Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Research article

Does dissatisfaction with, or accurate perception of overweight status help people reduce weight? Longitudinal study of Australian adults

verfasst von: Xiaoqi Feng, Andrew Wilson

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

With studies around the world suggesting a large proportion of people do not recognise that they are overweight (or feel satisfied with being overweight), this fuels the view that such ‘misperceptions’ need to be ‘corrected’. However, few longitudinal studies have examined the consequences of under-perceived weight status, nor over-perceived weight status (when a person feels overweight when they are not) and weight-related satisfaction on trajectories in body mass index (BMI).

Methods

Five-year BMI trajectories were examined among 8174 participants in an Australian nationally representative cohort. Each person was classified into groups according to their neighbourhood socioeconomic circumstances, baseline BMI and answers to “how satisfied are you with your current weight?” and “do you consider yourself to be… acceptable weight / underweight / overweight?” Gender-specific multilevel linear regressions were used to examine five-year BMI trajectories for people in each group, adjusting for potential confounders.

Results

At baseline, weight-related dissatisfaction and perceived overweight were generally associated with higher mean BMI for men and women, regardless of whether they were classified as ‘normal’ or overweight by World Health Organization (WHO) criteria. Mean BMI did not decrease among people classified as overweight who perceived themselves as overweight, or expressed weight-related dissatisfaction, regardless of where they lived. Among men and women with ‘normal’ BMI at baseline but expressing weight-related dissatisfaction, mean BMI increased disproportionately among those living in disadvantaged areas compared to their counterparts in affluent areas. Similarly, mean BMI rose disproportionately among people in disadvantaged areas who felt they were overweight despite having a ‘normal’ BMI by WHO criteria, compared to people with the same over-perceptions living in affluent areas. These differences exacerbated pre-existing socioeconomic inequities in mean BMI.

Conclusions

No evidence was found to suggest accurate recognition of overweight or expressing weight-related dissatisfaction leads to a lower BMI. However, there was evidence of an increase in mean BMI among people who felt dissatisfied with, or over-perceived their ‘normal’ weight, especially in socioeconomically disadvantaged areas. Correction of under-perceptions may not drive weight loss, but circumstances contributing to over-perception and dissatisfaction with weight status may contribute to increased weight gain and exacerbate socioeconomic inequities in BMI.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12889-019-6938-3) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Abkürzungen
ABS
Australian Bureau of Statistics
ARIA
Accessibility/Remoteness Index of Australia
BMI
Body mass index
CCD
Census Collection Districts
HILDA
Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia
SEIFA
Socio Economic Indices For Areas
US
United States
WHO
World Health Organization

Background

Weight gain among people living in disadvantaged neighbourhoods in high-income countries is comparatively higher and begins earlier than their peers in more affluent areas, especially for women [13]. Some longitudinal studies of adults in Australia [4] and the US [5] report comparatively greater weight gain over time among people who considered their weight status as ‘normal’, but were actually overweight or obese by World Health Organization (WHO) criteria. Evidence suggests under-perception of weight status is common [613]. Conventional wisdom suggests that correction of this ‘under-perception’ is a pre-requisite for behavioural change to achieve weight loss, or the slowing of the rate of weight gained over time [14, 15]. Evidence indicates there has been increased efforts by health professionals to correct under-perception of weight status in the US over the last 15 years [16].
However, some evidence suggests that under-perception of overweight status may be sometimes favourable. A recent systematic review found evidence to indicate that people who perceive their weight status as overweight were likely to gain more weight over time, despite also being more likely to attempt weight loss behaviours [17]. Studies of children have reported under-perception of overweight status to be associated with lower future weight gain [18] and lower blood pressure trajectories [19]. Meanwhile, there are some other studies that report problems with over-perception, or when people of ‘normal’ weight by WHO criteria perceive themselves as overweight or obese. Over-perception is common in some groups [20] and has been linked to future weight gain in adults [21, 22] and also contributing factors, such as increased psychological distress [23]. Similar findings have been reported in children and adolescents [24, 25]. Rather than being detrimental, under-perception of weight status may sometimes be protective (or indicative of the presence of some other protective factor, such as positive affect) against weight-related stigma and media-driven portrayals of idealised body size, which have been suggested to lead to heightened stress and maladaptive behaviours that contribute to weight gain [26, 27].
Satisfaction with current weight status has also been suggested to play a role in determining future weight loss. Studies have shown that body dissatisfaction and concern over weight and body shape intensify across adolescence [28], but then remain largely stable (particularly for women) for the rest of the lifecourse, despite physical changes associated with ageing [29]. Much of the research on body satisfaction and weight gain has been conducted on adolescents, with many studies finding more weight-related dissatisfaction among overweight adolescents to be predictive of future weight gain and maladaptive behaviours such as binge eating [3034]. These findings run counter to belief that dissatisfaction with overweight status is a necessary precursor for positive change.
The abovementioned evidence provides interesting but conflicting perspectives on whether correction of under-perceived weight status ought to be mainstream policy. On one hand, correction of under-perception may be seen to enable people to take action and perhaps, in some contexts, receive additional support services (e.g. enrolment in a behavioural change program). But on the other, the psychosocial stress associated with being labelled as overweight or obese, even if a person is overweight by WHO criteria, may have unintended consequences, such as compounding body dissatisfaction. Since the majority of evidence is from the US, more studies are warranted to understand if either of these duelling hypotheses are supported in other contexts. It is possible that evidence for both hypotheses within the same national context may be available due to heterogeneity of experience between population sub-groups, such as well-known differences in weight-related stigma and discrimination experienced between men and women [35, 36].
Furthermore, it is plausible that different experiences will manifest across strata of area-level socioeconomic circumstances due to variation in the visual normalisation of overweight status [37]. In communities where being overweight or obese by WHO criteria is the norm, visual normalisation theory suggests that the BMI threshold by which people judge themselves to be overweight is raised [37]. In many countries the prevalence of overweight and obesity is at or above 60%, and within those countries overweight and obesity tend to be higher in socioeconomically disadvantaged areas [38]. This suggests the possibility of visual normalisation patterned geographically within countries. Recent studies report under-perception is more common among adults in socioeconomically disadvantaged areas [39] and in children of families with low educational attainment [40], perhaps because of visual normalisation [37] of larger body sizes. This may exacerbate the effect of other factors that contribute to the ‘obesogenic’ environment in disadvantaged areas, such as higher ratios of unhealthy to healthy food outlets [41] and visual cues that stimulate appetite for unhealthy food [42].
Accordingly, the purpose of this study was to examine five-year trajectories in body mass index (BMI) among male and female Australian adults stratified by their WHO criteria-defined weight status at baseline. Differences in BMI trajectories in each of these groups were examined in relation to each person’s perception of and satisfaction with their baseline weight status, as well as the potential for effect modification by area-level socioeconomic circumstances.

Methods

Data

Data analysed in this study was extracted from the “Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia” (HILDA). Details of HILDA are already published [43]. In brief, HILDA is a nationally representative sample of approximately 15,000 individuals in 7000 households collected annually. A longitudinal sample of 4386 men and 3788 women aged > 15 years with complete BMI data in 2007 and 2012 was selected. These waves were selected as 2007 was the first year in which questions of perceived weight and weight-related satisfaction were asked, while data from 2012 was the most recent wave that was available at the time of analysis. Participants considered ‘underweight’ by WHO criteria (BMI < 18.5 kg/m2) were omitted as our focus was on contrasting people who were overweight or obese (BMI > 25 kg/m2) with those classified as ‘normal’ (BMI > 18.5 kg/m2 and < 25 kg/m2).

Body mass index and initial weight status

Self-reported height and weight were used to calculate BMI for each participant. BMI was considered in its continuous form for the outcome variable. Initial weight status was the BMI category at baseline (in 2007).
Two questions on weight-related perceptions were asked in the 2007 survey. The first asked “How satisfied are you with your current weight”. Answers were ‘very satisfied’, ‘satisfied’, ‘neither satisfied nor dissatisfied’, ‘dissatisfied’, ‘very dissatisfied’ or ‘refused/not stated’. We classified these answers into ‘dissatisfied’ (‘dissatisfied’, ‘very dissatisfied’), ‘not dissatisfied/ambivalent’ (‘very satisfied’, ‘satisfied’, ‘neither satisfied nor dissatisfied’) or ‘other’ (‘refused/not stated’). The second indicator was self-rated weight status, as follows: “Do you consider yourself to be… acceptable weight / underweight / overweight?” Answers were classified into ‘acceptable’, ‘overweight’ or ‘other’.

Neighbourhood socioeconomic disadvantage

The Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS) Socio Economic Indices For Areas (SEIFA) was used to measure neighbourhood socioeconomic disadvantage. The index selected for this study was of relative disadvantage, a composite indicator derived via principal components analysis by the Australian Bureau of Statistics to summarise multiple census data on income, education, employment, occupation, housing and other indicators of relative disadvantage (e.g. no car ownership) [44]. Lower values on this index denote an increasing concentration of disadvantaged people. As our focus was on disadvantaged communities and required stratification for purposes of comparing associations between weight change and weight-related (mis)perceptions across different levels of neighbourhood socioeconomic circumstances, this variable was inverted and then classified into tertiles so that higher strata denoted more disadvantaged areas. Our previous work [3, 45, 46] and that of others internationally [47] has shown that residents (especially women) in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhoods tend to have higher BMI on average and independent of related factors such as educational attainment, employment status and household income. In this study, this measure of neighbourhood socioeconomic circumstances was utilised as a potential effect modifier, to reveal plausible differences in weight change trajectories over time between people living in disadvantaged and affluent areas with different stated perceptions of their weight at baseline.

Potential confounding variables

A range of variables were identified to reduce probable sources of confounding based upon a synthesis of previous literature [4749]. These included age, whether a participant was living on their own or as part of a couple (married or cohabiting), the highest level of education achieved (less than high school, high school to advanced diploma, university or higher), average household gross income (expressed in quintiles), the percentage of time in the last year spent unemployed, and geographic remoteness. Geographic remoteness was measured using the Accessibility/Remoteness Index of Australia (ARIA) [50], which helped to distinguish between participants living in urban areas (defined by the “major city” category) and or regional and remote areas (defined by those living in “inner regional”, “outer regional”, “remote” and “very remote” areas of Australia).

Analytical strategy

The sample was described using cross-tabulations and mean BMIs at baseline and follow-up for each covariate. Multilevel linear regression models were used to investigate associations between BMI trajectories between baseline and follow-up with respect to neighbourhood disadvantage, initial weight status and the perceived weight status variables, adjusting for confounders. This was implemented by cross-classifying actual weight status at baseline with the weight-related perception variables, then fitting two-way interaction terms followed by a three-way interaction term between this cross-classified actual vs. perceived weight variable, neighbourhood disadvantage and time. These models were fitted separately for men and women due to known differences in risk of experiencing weight-related discrimination. The multilevel models were used to take account clustering of participants (level 1) within households (level 2) and areas of residence (level 3) as reported at baseline. Neighbourhoods were defined as Census Collection Districts (‘CCDs’), which are small areas containing approximately 225 residential dwellings on average. Results were presented using adjusted predicted mean BMI trajectories with 95% confidence intervals (95%CIs) from these models. All analyses were conducted in MLwIN v2.30 [51]. Ethical approval for the HILDA study was obtained from the Faculty of Business and Economics Human Ethics Advisory Committee at the University of Melbourne. Approval for the use of HILDA data was provided by the Government Department of Social Services.

Results

Table 1 shows mean BMI was slightly higher in more disadvantaged neighbourhoods among men (correlation coefficient = 0.07, p < 0.0001) and especially women (0.12, p < 0.0001). BMI tended to be appreciably higher among participants reporting dissatisfaction with their weight (correlation coefficient: men = 0.37, p < 0.0001; women = 0.41, p < 0.0001) and perceiving themselves as overweight (correlation coefficient: men = 0.54, p < 0.0001; women = 0.55, p < 0.0001). Mean BMI varied by age, couple status, highest educational qualification and percentage of the previous year spent unemployed, but less so across quintiles of annual household income and the geographic remoteness of the place of residence.
Table 1
Description of the study sample at baseline (wave 7) and 5-year follow-up (wave 12)
 
Men
Women
N (%)
Body Mass Index (mean)
N
Body Mass Index (mean)
(Baseline)
Baseline
5-year follow-up
(Baseline)
Baseline
5-year follow-up
N
4386
26.9
27.4
3788
26.1
26.8
Disadvantage tertiles
 Affluent
1381 (36.5%)
26.4
26.9
1561 (35.6%)
25.2
25.9
 Average
1261 (33.3%)
27.0
27.8
1494 (34.1%)
26.2
26.9
 Disadvantaged
1146 (30.3%)
27.3
27.7
1331 (30.4%)
27.1
27.6
Dissatisfied
 No/ambivalent
2640 (69.7%)
25.6
26.3
2422 (55.2%)
23.9
24.7
 Yes
1138 (30.0%)
29.8
30.1
1940 (44.2%)
29.0
29.5
 Missing
10 (0.3%)
26.9
27.5
24 (0.6%)
25.0
26.6
Perception
 Acceptable
2174 (57.4%)
25.0
25.7
2290 (52.2%)
23.2
24.0
 Overweight
1391 (36.7%)
30.6
30.9
1942 (44.3%)
30.0
30.5
 Missing
223 (5.9%)
21.2
22.6
154 (3.5%)
20.2
21.4
Age group
 15–24
557 (14.7%)
23.9
25.2
671 (15.3%)
23.5
24.4
 25–34
549 (14.5%)
26.7
26.4
675 (15.4%)
25.8
25.9
 35–44
732 (19.3%)
27.3
27.9
851 (19.4%)
26.4
27.1
 45–54
797 (21.0%)
27.7
28.1
855 (19.5%)
26.8
27.1
 55–64
590 (15.6%)
28.1
28.2
693 (15.8%)
27.7
27.9
 65–74
404 (10.7%)
27.1
27.9
417 (9.5%)
26.5
27.5
 75+
159 (4.2%)
26.2
26.1
224 (5.1%)
26.2
26.0
Couple status
 Yes
2652 (70.0%)
27.4
27.7
2814 (64.2%)
26.4
26.9
 No
1136 (30.0%)
25.7
26.8
1571 (35.8%)
25.6
26.6
 Refused
0 (0.0%)
0.0
0.0
   
Education
 <=year 11%
1044 (27.6%)
26.8
28.0
1664 (37.9%)
26.5
27.7
 Year 12 to adv diploma%
1892 (50.0%)
27.0
27.5
1676 (38.2%)
26.2
26.8
 university
850 (22.4%)
26.6
26.8
1045 (23.8%)
25.4
25.8
 undetermined
2 (0.1%)
26.3
26.8
1 (0.0%)
24.3
23.4
Percent unemployed
 0%
3558 (93.9%)
26.9
27.4
4090 (93.3%)
26.1
26.8
 1–24%
95 (2.5%)
26.2
28.4
112 (2.6%)
25.3
25.9
 25–49%
47 (1.2%)
25.3
26.7
68 (1.6%)
25.5
28.0
 50–74%
36 (1.0%)
26.0
27.1
45 (1.0%)
28.0
25.7
 75–100%
52 (1.4%)
26.0
26.8
71 (1.6%)
27.2
27.8
Income quintiles
 1 (low)
745 (19.7%)
27.1
27.2
1082 (24.7%)
26.8
27.1
 2
840 (22.2%)
26.7
27.6
976 (22.3%)
26.3
27.1
 3
902 (23.8%)
26.8
27.4
951 (21.7%)
26.4
27.1
 4
729 (19.2%)
27.0
27.6
787 (17.9%)
25.6
26.9
 5 (high)
572 (15.1%)
26.7
27.3
590 (13.5%)
24.9
26.0
Geographic remoteness
 Major city
2338 (61.7%)
26.7
27.3
2719 (62.0%)
26.0
26.5
 Inner regional
961 (25.4%)
27.1
27.6
1112 (25.4%)
26.2
27.1
 Outer regional
430 (11.4%)
27.2
28.0
482 (11.0%)
26.9
27.5
 remote
59 (1.6%)
27.5
28.2
73 (1.7%)
26.3
27.6
Statistically significant (p < 0.001) chi-square values of the weight-related perception and satisfaction variables, both overall and for men and women separately, suggested an imperfect correlation between perceived overweight and dissatisfaction with current weight status. Table 2 shows the extent that unadjusted mean BMI for men and women at baseline varied with respect to a cross-classification of actual weight status and perception of weight. Among men with a ‘normal’ BMI by WHO criteria, those expressing dissatisfaction with their weight had a lower BMI compared to their peers who did not. The opposite pattern was observed for women of ‘normal’ BMI. The perception of being overweight was associated with higher BMI for men and women regardless of whether they were actually overweight or ‘normal’. Some of these patterns at baseline appeared to vary by neighbourhood disadvantage. For example, men expressing dissatisfaction despite having ‘normal’ weight had lower BMI if living in a disadvantaged neighbourhood (mean = 20.7 kg/m2), compared to their peers in the same BMI category also expressing dissatisfaction but living in more affluent areas (mean = 22.2 kg/m2).
Table 2
Distribution of weight status, dissatisfaction and perception of weight status and mean body mass index at baseline, by gender and neighbourhood disadvantage
 
MEN
WOMEN
 
Neighbourhood Disadvantage (tertiles)
 
Neighbourhood Disadvantage (tertiles)
Total
Affluent
Average
Disadvantaged
Total
Affluent
Average
Disadvantaged
N
BMI (mean)
N
BMI (mean)
N
BMI (mean)
N
BMI (mean)
N
BMI (mean)
N
BMI (mean)
N
BMI (mean)
N
BMI (mean)
Weight status-Dissatisfied
 Normal-No/Ambivalent
1238
22.6
488
22.6
409
22.6
341
22.5
1657
21.6
656
21.5
546
21.6
455
21.6
 Overweight-No/Ambivalent
1402
28.3
449
27.7
469
28.5
484
28.7
765
28.8
212
27.9
265
28.9
288
29.5
 Normal-Dissatisfied
154
21.6
52
22.2
49
22.0
53
20.7
514
22.7
225
22.9
170
22.6
119
22.6
 Overweight-Dissatisfied
984
31.1
390
30.2
329
31.2
265
32.1
1426
31.2
462
30.4
504
31.2
460
32.1
 Other
10
26.9
2
27.2
5
26.7
3
27.0
24
25.0
6
24.3
9
23.4
9
27.0
Weight status-Perception
 Normal-Acceptable
1134
22.8
449
22.8
376
22.8
309
22.7
1707
21.8
708
21.7
563
21.7
436
21.9
 Overweight-Acceptable
1040
27.5
338
27.1
356
27.7
346
27.7
583
27.6
167
27.0
204
27.5
212
28.1
 Normal-Overweight
59
23.7
26
23.7
19
24.1
14
22.9
341
23.4
143
23.2
114
23.5
84
23.5
 Overweight-Overweight
1332
30.9
498
30.0
436
31.2
398
31.8
1601
31.4
507
30.5
565
31.4
529
32.3
 Other
223
21.2
70
20.9
74
21.7
79
21.0
154
20.2
36
19.4
48
19.6
70
21.0
BMI Body Mass Index
Predicted mean BMI trajectories and 95% confidence intervals from gender-stratified fully adjusted multilevel models with interactions between time, neighbourhood disadvantage and the cross-classification of baseline weight status and weight-related satisfaction and are shown in Fig. 1. Figure 2 shows virtually the same models, except perceived weight status was substituted in for weight-related satisfaction. Both figures reveal a complex range of trajectories in BMI change across these groups. The multilevel models on which the means were predicted for Figs. 1 and 2 are provided in Additional file 1: Table S1 and Additional file 2: Table S2, respectively.
In Fig. 1, men and women who were already overweight did not appear to gain or lose much weight between baseline and follow-up. This was regardless of whether participants felt dissatisfied with their weight or not. In contrast, Fig. 1 also shows gains in weight among men and women who began the study having ‘normal’ weight by WHO criteria. These gains were observed for persons who felt dissatisfied with their weight and those who did not. These patterns were consistent across strata of neighbourhood socioeconomic disadvantage.
In Fig. 2, as with Fig. 1, people who were overweight at baseline tended not to gain or lose weight on average across the 5 years. Weight gain was seen among people who had a ‘normal’ BMI by WHO criteria at baseline. Weight gain was observed for people who perceived themselves as being overweight, especially among men and women in disadvantaged neighbourhoods. People who felt their weight was acceptable gained weight.

Discussion

In our study, the first notable finding is that among people who were overweight or obese at baseline, people did not gain or lose weight (on average) regardless of whether they perceived themselves as overweight or not. The same results were observed when focussing on whether a person expressed dissatisfaction with their weight at baseline. Second, weight gain was more common for men and women who had a ‘normal’ weight status at baseline by WHO criteria, but a little more rapid for those who perceived themselves to be overweight or dissatisfied with their weight. Third, this weight gain among people who over-perceived their ‘normal’ weight status was greater for those living in disadvantaged neighbourhoods.
Our findings reflect an interplay between actual weight status, perceived weight status and neighbourhood disadvantage. It is not fully clear at the present time what explains these findings, though it may be helpful to rule some explanations out. First, people who under-perceived their overweight did not substantially gain more weight, though neither did they appear to lose weight on average. Some previous studies have suggested the possibility of a protective mis-perception of weight status among children [18, 19], wherein under-perception of overweight may help reduce the probability of gaining weight over time (or be indicative of other, unmeasured, protective factors). As little change occurred among people who were overweight regardless of their perception or level of satisfaction, this does not provide evidence to support the protective mis-perception hypothesis. Nor does it provide evidence to support the mainstreaming of attempts to correct for under-perceptions of overweight status. Additionally, visual normalisation of overweight may not only make it acceptable within certain contexts to be heavier by raising the lower threshold of what is considered overweight [5254], but it too may also provide an adjusted upper threshold as to what is generally felt to be too heavy for most people. This is an area that needs further research.
Our study does provide evidence for potentially correcting over-perceptions of weight status among people with a ‘normal’ BMI according to WHO criteria. Previous work has identified that people who perceive themselves as overweight have a higher probability of going on to gain weight [17], though fewer studies have differentiated between people who had a ‘normal’ BMI by WHO criteria from people who were actually overweight. A range of factors may be at play. First, regular exposure to media and advertising-driven portrayals of ideal body shapes and weights may be one reason why people may perceive themselves as overweight when they are not. Previous work has suggested that both men and (especially) women, tend to feel less satisfied with their bodies or be more likely to consider themselves overweight after viewing pictures of thin people (e.g. on television) [5560].
These negative comparisons may spur a range of possible responses, some of which may be patterned by neighbourhood socioeconomic circumstances. One being emotional decision-making and engagement in maladaptive behaviours [26, 27] associated with ‘future discounting’. This is when people trade off the long-term consequences of alcohol and binge eating food packed with sugar and carbohydrates to satisfy short-term needs to mask feelings of shame, hopelessness and a lack of control [61, 62]. It is perhaps more likely to occur among people in disadvantaged areas who may feel they have few other options, while being encouraged by local obesogenic food environment providing numerous cues and opportunities to purchase junk food [41, 42]. By contrast, it is known that people in more affluent circumstances tend to have more healthier food environment (or at least healthier options) and be more likely to monitor their weight and engage in diet management [63].
Comfort eating of calorific food is compounded by the release of glucocorticoids that increases a person’s appetite for food that gives them pleasure under stressful situations [64, 65]. It is known that people in disadvantaged neighbourhoods experience chronic levels of stress more often than their peers in more affluent circumstances [66]. Although there were no consistent gender differences found in our study, women may be especially vulnerable to maladaptive behaviours [6769], since there is greater value placed on female physical attractiveness [70] and a lower tolerance over minor weight gain among women than there is for men [7173].
Further socioeconomic patterning may be driven by who or what people feel is the driver of their over-perceived weight status. An experimental study showed people with heavier BMI living in more affluent circumstances were more willing to excuse their body weight as something that was not entirely within their control, whereas people with higher BMI in more disadvantaged surroundings were more likely to blame themselves for their weight status [70]. Evidence also suggests that as body size increases, which will have been more dramatic among those in our study who had ‘normal’ BMI at baseline, women (but not men) tend to disconnect from social activities [74, 75], potentially increasing the risk of social isolation and associated behaviours such as eating disorders [76]. All of these factors may help to explain greater weight gain among people who over-perceived their weight status, and especially those in disadvantaged areas, though further research is warranted.
The study benefits from panel data over a 5-year period containing rich information, including a range of socioeconomic and demographic variables on a large number of men and women stratified by neighbourhood socioeconomic circumstances, WHO-defined BMI category and whether they perceived themselves as overweight and/or felt dissatisfied with their weight status. The longitudinal design afforded insights into different mean BMI trajectories over 5 years across all of these groups, potentially for the first time within a single study. The multilevel approach permitted a disentangling of effects between people and their places of residence and allowed assessment of change in BMI, while taking into account variation in mean baseline values between each group.
Some of the limitations and areas for future study include small sample sizes for ethnic groups, which would have been interesting to examine given prior work suggesting variations in weight-related perceptions between people of different ethnicities and countries of birth [7780]. Another limitation is the known underreporting of weight and over-reporting of height [81], which means that the BMI variable used in our study can really only be considered a proxy for actual weight status. Studies that can replicate our longitudinal design but incorporate objectively measured BMI and/or other relevant outcome variables such as waist circumference and percentage abdominal fat would prove valuable next steps. It is important to note that this study is based upon observational data in which none of the perceptions of weight status, nor other variables analysed such as neighbourhood disadvantage, can be considered randomly assigned. As such, some residual confounding may remain even after multivariate adjustment. Finally, it would also be novel to examine not only whether mean BMI trajectories continue over a longer time-period among the groups in our study, but also to see at what point perceptions of weight change (if at all) over time.

Conclusions

The findings of this study indicate, if interpreted in a causal sense and with caution, that practices to correct weight-related misperceptions may not be effective in promoting weight loss. We observed no weight loss (or gain) on average among people already overweight or obese at baseline regardless of their weight-related perception or satisfaction. In contrast, people who had a ‘normal’ weight at baseline by WHO criteria gained weight on average, especially among those who were dissatisfied with their weight, or who perceived themselves as overweight, in disadvantaged neighbourhoods. Randomised trials are warranted to examine the extent that these findings are robust to experimental design.

Acknowledgements

This paper uses unit record data from the HILDA Survey. The HILDA Project was initiated and is funded by the Australian Government Department of Social Services (DSS), and is managed by the Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research (Melbourne Institute). The findings and views reported in this paper, however, are those of the authors and should not be attributed to either the DSS or the Melbourne Institute. The Australian Prevention Partnership Centre is funded by the NHMRC, the Australian, NSW and ACT health departments, and the HCF Health and Medical Research Foundation.

Funding

No funding was received for this study.

Availability of data and materials

Data was accessed under a licence the result of a peer-reviewed application. Data is otherwise confidential and cannot be released by the authors. Those interested in accessing the data must contact the Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research, Level 5, Business and Economics Building, 111 Barry Street, The University of Melbourne, Victoria 3010 Australia, Tel + 61 3 8344 2108, Email hildainquiries@unimelb.​edu.​au.
Ethical approval for HILDA was obtained from the Faculty of Business and Economics Human Ethics Advisory Committee at the University of Melbourne. Approval for the use of HILDA data was provided by the Government Department of Social Services.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Coogan PF, Cozier YC, Krishnan S, Wise LA, Adams-Campbell LL, Rosenberg L, Palmer JR. Neighborhood socioeconomic status in relation to 10-year weight gain in the black Women's health study. Obesity. 2010;18(10):2064–5.CrossRef Coogan PF, Cozier YC, Krishnan S, Wise LA, Adams-Campbell LL, Rosenberg L, Palmer JR. Neighborhood socioeconomic status in relation to 10-year weight gain in the black Women's health study. Obesity. 2010;18(10):2064–5.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Stafford M, Brunner EJ, Head J, Ross NA. Deprivation and the development of obesity: a multilevel, longitudinal study in England. Am J Prev Med. 2010;39(2):130–9.CrossRef Stafford M, Brunner EJ, Head J, Ross NA. Deprivation and the development of obesity: a multilevel, longitudinal study in England. Am J Prev Med. 2010;39(2):130–9.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Feng X, Wilson A. Getting bigger, quicker? Gendered socioeconomic trajectories in body mass index across the adult Lifecourse: a longitudinal study of 21,403 Australians. PLoS One. 2015;10(10):e0141499.CrossRef Feng X, Wilson A. Getting bigger, quicker? Gendered socioeconomic trajectories in body mass index across the adult Lifecourse: a longitudinal study of 21,403 Australians. PLoS One. 2015;10(10):e0141499.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Hernan AL, Versace VL, Laatikainen T, Vartiainen E, Janus ED, Dunbar JA. Association of weight misperception with weight loss in a diabetes prevention program. BMC Public Health. 2014;14(1):93.CrossRef Hernan AL, Versace VL, Laatikainen T, Vartiainen E, Janus ED, Dunbar JA. Association of weight misperception with weight loss in a diabetes prevention program. BMC Public Health. 2014;14(1):93.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Herring SJ, Oken E, Haines J, Rich-Edwards JW, Rifas-Shiman SL, Gillman MW. Misperceived pre-pregnancy body weight status predicts excessive gestational weight gain: findings from a US cohort study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2008;8(1):54.CrossRef Herring SJ, Oken E, Haines J, Rich-Edwards JW, Rifas-Shiman SL, Gillman MW. Misperceived pre-pregnancy body weight status predicts excessive gestational weight gain: findings from a US cohort study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2008;8(1):54.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson F, Cooke L, Croker H, Wardle J. Changing perceptions of weight in Great Britain: comparison of two population surveys. BMJ. 2008;337:a494.CrossRef Johnson F, Cooke L, Croker H, Wardle J. Changing perceptions of weight in Great Britain: comparison of two population surveys. BMJ. 2008;337:a494.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Salcedo V, Gutierrez-Fisac J, Guallar-Castillon P, Rodriguez-Artalejo F. Trends in overweight and misperceived overweight in Spain from 1987 to 2007. Int J Obes. 2010;34(12):1759–65.CrossRef Salcedo V, Gutierrez-Fisac J, Guallar-Castillon P, Rodriguez-Artalejo F. Trends in overweight and misperceived overweight in Spain from 1987 to 2007. Int J Obes. 2010;34(12):1759–65.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Langellier BA, Glik D, Ortega AN, Prelip ML. Trends in racial/ethnic disparities in overweight self-perception among US adults, 1988–1994 and 1999–2008. Public Health Nutr. 2015;18(12):2115–2125.CrossRef Langellier BA, Glik D, Ortega AN, Prelip ML. Trends in racial/ethnic disparities in overweight self-perception among US adults, 1988–1994 and 1999–2008. Public Health Nutr. 2015;18(12):2115–2125.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Blokstra A, Burns C, Seidell J. Perception of weight status and dieting behaviour in Dutch men and women. Int J Obes. 1999;23(1):7–17.CrossRef Blokstra A, Burns C, Seidell J. Perception of weight status and dieting behaviour in Dutch men and women. Int J Obes. 1999;23(1):7–17.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Mogre V, Mwinlenna PP, Oladele J. Distorted self-perceived weight status and its associated factors among civil servants in tamale, Ghana: a cross-sectional study. Arch Public Health. 2013;71(1):30.CrossRef Mogre V, Mwinlenna PP, Oladele J. Distorted self-perceived weight status and its associated factors among civil servants in tamale, Ghana: a cross-sectional study. Arch Public Health. 2013;71(1):30.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Caleyachetty R, Kengne AP, Muennig P, Rutter H, Echouffo-Tcheugui JB. Misperception of body weight among overweight or obese adults in Mauritius. Obes Res Clin Pract. 2016;10(2):216–9.CrossRef Caleyachetty R, Kengne AP, Muennig P, Rutter H, Echouffo-Tcheugui JB. Misperception of body weight among overweight or obese adults in Mauritius. Obes Res Clin Pract. 2016;10(2):216–9.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanenbaum HC, Felicitas JQ, Li Y, Tobias M, Chou C-P, Palmer PH, Spruijt-Metz D, Reynolds KD, Johnson CA, Xie B. Overweight perception: associations with weight control goals, attempts, and practices among Chinese female college students. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2016;116(3):458–66.CrossRef Tanenbaum HC, Felicitas JQ, Li Y, Tobias M, Chou C-P, Palmer PH, Spruijt-Metz D, Reynolds KD, Johnson CA, Xie B. Overweight perception: associations with weight control goals, attempts, and practices among Chinese female college students. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2016;116(3):458–66.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Herbert J, Peterson KL, Alston L, Allender S, Nichols M. Comparison between measured and perceived weight status in a nationally representative sample of Australian adults. Obes Res Clin Pract. 2017;11:414–25.CrossRef Herbert J, Peterson KL, Alston L, Allender S, Nichols M. Comparison between measured and perceived weight status in a nationally representative sample of Australian adults. Obes Res Clin Pract. 2017;11:414–25.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuchler F, Variyam J. Mistakes were made: misperception as a barrier to reducing overweight. Int J Obes. 2003;27(7):856–61.CrossRef Kuchler F, Variyam J. Mistakes were made: misperception as a barrier to reducing overweight. Int J Obes. 2003;27(7):856–61.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Brener ND, Eaton DK, Lowry R, McManus T. The association between weight perception and BMI among high school students. Obes Res. 2004;12(11):1866–74.CrossRef Brener ND, Eaton DK, Lowry R, McManus T. The association between weight perception and BMI among high school students. Obes Res. 2004;12(11):1866–74.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Hansen AR, Duncan D, Baidal JW, Hill A, Turner S, Zhang J. An increasing trend in health-care professionals notifying children of unhealthy weight status: NHANES 1999–2014. Int J Obes. 2016;40(10):1480.CrossRef Hansen AR, Duncan D, Baidal JW, Hill A, Turner S, Zhang J. An increasing trend in health-care professionals notifying children of unhealthy weight status: NHANES 1999–2014. Int J Obes. 2016;40(10):1480.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Haynes A, Kersbergen I, Sutin A, Daly M, Robinson E. A systematic review of the relationship between weight status perceptions and weight loss attempts, strategies, behaviours and outcomes. Obes Rev. 2018;19(3):347–63.CrossRef Haynes A, Kersbergen I, Sutin A, Daly M, Robinson E. A systematic review of the relationship between weight status perceptions and weight loss attempts, strategies, behaviours and outcomes. Obes Rev. 2018;19(3):347–63.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Sonneville K, Thurston I, Milliren C, Kamody R, Gooding H, Richmond T. Helpful or harmful? Prospective association between weight misperception and weight gain among overweight and obese adolescents and young adults. Int J Obes. 2016;40(2):328–32.CrossRef Sonneville K, Thurston I, Milliren C, Kamody R, Gooding H, Richmond T. Helpful or harmful? Prospective association between weight misperception and weight gain among overweight and obese adolescents and young adults. Int J Obes. 2016;40(2):328–32.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Unger ES, Kawachi I, Milliren CE, Sonneville KR, Thurston IB, Gooding HC, Richmond TK. Protective misperception? Prospective study of weight self-perception and Blood pressure in adolescents with overweight and obesity. J Adolesc Health. 2017;60:680–7.CrossRef Unger ES, Kawachi I, Milliren CE, Sonneville KR, Thurston IB, Gooding HC, Richmond TK. Protective misperception? Prospective study of weight self-perception and Blood pressure in adolescents with overweight and obesity. J Adolesc Health. 2017;60:680–7.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Strauss RS. Self-reported weight status and dieting in a cross-sectional sample of young adolescents: National Health and nutrition examination survey III. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1999;153(7):741–7.CrossRef Strauss RS. Self-reported weight status and dieting in a cross-sectional sample of young adolescents: National Health and nutrition examination survey III. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1999;153(7):741–7.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Sutin AR, Terracciano A. Perceived weight discrimination and obesity. PLoS One. 2013;8(7):e70048.CrossRef Sutin AR, Terracciano A. Perceived weight discrimination and obesity. PLoS One. 2013;8(7):e70048.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson E, Hunger J, Daly M. Perceived weight status and risk of weight gain across life in US and UK adults. Int J Obes. 2015;39:1721–6.CrossRef Robinson E, Hunger J, Daly M. Perceived weight status and risk of weight gain across life in US and UK adults. Int J Obes. 2015;39:1721–6.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Daly M, Robinson E, Sutin AR. Does knowing hurt? Perceiving oneself as overweight predicts future physical health and well-being. Psychol Sci. 2017;28(7):872–81.CrossRef Daly M, Robinson E, Sutin AR. Does knowing hurt? Perceiving oneself as overweight predicts future physical health and well-being. Psychol Sci. 2017;28(7):872–81.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson E, Sutin AR. Parental perception of weight status and weight gain across childhood. Pediatrics. 2016;137:e20153957.CrossRef Robinson E, Sutin AR. Parental perception of weight status and weight gain across childhood. Pediatrics. 2016;137:e20153957.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Almond D, Lee A, Schwartz AE. Impacts of classifying new York City students as overweight. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113(13):3488–91.CrossRef Almond D, Lee A, Schwartz AE. Impacts of classifying new York City students as overweight. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113(13):3488–91.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Sutin AR, Stephan Y, Grzywacz JG, Robinson E, Daly M, Terracciano A. Perceived weight discrimination, changes in health, and daily stressors. Obesity. 2016;24(10):2202–9.CrossRef Sutin AR, Stephan Y, Grzywacz JG, Robinson E, Daly M, Terracciano A. Perceived weight discrimination, changes in health, and daily stressors. Obesity. 2016;24(10):2202–9.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Nolan LJ, Eshleman A. Paved with good intentions: paradoxical eating responses to weight stigma. Appetite. 2016;102:15–24.CrossRef Nolan LJ, Eshleman A. Paved with good intentions: paradoxical eating responses to weight stigma. Appetite. 2016;102:15–24.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Calzo JP, Sonneville KR, Haines J, Blood EA, Field AE, Austin SB. The development of associations among body mass index, body dissatisfaction, and weight and shape concern in adolescent boys and girls. J Adolesc Health. 2012;51(5):517–23.CrossRef Calzo JP, Sonneville KR, Haines J, Blood EA, Field AE, Austin SB. The development of associations among body mass index, body dissatisfaction, and weight and shape concern in adolescent boys and girls. J Adolesc Health. 2012;51(5):517–23.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Tiggemann M. Body image across the adult life span: stability and change. Body Image. 2004;1(1):29–41.CrossRef Tiggemann M. Body image across the adult life span: stability and change. Body Image. 2004;1(1):29–41.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Loth KA, Watts AW, Van Den Berg P, Neumark-Sztainer D. Does body satisfaction help or harm overweight teens? A 10-year longitudinal study of the relationship between body satisfaction and body mass index. J Adolesc Health. 2015;57(5):559–61.CrossRef Loth KA, Watts AW, Van Den Berg P, Neumark-Sztainer D. Does body satisfaction help or harm overweight teens? A 10-year longitudinal study of the relationship between body satisfaction and body mass index. J Adolesc Health. 2015;57(5):559–61.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Neumark-Sztainer D, Paxton SJ, Hannan PJ, Haines J, Story M. Does body satisfaction matter? Five-year longitudinal associations between body satisfaction and health behaviors in adolescent females and males. J Adolesc Health. 2006;39(2):244–51.CrossRef Neumark-Sztainer D, Paxton SJ, Hannan PJ, Haines J, Story M. Does body satisfaction matter? Five-year longitudinal associations between body satisfaction and health behaviors in adolescent females and males. J Adolesc Health. 2006;39(2):244–51.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Sonneville KR, Calzo JP, Horton NJ, Haines J, Austin SB, Field AE. Body satisfaction, weight gain and binge eating among overweight adolescent girls. Int J Obes. 2012;36(7):944.CrossRef Sonneville KR, Calzo JP, Horton NJ, Haines J, Austin SB, Field AE. Body satisfaction, weight gain and binge eating among overweight adolescent girls. Int J Obes. 2012;36(7):944.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Suelter CS, Schvey N, Kelly NR, Shanks M, Thompson KA, Mehari R, Brady S, Yanovski SZ, Melby CL, Tanofsky-Kraff M. Relationship of pressure to be thin with gains in body weight and fat mass in adolescents. Pediatr Obes. 2018;13(1):14–22.CrossRef Suelter CS, Schvey N, Kelly NR, Shanks M, Thompson KA, Mehari R, Brady S, Yanovski SZ, Melby CL, Tanofsky-Kraff M. Relationship of pressure to be thin with gains in body weight and fat mass in adolescents. Pediatr Obes. 2018;13(1):14–22.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat van den Berg P, Neumark-Sztainer D. Fat’ n happy 5 years later: is it bad for overweight girls to like their bodies? J Adolesc Health. 2007;41(4):415–7.CrossRef van den Berg P, Neumark-Sztainer D. Fat’ n happy 5 years later: is it bad for overweight girls to like their bodies? J Adolesc Health. 2007;41(4):415–7.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Fouts G, Burggraf K. Television situation comedies: female weight, male negative comments, and audience reactions. Sex Roles. 2000;42(9–10):925–32.CrossRef Fouts G, Burggraf K. Television situation comedies: female weight, male negative comments, and audience reactions. Sex Roles. 2000;42(9–10):925–32.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Lieberman DL, Tybur JM, Latner JD. Disgust sensitivity, obesity stigma, and gender: contamination psychology predicts weight bias for women, not men. Obesity. 2012;20(9):1803–14.CrossRef Lieberman DL, Tybur JM, Latner JD. Disgust sensitivity, obesity stigma, and gender: contamination psychology predicts weight bias for women, not men. Obesity. 2012;20(9):1803–14.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson E. Overweight but unseen: a review of the underestimation of weight status and a visual normalization theory. Obes Rev. 2017;18:1200–9.CrossRef Robinson E. Overweight but unseen: a review of the underestimation of weight status and a visual normalization theory. Obes Rev. 2017;18:1200–9.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Ng M, Fleming T, Robinson M, Thomson B, Graetz N, Margono C, Mullany EC, Biryukov S, Abbafati C, Abera SF. Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2013. Lancet. 2014;384(9945):766–81.CrossRef Ng M, Fleming T, Robinson M, Thomson B, Graetz N, Margono C, Mullany EC, Biryukov S, Abbafati C, Abera SF. Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2013. Lancet. 2014;384(9945):766–81.CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Feng X, Wilson A. Neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage modifies the relationship between weight status and weight-related satisfaction. Int J Obes. 2016;40(12):1922–6.CrossRef Feng X, Wilson A. Neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage modifies the relationship between weight status and weight-related satisfaction. Int J Obes. 2016;40(12):1922–6.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Hochdorn A, Faleiros VP, Camargo BV, Bousfield AB, Wachelke JF, Quintão IP, Azzolina D, Gregori D. Obese children are thin in parents’ eyes: a psychologically, socially, or culturally driven bias? J Health Psychol. 2018;23(1):114–26.CrossRef Hochdorn A, Faleiros VP, Camargo BV, Bousfield AB, Wachelke JF, Quintão IP, Azzolina D, Gregori D. Obese children are thin in parents’ eyes: a psychologically, socially, or culturally driven bias? J Health Psychol. 2018;23(1):114–26.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Feng X, Astell-Burt T, Badland H, Mavoa S, Giles-Corti B. Modest ratios of fast food outlets to supermarkets and green grocers are associated with higher body mass index: longitudinal analysis of a sample of 15,229 Australians aged 45 years and older in the Australian National Liveability Study. Health Place. 2018;49:101–10.CrossRef Feng X, Astell-Burt T, Badland H, Mavoa S, Giles-Corti B. Modest ratios of fast food outlets to supermarkets and green grocers are associated with higher body mass index: longitudinal analysis of a sample of 15,229 Australians aged 45 years and older in the Australian National Liveability Study. Health Place. 2018;49:101–10.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin AA, Davidson TL. Human cognitive function and the obesogenic environment. Physiol Behav. 2014;136:185–93.CrossRef Martin AA, Davidson TL. Human cognitive function and the obesogenic environment. Physiol Behav. 2014;136:185–93.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Watson N, Wooden M. The household, income and labour dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey: wave 1 survey methodology’, HILDA project technical paper series no. 1/02, May. Melbourne: Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research, University of Melbourne; 2002. Watson N, Wooden M. The household, income and labour dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey: wave 1 survey methodology’, HILDA project technical paper series no. 1/02, May. Melbourne: Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research, University of Melbourne; 2002.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Pink B. Socio-economic indexes for areas (SEIFA). Canberra: Australian Bureau of Statistics; 2011. Pink B. Socio-economic indexes for areas (SEIFA). Canberra: Australian Bureau of Statistics; 2011.
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Feng X, Wilson A. Do neighbourhood socioeconomic circumstances not matter for weight status among Australian men? Multilevel evidence from a household survey of 14 691 adults. BMJ Open. 2015;5(9):e007052.CrossRef Feng X, Wilson A. Do neighbourhood socioeconomic circumstances not matter for weight status among Australian men? Multilevel evidence from a household survey of 14 691 adults. BMJ Open. 2015;5(9):e007052.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Feng X, Wilson A. Neighbourhood socioeconomic inequality and gender differences in body mass index: the role of unhealthy behaviours. Prev Med. 2017;101:171–7.CrossRef Feng X, Wilson A. Neighbourhood socioeconomic inequality and gender differences in body mass index: the role of unhealthy behaviours. Prev Med. 2017;101:171–7.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Lovasi GS, Hutson MA, Guerra M, Neckerman KM. Built environments and obesity in disadvantaged populations. Epidemiol Rev. 2009;31(1):7–20.CrossRef Lovasi GS, Hutson MA, Guerra M, Neckerman KM. Built environments and obesity in disadvantaged populations. Epidemiol Rev. 2009;31(1):7–20.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Diez Roux AV, Mair C. Neighborhoods and health. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1186(1):125–45.CrossRef Diez Roux AV, Mair C. Neighborhoods and health. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1186(1):125–45.CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Kawachi I, Berkman LF. Neighborhoods and health. USA: Oxford University Press; 2003.CrossRef Kawachi I, Berkman LF. Neighborhoods and health. USA: Oxford University Press; 2003.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Rasbash J, Browne W, Goldstein H, Yang M, Plewis I, Healy M, Woodhouse G, Draper D, Langford I, Lewis T. A user’s guide to MLwiN; 2000. Rasbash J, Browne W, Goldstein H, Yang M, Plewis I, Healy M, Woodhouse G, Draper D, Langford I, Lewis T. A user’s guide to MLwiN; 2000.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson E, Christiansen P. The changing face of obesity: exposure to and acceptance of obesity. Obesity. 2014;22(5):1380–6.CrossRef Robinson E, Christiansen P. The changing face of obesity: exposure to and acceptance of obesity. Obesity. 2014;22(5):1380–6.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat McLaren L, Gauvin L. Neighbourhood level versus individual level correlates of women's body dissatisfaction: toward a multilevel understanding of the role of affluence. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2002;56(3):193–9.CrossRef McLaren L, Gauvin L. Neighbourhood level versus individual level correlates of women's body dissatisfaction: toward a multilevel understanding of the role of affluence. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2002;56(3):193–9.CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat McLaren L, Gauvin L. Does the ‘average size’of women in the neighbourhood influence a woman's likelihood of body dissatisfaction? Health Place. 2003;9(4):327–35.CrossRef McLaren L, Gauvin L. Does the ‘average size’of women in the neighbourhood influence a woman's likelihood of body dissatisfaction? Health Place. 2003;9(4):327–35.CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Ogden J, Mundray K. The effect of the media on body satisfaction: the role of gender and size. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 1996;4(3):171–82.CrossRef Ogden J, Mundray K. The effect of the media on body satisfaction: the role of gender and size. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 1996;4(3):171–82.CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Benton C, Karazsia BT. The effect of thin and muscular images on women's body satisfaction. Body Image. 2015;13:22–7.CrossRef Benton C, Karazsia BT. The effect of thin and muscular images on women's body satisfaction. Body Image. 2015;13:22–7.CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Krawczyk R, Thompson JK. The effects of advertisements that sexually objectify women on state body dissatisfaction and judgments of women: the moderating roles of gender and internalization. Body Image. 2015;15:109–19.CrossRef Krawczyk R, Thompson JK. The effects of advertisements that sexually objectify women on state body dissatisfaction and judgments of women: the moderating roles of gender and internalization. Body Image. 2015;15:109–19.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Grabe S, Ward LM, Hyde JS. The role of the media in body image concerns among women: a meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(3):460.CrossRef Grabe S, Ward LM, Hyde JS. The role of the media in body image concerns among women: a meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(3):460.CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Groesz LM, Levine MP, Murnen SK. The effect of experimental presentation of thin media images on body satisfaction: a meta-analytic review. Int J Eat Disord. 2002;31(1):1–16.CrossRef Groesz LM, Levine MP, Murnen SK. The effect of experimental presentation of thin media images on body satisfaction: a meta-analytic review. Int J Eat Disord. 2002;31(1):1–16.CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Swami V, Frederick DA, Aavik T, Alcalay L, Allik J, Anderson D, Andrianto S, Arora A, Brännström Å, Cunningham J. The attractive female body weight and female body dissatisfaction in 26 countries across 10 world regions: results of the international body project I. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2010;36(3):309–25.CrossRef Swami V, Frederick DA, Aavik T, Alcalay L, Allik J, Anderson D, Andrianto S, Arora A, Brännström Å, Cunningham J. The attractive female body weight and female body dissatisfaction in 26 countries across 10 world regions: results of the international body project I. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2010;36(3):309–25.CrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Barlow P, Reeves A, McKee M, Galea G, Stuckler D. Unhealthy diets, obesity and time discounting: a systematic literature review and network analysis. Obes Rev. 2016;17(9):810–9.CrossRef Barlow P, Reeves A, McKee M, Galea G, Stuckler D. Unhealthy diets, obesity and time discounting: a systematic literature review and network analysis. Obes Rev. 2016;17(9):810–9.CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang L, Rashad I. Obesity and time preference: the health consequences of discounting the future. J Biosoc Sci. 2008;40(1):97–113.CrossRef Zhang L, Rashad I. Obesity and time preference: the health consequences of discounting the future. J Biosoc Sci. 2008;40(1):97–113.CrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Wardle J, Griffith J. Socioeconomic status and weight control practices in British adults. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2001;55(3):185–90.CrossRef Wardle J, Griffith J. Socioeconomic status and weight control practices in British adults. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2001;55(3):185–90.CrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Dallman MF. Stress-induced obesity and the emotional nervous system. Trends Endocrinol Metab. 2010;21(3):159–65.CrossRef Dallman MF. Stress-induced obesity and the emotional nervous system. Trends Endocrinol Metab. 2010;21(3):159–65.CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Dallman MF, Pecoraro NC, La Fleur SE, Warne JP, Ginsberg AB, Akana SF, Laugero KC, Houshyar H, Strack AM, Bhatnagar S. Glucocorticoids, chronic stress, and obesity. Prog Brain Res. 2006;153:75–105.CrossRef Dallman MF, Pecoraro NC, La Fleur SE, Warne JP, Ginsberg AB, Akana SF, Laugero KC, Houshyar H, Strack AM, Bhatnagar S. Glucocorticoids, chronic stress, and obesity. Prog Brain Res. 2006;153:75–105.CrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Matud MP. Gender differences in stress and coping styles. Pers Individ Dif. 2004;37(7):1401–15.CrossRef Matud MP. Gender differences in stress and coping styles. Pers Individ Dif. 2004;37(7):1401–15.CrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Ptacek JT, Smith RE, Zanas J. Gender, appraisal, and coping: a longitudinal analysis. J Pers. 1992;60(4):747–70.CrossRef Ptacek JT, Smith RE, Zanas J. Gender, appraisal, and coping: a longitudinal analysis. J Pers. 1992;60(4):747–70.CrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Tamres LK, Janicki D, Helgeson VS. Sex differences in coping behavior: a meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2002;6(1):2–30.CrossRef Tamres LK, Janicki D, Helgeson VS. Sex differences in coping behavior: a meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2002;6(1):2–30.CrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Donaghue N. The moderating effects of socioeconomic status on relationships between obesity framing and stigmatization of fat people. Fat Stud. 2014;3(1):6–16.CrossRef Donaghue N. The moderating effects of socioeconomic status on relationships between obesity framing and stigmatization of fat people. Fat Stud. 2014;3(1):6–16.CrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Fikkan JL, Rothblum ED. Is fat a feminist issue? Exploring the gendered nature of weight bias. Sex Roles. 2012;66(9–10):575–92.CrossRef Fikkan JL, Rothblum ED. Is fat a feminist issue? Exploring the gendered nature of weight bias. Sex Roles. 2012;66(9–10):575–92.CrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Thomas SL, Hyde J, Karunaratne A, Herbert D, Komesaroff PA. Being ‘fat’in today’s world: a qualitative study of the lived experiences of people with obesity in Australia. Health Expect. 2008;11(4):321–30.CrossRef Thomas SL, Hyde J, Karunaratne A, Herbert D, Komesaroff PA. Being ‘fat’in today’s world: a qualitative study of the lived experiences of people with obesity in Australia. Health Expect. 2008;11(4):321–30.CrossRef
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Puhl RM, Heuer CA. The stigma of obesity: a review and update. Obesity. 2009;17(5):941–64.CrossRef Puhl RM, Heuer CA. The stigma of obesity: a review and update. Obesity. 2009;17(5):941–64.CrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Brewis A, Trainer S, Han S, Wutich A. Publically misfitting: extreme weight and the everyday production and reinforcement of felt stigma. Med Anthropol Q. 2017;31(2):257–76.CrossRef Brewis A, Trainer S, Han S, Wutich A. Publically misfitting: extreme weight and the everyday production and reinforcement of felt stigma. Med Anthropol Q. 2017;31(2):257–76.CrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Schuster RC, Han SY, Brewis AA, Wutich A. Increasing overweight and obesity erodes engagement in one's neighborhood by women, but not men. Prev Med Rep. 2018;10:144–9.CrossRef Schuster RC, Han SY, Brewis AA, Wutich A. Increasing overweight and obesity erodes engagement in one's neighborhood by women, but not men. Prev Med Rep. 2018;10:144–9.CrossRef
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Levine MP. Loneliness and eating disorders. J Psychol. 2012;146(1–2):243–57.CrossRef Levine MP. Loneliness and eating disorders. J Psychol. 2012;146(1–2):243–57.CrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Paeratakul S, White MA, Williamson DA, Ryan DH, Bray GA. Sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and BMI in relation to self-perception of overweight. Obes Res. 2002;10(5):345–50.CrossRef Paeratakul S, White MA, Williamson DA, Ryan DH, Bray GA. Sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and BMI in relation to self-perception of overweight. Obes Res. 2002;10(5):345–50.CrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Dorsey RR, Eberhardt MS, Ogden CL. Racial/ethnic differences in weight perception. Obesity. 2009;17(4):790–5.CrossRef Dorsey RR, Eberhardt MS, Ogden CL. Racial/ethnic differences in weight perception. Obesity. 2009;17(4):790–5.CrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Mikolajczyk RT, Iannotti RJ, Farhat T, Thomas V. Ethnic differences in perceptions of body satisfaction and body appearance among US schoolchildren: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12(1):425.CrossRef Mikolajczyk RT, Iannotti RJ, Farhat T, Thomas V. Ethnic differences in perceptions of body satisfaction and body appearance among US schoolchildren: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12(1):425.CrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Wardle J, Haase AM, Steptoe A. Body image and weight control in young adults: international comparisons in university students from 22 countries. Int J Obes. 2006;30(4):644–51.CrossRef Wardle J, Haase AM, Steptoe A. Body image and weight control in young adults: international comparisons in university students from 22 countries. Int J Obes. 2006;30(4):644–51.CrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Rowland ML. Self-reported weight and height. Am J Clin Nutr. 1990;52(6):1125–33.CrossRef Rowland ML. Self-reported weight and height. Am J Clin Nutr. 1990;52(6):1125–33.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Does dissatisfaction with, or accurate perception of overweight status help people reduce weight? Longitudinal study of Australian adults
verfasst von
Xiaoqi Feng
Andrew Wilson
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-6938-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

BMC Public Health 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe