PR 1.74, 95% CI 1.53–1.97), and a higher consumption of fried food (PR 1.59, 95% CI 1.31–1.93). Social capital remained associated with overall healthy food consumption, whole wheat bread consumption, and meat-substitutes & fish consumption, but economic capital with none of the outcomes. In these multivariate models, PR’s for the low educational group attenuated considerably after inclusion of the capital variables (Table 4), compared to the model with only confounders (Table 3). For instance, the PR of the lowest compared to the highest educational group for having a low overall healthy food consumption decreased from 1.48 (95% CI 1.28–1.73) (when only adjusted for confounders; Table 3) to 1.22 (95% CI 1.04–1.43), when cultural, social and economic capital were taken into account (Table 4). However, educational inequalities in food consumption remained significant for all outcomes.
Table 4
Simultaneous adjustment of total cultural, social and economic capital on educational inequalities in food consumption, adjusted for confoundersa
 
Low overall healthy food consumption
Low whole wheat bread
Low vegetable
Low fish & meat-substitutes
High fried food
(n = 2947) b
(n = 2900) b
(n = 2934) b
(n = 2833) b
(n = 2853) b
OR
(95% CI)
OR
(95% CI)
OR
(95% CI)
OR
(95% CI)
OR
(95% CI)
Educational level
 1 High
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
 2
1.09
(0.98–1.20)
1.23**
(1.08–1.40)
1.20***
(1.09–1.32)
1.18**
(1.05–1.33)
1.42***
(1.18–1.71)
 3
1.08
(0.97–1.20)
1.26**
(1.09–1.47)
1.18**
(1.06–1.31)
1.10
(0.97–1.26)
1.35**
(1.08–1.69)
 4 Low
1.22*
(1.04–1.43)
1.19
(0.92–1.53)
1.17
(0.98–1.40)
1.23*
(1.00–1.51)
1.66**
(1.15–2.38)
Total cultural capital
 High
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
 Mid
1.23***
(1.11–1.37)
1.13
(0.98–1.30)
1.30***
(1.16–1.45)
1.32***
(1.15–1.51)
1.29*
(1.05–1.59)
 Low
1.35***
(1.22–1.49)
1.21**
(1.05–1.38)
1.55***
(1.40–1.71)
1.74***
(1.53–1.97)
1.59***
(1.31–1.93)
Total social capital
 High
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
 Mid
1.01
(0.91–1.11)
0.98
(0.86–1.11)
0.96
(0.87–1.05)
1.05
(0.94–1.18)
0.92
(0.77–1.10)
 Low
1.11*
(1.01–1.22)
1.23**
(1.08–1.40)
1.02
(0.93–1.12)
1.21***
(1.08–1.36)
0.87
(0.72–1.05)
Total economic capital
 High
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
1.00
 
 Mid
0.97
(0.87–1.07)
1.09
(0.95–1.24)
0.93
(0.84–1.02)
0.89
(0.79–1.01)
1.00
(0.83–1.21)
 Low
1.06
(0.95–1.18)
1.08
(0.93–1.25)
0.98
(0.89–1.09)
0.93
(0.82–1.06)
1.03
(0.83–1.27)
* = p < .050, ** = p ≤ .010, *** = p ≤ .001; OR odds ratio. a All models included educational level, total cultural capital, total social capital, total economic capital, and confounders (age, sex, ethnic background and marital status). b Varying sample sizes due to missing values on the food outcomes

Discussion

This is the first study to investigate the contributions of cultural, social and economic capital to educational inequalities in food consumption among adults. Educational inequalities in healthy food consumption were larger than those in unhealthy food consumption. Cultural capital contributed to the explanation of educational inequalities in food consumption more so than social and economic capital. Associations between cultural capital and food consumption remained significant when adjusted for social and economic capital.
Our finding that low educational groups consumed less healthy foods is in line with previous empirical studies [15]. These results also largely confirm Bourdieu’s own observations regarding food consumption, as reported in his book Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgement of Taste [21]. He wrote that individuals from lower classes with a low level of capital tended to prefer ‘heavy, fatty, fattening foods, which are also cheap’ ([21], p. 177) and preferred ‘the plentiful’ as opposed to ‘the light, refined, and delicate foods’ valued by high classes with higher levels of capital [19, 21]. Further, he observed that those with high cultural capital seemed to be more inclined towards asceticism and pursue original foods with an abundance of vegetables, whereas those with high economic capital preferred more traditional, rich dishes - a taste that resembles those of lower classes [19, 21]. In line with this, we saw that those with higher economic capital were more likely to consume more of the “traditional, rich” red meat products (e.g. beef, pork, mince, and burgers).
While we found clear positive relations between educational level and healthy food consumption, and between cultural capital and healthy food consumption, fewer associations with unhealthy food consumption were found. Apparently, possessing higher levels of cultural capital facilitates the choice of healthy foods, but having more cultural capital does not seem to prevent against unhealthy food consumption. This finding may suggest that high educated, with more cultural capital, make healthy food choices for other reasons than for reasons of health (because, if the latter was the case, one would expect them to also refrain from unhealthy foods). Following Bourdieu’s line of reasoning, one interpretation could be that healthy foods are consumed for reasons of distinction, and that consuming healthy foods is considered a more effective means of ‘distinction’ than refraining from unhealthy foods. A reason for this could be that a behaviour you practise (e.g. eating healthy foods) is more visible and obvious, and therefore more useful for distinction, than a behaviour you refrain from (e.g. not eating unhealthy foods). The findings from a study into educational differences in ‘super foods’ consumption also point to this mechanism of distinction [39].
Economic capital showed only weak associations with food consumption, and did hardly contribute to the explanation of educational inequalities in healthy and unhealthy food consumption. This could be due to the selection of the specific food products that were analysed, as the unhealthy food options (white bread, fried foods, red meat products) may not necessarily be cheaper than their healthy counterparts. However, also previous research from the Netherlands did not found indications that price considerations are an important barrier for healthy food consumption among low educational groups in the Netherlands [2, 40, 41].
Our finding that cultural capital adds to explanation of educational inequalities in food choices, over and above economic and social capital, is in line with two previous studies among adolescents [18, 20]. Taking all capital variables into account in multivariate models considerably reduced the educational inequalities in healthy and unhealthy food consumption, but not completely. This indicates that other factors than those covered by cultural, social, and economic capital contribute to the observed gradients. A factor that we did not take into account, and that has found to play a role in the international context (e.g. U.K. and U.S.), is the neighbourhood food environment, i.e. the accessibility and availability of healthy foods [42, 43]. In a compact country like the Netherlands, where the average distance from home to a supermarket (in which, in general, a wide variety of healthy, good-quality food products is available against reasonable prices) is 900 m [44], we have no signs of the existence of so-called ‘food deserts’ [45], nor the existence of large inequalities in food environmental attributes between low and high socioeconomic neighbourhoods [46].

Methodological considerations

This first large-scale study investigating cultural, social and economic capital in order to quantify their role for explaining educational inequalities in healthy and unhealthy food consumption among adults has some clear strengths. We operationalised all capital variables in a theory-based way, developed indicators for cultural capital that may be more likely causally related to healthy food consumption than the more classical indicators (e.g. number of books, cultural participation [19, 28]), the sample was large with almost 3000 respondents, and multiple outcomes of food consumption were investigated. However, also limitations need to be taken into account when interpreting the results. A first limitation is the measurement of food consumption, which only provided frequency information of food products consumed, not portion sizes. Clearly such a questionnaire can only provide crude estimates of food consumption, and does not allow to calculate whether participants meet recommendations for certain intakes, e.g. fruits and vegetables, nor to calculate a score indicating the overall healthiness of a person’s diet. Therefore, this study cannot provide evidence that having more cultural capital leads to an overall more healthy diet – something that should be investigated in future research. However, analysing specific food groups as separate outcomes also has advantages. First, it allowed us to investigate to what extent certain types of capitals are more or less important for some food outcomes than for others. Secondly, this approach showed that educational inequalities are more pronounced for healthy than unhealthy food outcomes – something that would not have become clear from analysing an overall diet score.
Secondly, the measures of cultural capital were developed in a systematic way [5], however, these were framed specifically in relation to food consumption. Being more proximal to the outcome of interest may have contributed to the stronger associations of cultural capital with food consumption, compared to the more generally-framed economic and social capital measures. Thirdly, the inequalities in food consumption we report are likely an underestimation of the true inequalities, for two reasons: 1) replacement of missing values on the educational level variable in the GLOBE-2011 survey data with information from the GLOBE-2004 survey may have introduced a bias, as participants’ highest attainted educational level could have increased over time, and 2) the response to the GLOBE-2011 was relatively good (67.2%), but lower among low educated (55.5%). Lastly, this cross-sectional study cannot show insight in the direction of the associations between educational level, capital and food consumption. Acknowledging these limitations, the paper represents a novel contribution to the existing literature on educational inequalities in food consumption.

Recommendations for policy and future research

Cultural capital offers new entry-points for the promotion of healthy food consumption among low educational groups. The strong association between cultural capital and healthy food consumption implies that deeply rooted cultural resources acquired over a lifelong socialization period are relevant for food consumption. In order to improve healthy food consumption it may be important to start early in life and make healthy diets part of this socialization process, in order to, for instance, develop the broad range of skills needed for a healthy diet. Future research should investigate the specific (and causal) underlying mechanisms between educational level, cultural capital and healthy food consumption, which is needed for the development of evidence-based interventions. Especially, a better understanding is needed in the socio-cultural processes through which cultural capital is acquired, and qualitative studies are likely necessary to gain such insights. Recent work from our group (Oude Groeniger J, de Koster W, van der Waal J, Mackenbach JP, Kamphuis CBM, van Lenthe FJ: How does cultural capital make you thin? Exploring cultural signifiers that explain the relationship between cultural capital and body mass index, submitted) points to the importance of cultural signifiers (i.e. asceticism, refinement, reflexivity) as mechanisms between cultural capital and maintaining a healthy weight.

Conclusion

Cultural capital is related to healthy food consumption and contributes to the explanation of educational inequalities in healthy food consumption, over and above economic and social capital. The socialisation processes through which cultural capital is acquired could offer new entry-points for the promotion of healthy food consumption among low educational groups.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Tessa Jansen for her support with the data collection for this study.

Funding

The study was supported by a grant from the Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development (grant number 200500005). The Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to privacy regulations, but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki. The use of personal data in the GLOBE study is in compliance with the Dutch Personal Data Protection Act and the Municipal Database Act, and has been registered with the Dutch Data Protection Authority (number 1248943). No formal approval of the medical ethics committee of the Erasmus University Medical Centre was required for the study.
N.A.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Ball K, Crawford D, Mishra G. Socio-economic inequalities in women’s fruit and vegetable intakes: a multilevel study of individual, social and environmental mediators. Public Health Nutr. 2006;9(5):623–30.CrossRef Ball K, Crawford D, Mishra G. Socio-economic inequalities in women’s fruit and vegetable intakes: a multilevel study of individual, social and environmental mediators. Public Health Nutr. 2006;9(5):623–30.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Giskes K, Van Lenthe FJ, Kamphuis CBM, Huisman M, Brug J, Mackenbach JP. Household and food shopping environments: do they play a role in socioeconomic inequalities in fruit and vegetable consumption? A multilevel study among Dutch adults. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2009;63(2):113–20.CrossRef Giskes K, Van Lenthe FJ, Kamphuis CBM, Huisman M, Brug J, Mackenbach JP. Household and food shopping environments: do they play a role in socioeconomic inequalities in fruit and vegetable consumption? A multilevel study among Dutch adults. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2009;63(2):113–20.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Olstad DL, Leech RM, Livingstone KM, Ball K, Thomas B, Potter J, et al. Are dietary inequalities among Australian adults changing? A nationally representative analysis of dietary change according to socioeconomic position between 1995 and 2011–13. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2018;15(1):30.CrossRef Olstad DL, Leech RM, Livingstone KM, Ball K, Thomas B, Potter J, et al. Are dietary inequalities among Australian adults changing? A nationally representative analysis of dietary change according to socioeconomic position between 1995 and 2011–13. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2018;15(1):30.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Koc H, van Kippersluis H. Thought for food: nutritional information and educational disparities in diet. J Hum Cap. 2017;11(4):508–52.CrossRef Koc H, van Kippersluis H. Thought for food: nutritional information and educational disparities in diet. J Hum Cap. 2017;11(4):508–52.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Kamphuis CB, Jansen T, Mackenbach JP, van Lenthe FJ. Bourdieu’s cultural Capital in Relation to food choices: a systematic review of cultural capital indicators and an empirical proof of concept. PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0130695.CrossRef Kamphuis CB, Jansen T, Mackenbach JP, van Lenthe FJ. Bourdieu’s cultural Capital in Relation to food choices: a systematic review of cultural capital indicators and an empirical proof of concept. PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0130695.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Giskes K, Avendano M, Brug J, Kunst AE. A systematic review of studies on socioeconomic inequalities in dietary intakes associated with weight gain and overweight/obesity conducted among European adults. Obes Rev. 2010;11(6):413–29.CrossRef Giskes K, Avendano M, Brug J, Kunst AE. A systematic review of studies on socioeconomic inequalities in dietary intakes associated with weight gain and overweight/obesity conducted among European adults. Obes Rev. 2010;11(6):413–29.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Beauchamp A, Backholer K, Magliano D, Peeters A. The effect of obesity prevention interventions according to socioeconomic position: a systematic review. Obes Rev. 2014;15(7):541–54.CrossRef Beauchamp A, Backholer K, Magliano D, Peeters A. The effect of obesity prevention interventions according to socioeconomic position: a systematic review. Obes Rev. 2014;15(7):541–54.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Hillier-Brown FC, Bambra CL, Cairns JM, Kasim A, Moore HJ, Summerbell CD. A systematic review of the effectiveness of individual, community and societal level interventions at reducing socioeconomic inequalities in obesity amongst children. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:834.CrossRef Hillier-Brown FC, Bambra CL, Cairns JM, Kasim A, Moore HJ, Summerbell CD. A systematic review of the effectiveness of individual, community and societal level interventions at reducing socioeconomic inequalities in obesity amongst children. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:834.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Darmon N, Drewnowski A. Contribution of food prices and diet cost to socioeconomic disparities in diet quality and health: a systematic review and analysis. Nutr Rev. 2015;73(10):643–60.CrossRef Darmon N, Drewnowski A. Contribution of food prices and diet cost to socioeconomic disparities in diet quality and health: a systematic review and analysis. Nutr Rev. 2015;73(10):643–60.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Giskes K, Van Lenthe FJ, Brug J, Mackenbach JP, Turrell G. Socioeconomic inequalities in food purchasing: the contribution of respondent-perceived and actual (objectively measured) price and availability of foods. Prev Med. 2007;45(1):41–8.CrossRef Giskes K, Van Lenthe FJ, Brug J, Mackenbach JP, Turrell G. Socioeconomic inequalities in food purchasing: the contribution of respondent-perceived and actual (objectively measured) price and availability of foods. Prev Med. 2007;45(1):41–8.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Conklin AI, Forouhi NG, Surtees P, Khaw KT, Wareham NJ, Monsivais P. Social relationships and healthful dietary behaviour: evidence from over-50s in the EPIC cohort, UK. Soc Sci Med. 2014;100:167–75.CrossRef Conklin AI, Forouhi NG, Surtees P, Khaw KT, Wareham NJ, Monsivais P. Social relationships and healthful dietary behaviour: evidence from over-50s in the EPIC cohort, UK. Soc Sci Med. 2014;100:167–75.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Ball K, Jeffery RW, Abbott G, McNaughton SA, Crawford D. Is healthy behavior contagious: associations of social norms with physical activity and healthy eating. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7:86.CrossRef Ball K, Jeffery RW, Abbott G, McNaughton SA, Crawford D. Is healthy behavior contagious: associations of social norms with physical activity and healthy eating. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7:86.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Lindstrom M, Hanson BS, Wirfalt E, Ostergren PO. Socioeconomic differences in the consumption of vegetables, fruit and fruit juices. The influence of psychosocial factors. Eur J Pub Health. 2001;11(1):51–9.CrossRef Lindstrom M, Hanson BS, Wirfalt E, Ostergren PO. Socioeconomic differences in the consumption of vegetables, fruit and fruit juices. The influence of psychosocial factors. Eur J Pub Health. 2001;11(1):51–9.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Abel T. Cultural capital and social inequality in health. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008;62(7):e13.CrossRef Abel T. Cultural capital and social inequality in health. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008;62(7):e13.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Khawaja M, Mowafi M. Cultural capital and self-rated health in low income women: evidence from the urban health study, Beirut, Lebanon. J Urban Health. 2006;83(3):444–58.CrossRef Khawaja M, Mowafi M. Cultural capital and self-rated health in low income women: evidence from the urban health study, Beirut, Lebanon. J Urban Health. 2006;83(3):444–58.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Rozin P. Food choice: an introduction. In: Frewer L, van Trijp H, editors. Understanding consumers of food products. Cambridge: Woodhead Publishing Limited; 2007. Rozin P. Food choice: an introduction. In: Frewer L, van Trijp H, editors. Understanding consumers of food products. Cambridge: Woodhead Publishing Limited; 2007.
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Rozin P. The meaning of food in our lives: a cross-cultural perspective on eating and well-being. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2005;37 Suppl 2:S107–12.CrossRef Rozin P. The meaning of food in our lives: a cross-cultural perspective on eating and well-being. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2005;37 Suppl 2:S107–12.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Fismen AS, Samdal O, Torsheim T. Family affluence and cultural capital as indicators of social inequalities in adolescent’s eating behaviours: a population-based survey. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:1036.CrossRef Fismen AS, Samdal O, Torsheim T. Family affluence and cultural capital as indicators of social inequalities in adolescent’s eating behaviours: a population-based survey. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:1036.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Christensen VT, Carpiano RM. Social class differences in BMI among Danish women: applying Cockerham’s health lifestyles approach and Bourdieu’s theory of lifestyle. Soc Sci Med. 2014;112:12–21.CrossRef Christensen VT, Carpiano RM. Social class differences in BMI among Danish women: applying Cockerham’s health lifestyles approach and Bourdieu’s theory of lifestyle. Soc Sci Med. 2014;112:12–21.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat De Clercq B, Abel T, Moor I, Elgar FJ, Lievens J, Sioen I, et al. Social inequality in adolescents’ healthy food intake: the interplay between economic, social and cultural capital. Eur J Pub Health. 2017;27(2):279–86. De Clercq B, Abel T, Moor I, Elgar FJ, Lievens J, Sioen I, et al. Social inequality in adolescents’ healthy food intake: the interplay between economic, social and cultural capital. Eur J Pub Health. 2017;27(2):279–86.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Bourdieu P. Distinction. A social critique of the judgement of taste. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1984. Bourdieu P. Distinction. A social critique of the judgement of taste. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1984.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Bourdieu P. The forms of capital. In: Richardson R, editor. Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of education. New York: Greenwood Press; 1986. Bourdieu P. The forms of capital. In: Richardson R, editor. Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of education. New York: Greenwood Press; 1986.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Lareau A, Weininger EB. Cultural capital in educational research: a critical assessment. Theor Soc. 2003;32(5–6):567–606.CrossRef Lareau A, Weininger EB. Cultural capital in educational research: a critical assessment. Theor Soc. 2003;32(5–6):567–606.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat van Lenthe FJ, Kamphuis CB, Beenackers MA, Jansen T, Looman CW, Nusselder WJ, et al. Cohort profile: understanding socioeconomic inequalities in health and health behaviours: the GLOBE study. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(3):721–30.CrossRef van Lenthe FJ, Kamphuis CB, Beenackers MA, Jansen T, Looman CW, Nusselder WJ, et al. Cohort profile: understanding socioeconomic inequalities in health and health behaviours: the GLOBE study. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(3):721–30.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Bovens M, Dekker P, Tiemeijer W. Gescheiden werelden. Den Haag: SCP en WRR; 2014. Bovens M, Dekker P, Tiemeijer W. Gescheiden werelden. Den Haag: SCP en WRR; 2014.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat van de Werfhorst H. Een kloof van alle tijden. Verschillen tussen lager en hoger opgeleiden in werk, cultuur en politiek. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press; 2015. van de Werfhorst H. Een kloof van alle tijden. Verschillen tussen lager en hoger opgeleiden in werk, cultuur en politiek. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press; 2015.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Berkel-Van Schaik AB, Tax B. Towards a standard operationalisation of socioeconomic status for epidemiological and socio-medical research [in Dutch]. Rijswijk: Ministerie van WVC; 1990. Van Berkel-Van Schaik AB, Tax B. Towards a standard operationalisation of socioeconomic status for epidemiological and socio-medical research [in Dutch]. Rijswijk: Ministerie van WVC; 1990.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Bogers RP, Van Assema P, Kester AD, Westerterp KR, Dagnelie PC. Reproducibility, validity, and responsiveness to change of a short questionnaire for measuring fruit and vegetable intake. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159(9):900–9.CrossRef Bogers RP, Van Assema P, Kester AD, Westerterp KR, Dagnelie PC. Reproducibility, validity, and responsiveness to change of a short questionnaire for measuring fruit and vegetable intake. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159(9):900–9.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Drewnowski A, Hann C. Food preferences and reported frequencies of food consumption as predictors of current diet in young women. Am J Clin Nutr. 1999;70(1):28–36.CrossRef Drewnowski A, Hann C. Food preferences and reported frequencies of food consumption as predictors of current diet in young women. Am J Clin Nutr. 1999;70(1):28–36.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat van Genugten L, van Empelen P, Flink I, Oenema A. Systematic development of a self-regulation weight-management intervention for overweight adults. BMC Public Health. 2010;10:649.CrossRef van Genugten L, van Empelen P, Flink I, Oenema A. Systematic development of a self-regulation weight-management intervention for overweight adults. BMC Public Health. 2010;10:649.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Pollard TM, Steptoe A, Wardle J. Motives underlying healthy eating: using the food choice questionnaire to explain variation in dietary intake. J Biosoc Sci. 1998;30(2):165–79.CrossRef Pollard TM, Steptoe A, Wardle J. Motives underlying healthy eating: using the food choice questionnaire to explain variation in dietary intake. J Biosoc Sci. 1998;30(2):165–79.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Sun X, Rehnberg C, Meng Q. How are individual-level social capital and poverty associated with health equity? A study from two Chinese cities. Int J Equity Health. 2009;8:2.CrossRef Sun X, Rehnberg C, Meng Q. How are individual-level social capital and poverty associated with health equity? A study from two Chinese cities. Int J Equity Health. 2009;8:2.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Oort FV, Van Lenthe FJ, Mackenbach JP. Material, psychosocial, and behavioural factors in the explanation of educational inequalities in mortality in the Netherlands. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2005;59(3):214–20.CrossRef Van Oort FV, Van Lenthe FJ, Mackenbach JP. Material, psychosocial, and behavioural factors in the explanation of educational inequalities in mortality in the Netherlands. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2005;59(3):214–20.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Barros AJ, Hirakata VN. Alternatives for logistic regression in cross-sectional studies: an empirical comparison of models that directly estimate the prevalence ratio. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2003;3:21.CrossRef Barros AJ, Hirakata VN. Alternatives for logistic regression in cross-sectional studies: an empirical comparison of models that directly estimate the prevalence ratio. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2003;3:21.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Martinez BAF, Leotti VB, Silva GSE, Nunes LN, Machado G, Corbellini LG. Odds Ratio or Prevalence Ratio? An Overview of Reported Statistical Methods and Appropriateness of Interpretations in Cross-sectional Studies with Dichotomous Outcomes in Veterinary Medicine. Front Vet Sci. 2017;4:193.CrossRef Martinez BAF, Leotti VB, Silva GSE, Nunes LN, Machado G, Corbellini LG. Odds Ratio or Prevalence Ratio? An Overview of Reported Statistical Methods and Appropriateness of Interpretations in Cross-sectional Studies with Dichotomous Outcomes in Veterinary Medicine. Front Vet Sci. 2017;4:193.CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Oude Groeniger J, van Lenthe FJ, Beenackers MA, Kamphuis CB. Does social distinction contribute to socioeconomic inequalities in diet: the case of ‘superfoods’ consumption. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2017;14(1):40.CrossRef Oude Groeniger J, van Lenthe FJ, Beenackers MA, Kamphuis CB. Does social distinction contribute to socioeconomic inequalities in diet: the case of ‘superfoods’ consumption. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2017;14(1):40.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Kamphuis CB, de Bekker-Grob EW, van Lenthe FJ. Factors affecting food choices of older adults from high and low socioeconomic groups: a discrete choice experiment. Am J Clin Nutr. 2015;101(4):768–74.CrossRef Kamphuis CB, de Bekker-Grob EW, van Lenthe FJ. Factors affecting food choices of older adults from high and low socioeconomic groups: a discrete choice experiment. Am J Clin Nutr. 2015;101(4):768–74.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Kamphuis CB, van Lenthe FJ, Giskes K, Brug J, Mackenbach JP. Perceived environmental determinants of physical activity and fruit and vegetable consumption among high and low socioeconomic groups in the Netherlands. Health Place. 2007;13(2):493–503.CrossRef Kamphuis CB, van Lenthe FJ, Giskes K, Brug J, Mackenbach JP. Perceived environmental determinants of physical activity and fruit and vegetable consumption among high and low socioeconomic groups in the Netherlands. Health Place. 2007;13(2):493–503.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker RE, Keane CR, Burke JG. Disparities and access to healthy food in the United States: a review of food deserts literature. Health Place. 2010;16(5):876–84.CrossRef Walker RE, Keane CR, Burke JG. Disparities and access to healthy food in the United States: a review of food deserts literature. Health Place. 2010;16(5):876–84.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Black C, Moon G, Baird J. Dietary inequalities: what is the evidence for the effect of the neighbourhood food environment? Health Place. 2014;27:229–42.CrossRef Black C, Moon G, Baird J. Dietary inequalities: what is the evidence for the effect of the neighbourhood food environment? Health Place. 2014;27:229–42.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Helbich M, Schadenberg B, Hagenauer J, Poelman M. Food deserts? Healthy food access in Amsterdam. Appl Geogr. 2017;83:1–12.CrossRef Helbich M, Schadenberg B, Hagenauer J, Poelman M. Food deserts? Healthy food access in Amsterdam. Appl Geogr. 2017;83:1–12.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Timmermans J, Dijkstra C, Kamphuis C, Huitink M, van der Zee E, Poelman M. ‘Obesogenic’ School Food Environments? An Urban Case Study in The Netherlands. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(4):619.CrossRef Timmermans J, Dijkstra C, Kamphuis C, Huitink M, van der Zee E, Poelman M. ‘Obesogenic’ School Food Environments? An Urban Case Study in The Netherlands. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(4):619.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Does cultural capital contribute to educational inequalities in food consumption in the Netherlands? A cross-sectional analysis of the GLOBE-2011 survey
verfasst von
Carlijn B. M. Kamphuis
Joost Oude Groeniger
Frank J. van Lenthe
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2018
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
International Journal for Equity in Health / Ausgabe 1/2018
Elektronische ISSN: 1475-9276
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12939-018-0884-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

International Journal for Equity in Health 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe