Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Diabetologia 3/2018

Open Access 29.11.2017 | Article

Effects of hypoglycaemia on working memory and regional cerebral blood flow in type 1 diabetes: a randomised, crossover trial

verfasst von: Michael Gejl, Albert Gjedde, Birgitte Brock, Arne Møller, Eelco van Duinkerken, Hanne L. Haahr, Charlotte T. Hansen, Pei-Ling Chu, Kirstine L. Stender-Petersen, Jørgen Rungby

Erschienen in: Diabetologia | Ausgabe 3/2018

Abstract

Aims/hypothesis

The aim of this randomised, crossover trial was to compare cognitive functioning and associated brain activation patterns during hypoglycaemia (plasma glucose [PG] just below 3.1 mmol/l) and euglycaemia in individuals with type 1 diabetes mellitus.

Methods

In this patient-blinded, crossover study, 26 participants with type 1 diabetes mellitus attended two randomised experimental visits: one hypoglycaemic clamp (PG 2.8 ± 0.2 mmol/l, approximate duration 55 min) and one euglycaemic clamp (PG 5.5 mmol/l ± 10%). PG levels were maintained by hyperinsulinaemic glucose clamping. Cognitive functioning was assessed during hypoglycaemia and euglycaemia conditions using a modified version of the digit symbol substitution test (mDSST) and control DSST (cDSST). Simultaneously, regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) was measured in pre-specified brain regions by six H2 15O-positron emission tomographies (PET) per session.

Results

Working memory was impaired during hypoglycaemia as indicated by a statistically significantly lower mDSST score (estimated treatment difference [ETD] −0.63 [95% CI −1.13, −0.14], p = 0.014) and a statistically significantly longer response time (ETD 2.86 s [7%] [95% CI 0.67, 5.05], p = 0.013) compared with euglycaemia. During hypoglycaemia, mDSST task performance was associated with increased activity in the frontal lobe regions, superior parietal lobe and thalamus, and decreased activity in the temporal lobe regions (p < 0.05). Working memory activation (mDSST − cDSST) statistically significantly increased blood flow in the striatum during hypoglycaemia (ETD 0.0374% [95% CI 0.0157, 0.0590], p = 0.002).

Conclusions/interpretation

During hypoglycaemia (mean PG 2.9 mmol/l), working memory performance was impaired. Altered performance was associated with significantly increased blood flow in the striatum, a part of the basal ganglia implicated in regulating motor functions, memory, language and emotion.

Trial registration

NCT01789593, clinicaltrials.​gov

Funding

This study was funded by Novo Nordisk.
Begleitmaterial
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00125-017-4502-1) contains peer-reviewed but unedited supplementary material, which is available to authorised users.
Abkürzungen
AE
Adverse event
CBF
Cerebral blood flow
cDSST
Control digit symbol substitution test
DSST
Digit symbol substitution test
ETD
Estimated treatment difference
fMRI
Functional MRI
mDSST
Modified digit symbol substitution test
PASAT
Paced auditory serial addition task
PET
Positron emission tomography
PG
Plasma glucose
rCBF
Regional cerebral blood flow
ROI
Regions of interest

Introduction

Cognitive performance, even during simple tasks, is impaired during acute hypoglycaemia (plasma glucose [PG] <2.5 mmol/l [45.0 mg/dl]) in individuals with type 1 diabetes mellitus [1, 2] and the degree of impairment depends on the level of hypoglycaemia [3]. Additionally, the cognitive impairment induced by hypoglycaemia (PG 2.5–2.7 mmol/l) can remain following the restoration of euglycaemia [46]; with some studies showing cognitive impairments lasting for approximately 45–75 min after euglycaemic restoration [7, 8]. Working memory is an important aspect of cognitive function and is susceptible to the effects of hypoglycaemia. It has been demonstrated using functional MRI (fMRI) that, compared with healthy individuals, people with type 1 diabetes mellitus require higher levels of brain activation to attain parity for working memory performance during hypoglycaemia (PG ≤2.8 mmol/l [50.4 mg/dl]) [9]. Thus, identification of cerebral activation patterns during working memory performance at different PG levels could enhance our understanding of mechanisms underlying the reduced cerebral efficiency seen in type 1 diabetes mellitus [9]. For example, it remains to be clarified if milder hypoglycaemic episodes, PG just below 3.1 mmol/l and previously associated with altered brain activity [10, 11], have a similar negative impact on working memory to those demonstrated at PG ≤2.8 mmol/l. In individuals with type 1 diabetes mellitus, cerebral blood flow (CBF) and cerebral glucose metabolism [12] within total grey matter are correlated. This correlation becomes stronger when adjusted for glucose levels, allowing CBF assessment to be used as a proxy for cerebral metabolism [13]. However, there are some limitations to assessment with fMRI; namely it does not provide a direct measure of neuronal oxygen consumption or neuronal activation and results are vulnerable to movement distortion. Conversely, radiolabelled water (H2 15O) positron emission tomography (PET) is a direct measure of cerebral oxygen consumption (and thus neuronal activation) that is less affected by movement and more quantifiable than fMRI [14]. This hypothesis-driven study aimed to test if cognitive performance (assessed by H2 15O PET) and associated CBF estimates are affected at less pronounced levels of hypoglycaemia than previously studied and if cognitive performance is affected in the recovery phase following less pronounced hypoglycaemia.

Methods

Study design

This randomised, single-centre, patient-blinded, two-period crossover study compared cognitive performance (assessed by working memory performance and reaction time) under hypoglycaemic (aiming for a PG target just below 3.1 mmol/l; the clamp target was defined as 2.8 ± 0.2 [2.6–3.0] mmol/l) and euglycaemic (PG clamp target 5.5 mmol/l ± 10%) conditions in participants with type 1 diabetes mellitus (ESM Fig. 1). Blood was drawn at pre-specified time points to assess counter-regulatory hormone responses. Hypoglycaemia awareness and symptoms during both clamps were also assessed. Participants underwent the sequence of glycaemic conditions in a blind and randomised order determined by sequential enrolment and lowest available number assignment. The two experimental visits were separated by 21–42 days (to avoid effects of counter-regulatory hormone responses or other physiological effects of hypoglycaemia). Female participants attended the two visits at the same stage of their menstrual cycle. The study was conducted from 14 January to 1 December 2013 at the Department of Endocrinology and Department of Nuclear Medicine and PET Center, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants before any study-related activities. The study was conducted in compliance with International Conference on Harmonisation Good Clinical Practice [15], the Declaration of Helsinki [16] and was approved according to local regulations by an independent ethics committee.

Participants

Inclusion criteria for participants screened (n = 37) were right-handedness, age 18–64 years, BMI 18.0–28.0 kg/m2, HbA1c ≤9.0% (≤75 mmol/mol), diagnosed with type 1 diabetes mellitus and treated with multiple daily insulin injections or continuous subcutaneous insulin infusion for ≥12 months prior to screening. Key exclusion criteria included known central nervous system abnormalities, structural brain abnormalities (identified by structural MRI scans during screening), severe hypoglycaemia (requiring third party assistance) or ketoacidosis in the last 6 months, clinically defined hypoglycaemic unawareness, and treatment with medications potentially interfering with glucose metabolism. Key experimental visit exclusion criteria included occurrence of a hypoglycaemic event (with PG ≤3.9 mmol/l) within the preceding 48 h. Full inclusion and exclusion criteria are listed in ESM Table 1.

Cognitive tests

The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (third edition) revised digit symbol substitution test (DSST) [17] was previously adapted and validated to specifically measure working memory [18], and has been successfully used with fMRI [19]. Here, we used the stimuli of this adapted DSST paradigm and modified it to our specific PET procedure. As with the fMRI design, we used the modified DSST (mDSST) task to measure working memory. A control task (cDSST) was used for non-task-related brain activation (without a working memory load), including visual and motor cortex activation (for eye sight and movement) and index finger movement.
Subtraction of regional CBF (rCBF) patterns during cDSST from those during mDSST was interpreted as indicative of brain activation patterns exclusively associated with the operation of working memory. The mDSST task consisted of three blocks of 32 randomly presented digit–symbol combinations (ESM Methods: Cognitive tests – mDSST and cDSST task combination blocks and ESM Fig. 2), fitting the specific H2 15O PET design. The cDSST task had the same basic design. Each block began with test instructions shown for 7.8 s and was 0.2 s longer than every 3 min PET acquisition, to ensure participants were engaged in the mDSST or cDSST task during the full 3 min PET acquisition. Tasks were presented using E-prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tool, Pittsburgh, PA, USA) through audiovisual goggles inside the PET scanner. Correct responses (no response was considered as incorrect) and response time were recorded and analysed.
To test working memory following a prolonged recovery phase (75–90 min after PG of 5–6 mmol/l was restored) following hypoglycaemia or euglycaemia, the paced auditory serial addition task (PASAT) was preferred over the mDSST to avoid bias from habituation [20, 21]. During the PASAT, participants heard a digit and had to add the next digit (presented 3 or 2 s later) and report the sum aloud. Both parts consisted of 60 digits and correct responses were recorded.

PET imaging

Each participant underwent PET imaging at both visits. First, a 6 min transmission scan for attenuation correction was performed. Thereafter, six 3 min tomography sessions with either mDSST or cDSST in a fixed order were performed. Cerebral activity levels were measured as change in brain uptake of radiolabelled water (H2 15O), the retention of which matches the rate of CBF, by means of a high-resolution research tomograph (Siemens/CTI, Knoxville, TN, USA) operating in 3-D mode. Additional detail is provided in ESM Methods: PET imaging.

Experimental visit procedures

Participants attended the study site at approximately 20:00 hours on the day before each experimental procedure, at which point normal insulin treatment was suspended. Participants stayed overnight to ensure stabilisation of PG within the range of 5–8 mmol/l via variable intravenous infusion of insulin (Actrapid®, 100 U/ml) and glucose (20% glucose/dextrose/10 mmol/l KCl), before initiation of experimental procedures at 08:00 hours the following day. On the days of the experimental procedures, cognitive tests were briefly performed ≥1 h before initiation of hypoglycaemia or euglycaemia to prevent practice effects. Each glycaemic condition was preceded by a 60 min run-in period whereby variable intravenous infusions of glucose or human soluble insulin were delivered to obtain a steady-state PG target level of 5.5 mmol/l ± 10%. During the run-in and clamps, the participants’ cannulated hand was placed in a thermoregulated box with their arterialised venous blood sampled for PG measurements using a benchtop glucose analyser (YSI 2300 Stat Plus, Yellow Springs, OH, USA). Euglycaemia was maintained using a glucose clamp (glucose/Actrapid® infusion) for approximately 1 h after which insulin (Actrapid®, 100 U/ml) was given at an infusion rate of 3 mU kg−1 min−1 for 10 min and then reduced to 1.5 mU kg−1 min−1 thereafter. Euglycaemia was maintained or hypoglycaemia induced with glucose infusion rate adjusted accordingly to meet the PG target for approximately 55 min, during which H2 15O PET scans and cDSST and mDSST tasks were performed (Fig. 1). Following glycaemic clamps, participants were brought back to euglycaemia (with glucose infusions to reach a PG target of 5–6 mmol/l) and after approximately 75–90 min the PASAT was conducted. After experimental procedures, participants resumed usual insulin treatment.

Counter-regulatory hormones

Counter-regulatory hormones were measured as a validation that the PG target was sufficient to elicit a counter-regulatory response, and thus, hypoglycaemia. Hormonal responses (noradrenaline [norepinephrine], glucagon, cortisol and growth hormones) were measured 30 min prior to induction of hypoglycaemia or euglycaemia, and 45 min and 150 min (just prior to PASAT test) after induction of hypoglycaemia or euglycaemia.

Hypoglycaemia awareness and symptoms

At screening, hypoglycaemia unawareness was assessed by asking participants ‘Can you feel your hypos?’ and checking their medical files for any indication of unawareness. During the study, hypoglycaemia awareness was assessed both 30 min prior to and 45 min after induction of hypoglycaemia or euglycaemia, by asking participants ‘Do you feel hypo?’. The hypoglycaemia symptoms questionnaire (based on the Edinburgh Condition Scale) [22] was completed by participants 30 min prior to and 55 min after induction of hypoglycaemia or euglycaemia. It measured autonomic (sweating, palpitations, shaking and hunger), neuroglycopenic (confusion, drowsiness, odd behaviour, speech difficulty and incoordination) and general malaise (headache and nausea) symptoms on a seven point Likert scale.

rCBF assessment

Cerebral activation was measured as rCBF in 19 pre-specified regions of interest (ROI). Given the hypothesis-driven nature of this study, these regions were selected in accordance with relevant literature for one or more of the following criteria: DSST evoked brain activity patterns in normal conditions, i.e. euglycaemia, without working memory load (precuneus, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus/cortex, posterior cingulate gyrus, posterior supramarginal gyrus and orbitofrontal cortex) [19, 23]; neural substrate of DSST performance (inferior frontal gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, superior parietal lobe, precuneus, posterior cingulate gyrus/cortex and parahippocampal gyrus) [24, 25]; brain areas showing changes in functional activity in response to hypoglycaemia (medial temporal lobe, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, insula, globus pallidum, striatum, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, precuneus, posterior cingulate gyrus, posterior supramarginal gyrus and primary visual cortex) [9, 2630]; and brain areas reported as involved in working memory tasks (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, insula, superior parietal lobe, anterior cingulate gyrus/cortex, hippocampus and thalamus) [3133]. All rCBF measures were normalised to measures in the cerebral cortex, as this region is considered to be less impacted by the duration of type 1 diabetes mellitus [34]. In the present study, there was no significant difference in rCBF in the cerebral cortex between hypoglycaemia and euglycaemia during either cDSST or mDSST tasks.
Three endpoints were used to determine regional cerebral activation: rCBF during mDSST and cDSST performances, and rCBF for working memory. For the cDSST and mDSST endpoints, rCBF was calculated by subtracting the mean of three rCBF values for euglycaemia from the mean of three rCBF values for hypoglycaemia. To isolate changes as a result of working memory, rCBF values during the mDSST test were corrected for the rCBF values during cDSST, by subtracting the mean of three cDSST rCBF values from the mean of three mDSST rCBF values; this correction was conducted for measurements taken during both glycaemic clamps with the totals for euglycaemia subtracted from those for hypoglycaemia ([mean (3 × CBF during mDSST) − mean (3 × CBF during cDSST)] hypoglycaemia − [mean (3 × CBF during mDSST) − mean (3 × CBF during cDSST)] euglycaemia) to isolate changes in working memory during hypoglycaemia.

Endpoints and statistical analyses

The primary objective of the study was to compare cognitive performance during hypoglycaemia with that during euglycaemia. The primary endpoint was the number of correct mDSST scores. For each glycaemic condition, mean mDSST scores, reaction time and PASAT scores were compared using a linear mixed-effect model with glycaemic condition and period as fixed factors and participant as a random factor; mean differences between hypoglycaemia and euglycaemia were estimated from the model and corresponding 95% CI and p values were calculated. The rCBF and predefined ROI (during both cDSST and mDSST), as well as the difference in rCBF between the two tasks, were compared during glycaemic conditions using an analysis of variance with glycaemic condition, period and participant as fixed factors. Because of the hypothesis-driven nature of this trial, no correction for multiplicity was performed with regard to different ROI analyses.
The SD for DSST score between euglycaemia and hypoglycaemia (PG 2.5 mmol/l) was determined in a previous trial (NCT01002768) to be approximately nine. Assuming a similar variability in this trial, using a 5% significance level and two-sided paired t test, a sample size of 25 participants completing both periods was calculated to have 90% power to detect a true difference in DSST score between hypoglycaemia and euglycaemia of approximately six. A total of 28 participants were randomised to ensure at least 25 participants completing both experimental visits.

Results

Participant disposition

Of the 37 participants screened, 29 (22 men and seven women) were randomised and 26 completed both experimental visits (ESM Fig. 3); data are presented for completers only (Table 1). Three participants were withdrawn after randomisation (two men and one woman) for inability to place the venous catheter (n = 1), erroneous randomisation (n = 1) and meeting experimental visit exclusion criteria (n = 1). Mean age of participants was 38.7 years (range 19.0–65.1 years), BMI was 24.6 kg/m2, HbA1c was 7.3% (56.7 mmol/mol) and duration of diabetes was 18.7 years (Table 1). The educational level of participants was a mean ± SD of 14.0 ± 2.0 years (Table 1). Individual PG profiles during hypoglycaemic and euglycaemic clamps are shown in ESM Fig. 4. Mean ± SD PG achieved during hypoglycaemia was 2.9 ± 0.14 mmol/l, which is within the target PG. During the study four adverse events (AEs) were reported in three participants (two each during hypoglycaemia [presyncope and orthostatic hypotension] and euglycaemia [headache and nausea]). All AEs were non-serious, mild and classified as not related to Actrapid or devices used. One participant reported an AE after release from the trial site (flank pain).
Table 1
Baseline characteristics of completers
Characteristic
Value
Number of participants
26
Age, years (mean ± SD)
38.7 ± 15.3
BMI, kg/m2 (mean ± SD)
24.6 ± 2.7
Race
  Of European descent, n (%)
26 (100.0)
Sex
  Female, n (%)
6 (23.1)
  Male, n (%)
20 (76.9)
Educational level, years (mean ± SD [min–max])
14.0 ± 2.0 (10.0–18.0)
Diabetes characteristics (mean [min–max])
  Duration of diabetes, years
18.7 (3.1–46.4)
  HbA1c, mmol/mol
56.7 (39.0–74.0)
  HbA1c, %
7.3 (5.7–8.9)
Baseline information was recorded at screening and/or randomisation. If an assessment was recorded on both visits, the randomisation value was used as the baseline value

Cognitive performance

On average, participants had a statistically significantly lower mean mDSST score (± SD) during hypoglycaemia than euglycaemia (30.3 ± 1.5 vs 30.9 ± 0.6), with an estimated treatment difference (ETD) of −0.63 (95% CI −1.13, −0.14; p = 0.014) (Fig. 2a). Mean ± SD total response time during mDSST and hypoglycaemia was significantly longer than during euglycaemia (41.5 ± 8.9 s vs 38.7 ± 7.3 s), with an ETD of 2.86 s (95% CI 0.67, 5.05; p = 0.013) (Fig. 2b). Exclusion of two outliers (Fig. 2 and ESM Table 2) from the statistical analysis did not affect outcomes, with the mDSST score remaining significantly lower (ETD −0.32 [95% CI −0.62, −0.03], p = 0.035) and total response time remaining longer during hypoglycaemia compared with euglycaemia (ETD 2.00 s [95% CI 0.48, 3.52], p = 0.012). For working memory in the recovery phase, PASAT scores were not statistically significantly different following hypoglycaemia compared with euglycaemia, for either the 3 s PASAT (ETD −0.37 [95% CI −3.26, 2.52], p = 0.794) or the 2 s PASAT (ETD 0.79 [95% CI −1.34, 2.91], p = 0.454; ESM Table 3).

rCBF

Effects of hypoglycaemia on rCBF during cDSST
During the cDSST, without a working memory load, rCBF was statistically significantly lower during hypoglycaemia compared with euglycaemia in three temporal lobe regions (hippocampus, medial temporal lobe and parahippocampal gyrus) and in the striatum. In addition, rCBF was statistically significantly increased in six frontal lobe regions (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, orbitofrontal cortex, superior frontal gyrus and ventromedial prefrontal cortex), the superior parietal lobe and in the thalamus (Fig. 3a and ESM Table 4).
Effects of hypoglycaemia on rCBF during mDSST
During the mDSST (with a working memory load) rCBF was statistically significantly increased in three frontal lobe regions (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal gyrus and middle frontal gyrus), the superior parietal lobe and the thalamus, and statistically significantly decreased in the hippocampus and the medial temporal lobe during hypoglycaemia (Fig. 3b and ESM Table 4).
Differences in rCBF between hypoglycaemia and euglycaemia during cDSST and mDSST
Isolating working memory (measured as the rCBF during mDSST corrected for cDSST), there was statistically significantly higher blood flow in the striatum during hypoglycaemia when compared with euglycaemia (Fig. 3c), with an ETD of 0.0374% (95% CI 0.0157%, 0.0590%; p = 0.002).

Hypoglycaemia assessments and counter-regulatory hormone responses

When asked ‘Do you feel hypo?’ 30 min prior to induction of glycaemic conditions, the proportion of participants responding ‘no’ was 96.2% and 92.3%, for those exposed to prior hypoglycaemia or euglycaemia, respectively. During the glycaemic clamps (nominal time 45 min) proportionally more participants answered ‘yes’ when they were asked if they felt hypoglycaemic during hypoglycaemia (42.3%) compared with euglycaemia (11.5%). When we compared these subgroups in a post hoc analysis to determine the impact on the primary endpoint (mDSST score and response times), we found no differences between those responding ‘yes’ or ‘no’ to feeling the experimental hypoglycaemia (ESM Results). Mean ± SD hypoglycaemic symptom scores 30 min prior to induction of hypoglycaemia and euglycaemia were 17.69 ± 5.67 and 18.69 ± 4.88, respectively. Hypoglycaemic symptom scores during hypoglycaemia (55 min after initiating the induction of hypoglycaemia) and euglycaemia were 23.38 ± 11.67 and 18.27 ± 4.75, respectively.
Noradrenaline, cortisol and growth hormone responses were increased during hypoglycaemia compared with euglycaemia. The response of glucagon was compromised when comparing hypoglycaemia with euglycaemia. The counter-regulatory hormonal responses are shown in Fig. 4.

Discussion

This multidisciplinary study examined how cognitive performance and its concurrent rCBF were affected by hypoglycaemia at a mean PG of 2.9 mmol/l. This is a higher level of PG than has been tested in previous studies [1, 2], but matches the level recommended in reporting of clinical trials by the recent position statement from the International Hypoglycaemia Study Group [35] and is considered to be ‘sufficiently low to indicate serious, clinically important hypoglycaemia’. During hypoglycaemia there was a modest, yet significant, decrease in the number of correct mDSST responses referring to working memory, as well as a significant increase in response time. The effects of hypoglycaemia on rCBF in the striatum was statistically significant, highly uniform and with little variation (in SD) between participants. Furthermore, use of working memory during hypoglycaemia was associated with a significant increase in blood flow in the striatum.
Previous studies have reported reductions in cognitive function scores in the range of 2–40% during hypoglycaemia [8, 36]. The comparatively small decrease in number of correct mDSST responses in our study indicates, as expected, that working memory function was relatively preserved with regard to accuracy but at the cost of response speed [3]. These results indicate that during hypoglycaemia, at a mean PG of 2.9 mmol/l, participants with type 1 diabetes mellitus may be able to keep performance at a similar level as during euglycaemia, at least when performing less complex tasks, but at a 7% slower execution rate (3 s over a block of 32 questions). While this level of slowing may not be of great importance for many daily functions where response time is not critical, it could become serious in tasks which do depend on rapid information processing, such as driving.
Our study showed no difference in the number of correct PASAT responses in the recovery phase following hypoglycaemia compared with euglycaemia, possibly reflecting time of testing (75–90 min after euglycaemia restoration). Cognitive performance during recovery from hypoglycaemia has varied between different studies [46]. In one study, the cognitive tests were repeated at 10–15 min intervals up to 85 min after restoration of euglycaemia, and cognitive performance, for some tasks, was found to be only impaired for up to 10 min after euglycaemia was restored [6].
During the hypoglycaemic clamp, approximately 58% of participants answered ‘no’ to feeling hypoglycaemic, despite efforts made to exclude participants with impaired awareness of hypoglycaemia. Despite this, rCBF was significantly impacted during both cDSST and mDSST tasks. Indeed, both were associated with increased rCBF in frontal lobe regions (goal-directed action, behavioural control and problem solving) [34, 37] and the thalamus (relay station) and decreased rCBF in temporal lobe regions (memory functions). Furthermore, some regions that were deactivated during cDSST (and hence received reduced rCBF) were less affected during mDSST when working memory function was required; hence, rCBF was also higher in the basal ganglia and insula.
The effects of hypoglycaemia on rCBF in the thalamus, insula and globus pallidus were not statistically significantly different between cDSST and mDSST, but the effect in the striatum was significantly different and was highly uniform across participants, as indicated by the narrow confidence intervals (Fig. 3c).
In people with type 1 diabetes mellitus, studies have revealed increased CBF in the hypothalamus, brainstem, anterior cingulate cortex and putamen when reducing PG from 5.2 to 4.3 mmol/l. This suggests that these regions are sensitive to small blood glucose changes, and that even small sudden glycaemic changes may be of clinical importance [11]. In the current study, increased rCBF in the frontal lobe areas during hypoglycaemia could reflect the processing of input from the thalamus, which was also highly activated. Working memory was associated with increased blood flow in the striatum, a part of the basal ganglia thought to support motor functions, memory, language and emotion regulation. Increased rCBF in this region may suggest an increased support of this subcortical system during a task that is otherwise related to frontal regions, suggesting that the brain requires more resources to maintain performance during hypoglycaemia. A less efficient brain (i.e. recruiting more resources to preserve cognitive performance) during hypoglycaemia (PG ≤2.8 mmol/l) in individuals with type 1 diabetes mellitus was also shown by a previous study that used a working memory task during fMRI [9].

Study limitations

Regional blood flow averages did not allow testing for significant changes of blood flow in smaller parts of individual brain regions; this would require specific voxel-based analysis. A further limitation is that individual responses to hypoglycaemia can vary widely [38], as illustrated here by the finding that about half of participants had an impaired awareness of hypoglycaemia during the clamp and thus did not feel the impact of hypoglycaemia during the hypoglycaemic clamp. This may be attributed to the milder levels of hypoglycaemia achieved in the study (PG 2.9 mmol/l) and the comparable threshold for appearance of symptoms [39, 40]; however, it may also reflect the inclusion of participants with previously unrecognised unawareness and an inability to identify reduced awareness in everyday life. A more complete picture of potential impairments in counter-regulatory hormonal responses would have been afforded if adrenaline (epinephrine) had also been measured. The cDSST and mDSST tasks were not alternated and therefore the rCBF response to hypoglycaemia may be subject to time-dependent differences in activation. Although a time-dependent impact of hypoglycaemia on the brain has been suggested [29], H2 15O PET imaging for the cDSST and mDSST tasks commenced after the target hypoglycaemic PG was reached (0–20 min and 30–60 min, respectively). Therefore, effects of decreasing PG on the brain were not captured in this study and will limit any potential time-dependent effect. Finally, no cognitive dysfunction screening was carried out on participants prior to the study; however, cognitive problems related to working memory or other memory functions are not typically seen in adults with type 1 diabetes mellitus [41]. Despite these limitations the data still challenge the prevailing notion that working memory is not impaired until glucose levels are <2.8 mmol/l and shows that working memory is also impaired between 2.8 and 3.0 mmol/l, the glucose levels that generally reflect the lower and upper limits for the manifestation of hypoglycaemia symptoms [39, 40].
In conclusion, this hypothesis-driven study demonstrated that working memory performance and reaction times are adversely affected by hypoglycaemia in individuals with type 1 diabetes mellitus, and that they are associated with measurable effects on rCBF, even at mean PG concentrations of 2.9 mmol/l. The findings are clinically relevant, supporting the importance of reducing the risk of hypoglycaemic episodes at this level.

Acknowledgements

The expert technical assistance of A. Mengel, L. Egefjord, M. Bohl, J. Larsen, K. Vang, U. Sigsgaard, L.B. Vad, D.G. Zeidler and the staff at PET Center and Department of Anesthesiology, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark is gratefully acknowledged.
The authors thank the trial staff and participants. They also thank C. Rosano (Department of Epidemiology, University of Pittsburgh, PA, USA) and A. Howard (School of Medicine and Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, PA, USA) for providing the DSST tests, and C. Ryan (Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, PA, USA) for his valuable help interpreting the DSST results in the context of existing literature. The authors thank M. Gudmundsson, K. Kaplan and E. Hachmann-Nielsen (Novo Nordisk, Søborg, Denmark) for assistance with article preparation. The authors acknowledge medical writing and submission support provided by S. Mason, L. Brackenbury, G.F. Hansa-Wilkinson and D. Renshaw of Watermeadow Medical, an Ashfield company, part of UDG Healthcare. This support was funded by Novo Nordisk. Finally, N. Møller (Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark) is thanked for invaluable guidance regarding the clamp procedures.

Data availability

The data generated during and/or analysed during the current trial are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Funding

This study was funded by Novo Nordisk. Novo Nordisk was involved in the trial design and protocol development, provided logistical support, and obtained the data, which were evaluated jointly by the authors and the sponsor. All authors interpreted the data and wrote the manuscript together with the sponsor’s medical writing services. The authors had the final decision to approve and submit the manuscript for publication. M. Gejl was financially supported by the Department of Biomedicine, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark, and supported by grants of the Anniversary Fund for doctors and Health Research Fund of Central Denmark Region. E. van Duinkerken was supported by a grant of the Brazilian National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq).

Duality of interest

EvD reports consultancy for Novo Nordisk. HLH, CTH and KLSP are employees and shareholders at Novo Nordisk. PLC was an employee and stakeholder of Novo Nordisk throughout the duration of the study. MG, AG, BB, AM and JR declare that there is no duality of interest associated with their contribution to this manuscript.

Contribution statement

MG, BB and JR contributed to the trial design, participant recruitment, experiments, data interpretation, discussion of the results, and review and editing of the manuscript. PLC contributed to the statistical analyses, and review and editing of the manuscript. AG researched and analysed data, and revised the manuscript. AM contributed to the trial design, experiments, and revised the manuscript. KLSP, HLH and CTH contributed to the trial design, data interpretation, discussion of the results, and review and editing of the manuscript. EvD adapted the working memory test for use in the PET scanner, contributed to the interpretation of the results, and revised the manuscript. All authors provided approval of the version to be published. JR is the guarantor of this work and, as such, had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Innere Medizin

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Innere Medizin erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Innere Medizin, den Premium-Inhalten der internistischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten internistischen Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Allgemeinmedizin

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Allgemeinmedizin erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Premium-Inhalten der allgemeinmedizinischen Zeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Allgemeinmedizin-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Anhänge

Electronic supplementary material

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Allen KV, Pickering MJ, Zammitt NN et al (2015) Effects of acute hypoglycemia on working memory and language processing in adults with and without type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care 38:1108–1115CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Allen KV, Pickering MJ, Zammitt NN et al (2015) Effects of acute hypoglycemia on working memory and language processing in adults with and without type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care 38:1108–1115CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Graveling AJ, Deary IJ, Frier BM (2013) Acute hypoglycemia impairs executive cognitive function in adults with and without type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care 36:3240–3246CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Graveling AJ, Deary IJ, Frier BM (2013) Acute hypoglycemia impairs executive cognitive function in adults with and without type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care 36:3240–3246CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Inkster B, Frier BM (2012) The effects of acute hypoglycaemia on cognitive function in type 1 diabetes. Br J Diabetes Vasc Dis 12:221–226CrossRef Inkster B, Frier BM (2012) The effects of acute hypoglycaemia on cognitive function in type 1 diabetes. Br J Diabetes Vasc Dis 12:221–226CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Evans ML, Pernet A, Lomas J, Jones J, Amiel SA (2000) Delay in onset of awareness of acute hypoglycemia and of restoration of cognitive performance during recovery. Diabetes Care 23:893–897CrossRefPubMed Evans ML, Pernet A, Lomas J, Jones J, Amiel SA (2000) Delay in onset of awareness of acute hypoglycemia and of restoration of cognitive performance during recovery. Diabetes Care 23:893–897CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Gold AE, MacLeod KM, Deary IJ, Frier BM (1995) Hypoglycemia-induced cognitive dysfunction in diabetes mellitus: effect of hypoglycemia unawareness. Physiol Behav 58:501–511CrossRefPubMed Gold AE, MacLeod KM, Deary IJ, Frier BM (1995) Hypoglycemia-induced cognitive dysfunction in diabetes mellitus: effect of hypoglycemia unawareness. Physiol Behav 58:501–511CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Zammitt NN, Warren RE, Deary IJ, Frier BM (2008) Delayed recovery of cognitive function following hypoglycemia in adults with type 1 diabetes: effect of impaired awareness of hypoglycemia. Diabetes 57:732–736CrossRefPubMed Zammitt NN, Warren RE, Deary IJ, Frier BM (2008) Delayed recovery of cognitive function following hypoglycemia in adults with type 1 diabetes: effect of impaired awareness of hypoglycemia. Diabetes 57:732–736CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Blackman JD, Towle VL, Sturis J, Flewis G, Spire J-P, Polonsky KS (1992) Hypoglycemic thresholds for cognitive dysfunction in IDDM. Diabetes 41:392CrossRefPubMed Blackman JD, Towle VL, Sturis J, Flewis G, Spire J-P, Polonsky KS (1992) Hypoglycemic thresholds for cognitive dysfunction in IDDM. Diabetes 41:392CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Gonder-Frederick LA, Cox DJ, Driesen NR, Ryan CM, Clarke WL (1994) Individual differences in neurobehavioral disruption during mild and moderate hypoglycemia in adults with IDDM. Diabetes 43:1407–1412CrossRefPubMed Gonder-Frederick LA, Cox DJ, Driesen NR, Ryan CM, Clarke WL (1994) Individual differences in neurobehavioral disruption during mild and moderate hypoglycemia in adults with IDDM. Diabetes 43:1407–1412CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Bolo NR, Musen G, Jacobson AM et al (2011) Brain activation during working memory is altered in patients with type 1 diabetes during hypoglycemia. Diabetes 60:3256–3264CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bolo NR, Musen G, Jacobson AM et al (2011) Brain activation during working memory is altered in patients with type 1 diabetes during hypoglycemia. Diabetes 60:3256–3264CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Mangia S, Tesfaye N, De Martino F et al (2012) Hypoglycemia-induced increases in thalamic cerebral blood flow are blunted in subjects with type 1 diabetes and hypoglycemia unawareness. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 32:2084–2090CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Mangia S, Tesfaye N, De Martino F et al (2012) Hypoglycemia-induced increases in thalamic cerebral blood flow are blunted in subjects with type 1 diabetes and hypoglycemia unawareness. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 32:2084–2090CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Page KA, Arora J, Qiu M, Relwani R, Constable RT, Sherwin RS (2009) Small decrements in systemic glucose provoke increases in hypothalamic blood flow prior to the release of counterregulatory hormones. Diabetes 58:448–452CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Page KA, Arora J, Qiu M, Relwani R, Constable RT, Sherwin RS (2009) Small decrements in systemic glucose provoke increases in hypothalamic blood flow prior to the release of counterregulatory hormones. Diabetes 58:448–452CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Vafaee MS, Vang K, Bergersen LH, Gjedde A (2012) Oxygen consumption and blood flow coupling in human motor cortex during intense finger tapping: implication for a role of lactate. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 32:1859–1868CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Vafaee MS, Vang K, Bergersen LH, Gjedde A (2012) Oxygen consumption and blood flow coupling in human motor cortex during intense finger tapping: implication for a role of lactate. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 32:1859–1868CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat van Golen LW, Huisman MC, Ijzerman RG et al (2013) Cerebral blood flow and glucose metabolism measured with positron emission tomography are decreased in human type 1 diabetes. Diabetes 62:2898–2904CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral van Golen LW, Huisman MC, Ijzerman RG et al (2013) Cerebral blood flow and glucose metabolism measured with positron emission tomography are decreased in human type 1 diabetes. Diabetes 62:2898–2904CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Kameyama M, Murakami K, Jinzaki M (2016) Comparison of [(15)O] H(2)O positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging in activation studies. World J Nucl Med 15:3–6PubMedPubMedCentral Kameyama M, Murakami K, Jinzaki M (2016) Comparison of [(15)O] H(2)O positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging in activation studies. World J Nucl Med 15:3–6PubMedPubMedCentral
15.
Zurück zum Zitat International Conference on Harmonisation of Technical Requirements for Registration of Pharmaceuticals for Human Use (1996) ICH harmonised tripartite guideline: guideline for good clinical practice E6 (R1). International Conference on Harmonisation, Geneva International Conference on Harmonisation of Technical Requirements for Registration of Pharmaceuticals for Human Use (1996) ICH harmonised tripartite guideline: guideline for good clinical practice E6 (R1). International Conference on Harmonisation, Geneva
16.
Zurück zum Zitat World Medical Association (2013) WMA Declaration of Helsinki - Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. 64th WMA General Assembly, October 2013, Fortaleza, Brazil World Medical Association (2013) WMA Declaration of Helsinki - Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. 64th WMA General Assembly, October 2013, Fortaleza, Brazil
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D (1981) WAIS-R manual. The Psychological Corporation, New York Wechsler D (1981) WAIS-R manual. The Psychological Corporation, New York
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Salthouse TA, Fristoe N, Rhee SH (1996) How localized are age-related effects on neuropsychological measures? Neuropsychology 10:272–285CrossRef Salthouse TA, Fristoe N, Rhee SH (1996) How localized are age-related effects on neuropsychological measures? Neuropsychology 10:272–285CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Venkatraman VK, Aizenstein H, Guralnik J et al (2010) Executive control function, brain activation and white matter hyperintensities in older adults. NeuroImage 49:3436–3442CrossRefPubMed Venkatraman VK, Aizenstein H, Guralnik J et al (2010) Executive control function, brain activation and white matter hyperintensities in older adults. NeuroImage 49:3436–3442CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Gronwall DM (1977) Paced auditory serial-addition task: a measure of recovery from concussion. Percept Mot Skills 44:367–373CrossRefPubMed Gronwall DM (1977) Paced auditory serial-addition task: a measure of recovery from concussion. Percept Mot Skills 44:367–373CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Schachinger H, Cox D, Linder L, Brody S, Keller U (2003) Cognitive and psychomotor function in hypoglycemia: response error patterns and retest reliability. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 75:915–920CrossRefPubMed Schachinger H, Cox D, Linder L, Brody S, Keller U (2003) Cognitive and psychomotor function in hypoglycemia: response error patterns and retest reliability. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 75:915–920CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Deary IJ, Hepburn DA, MacLeod KM, Frier BM (1993) Partitioning the symptoms of hypoglycaemia using multi-sample confirmatory factor analysis. Diabetologia 36:771–777CrossRefPubMed Deary IJ, Hepburn DA, MacLeod KM, Frier BM (1993) Partitioning the symptoms of hypoglycaemia using multi-sample confirmatory factor analysis. Diabetologia 36:771–777CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Kannurpatti SS, Motes MA, Rypma B, Biswal BB (2010) Neural and vascular variability and the fMRI-BOLD response in normal aging. Magn Reson Imaging 28:466–476CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kannurpatti SS, Motes MA, Rypma B, Biswal BB (2010) Neural and vascular variability and the fMRI-BOLD response in normal aging. Magn Reson Imaging 28:466–476CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakahachi T, Ishii R, Iwase M et al (2008) Frontal activity during the digit symbol substitution test determined by multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuropsychobiology 57:151–158CrossRefPubMed Nakahachi T, Ishii R, Iwase M et al (2008) Frontal activity during the digit symbol substitution test determined by multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuropsychobiology 57:151–158CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Usui N, Haji T, Maruyama M et al (2009) Cortical areas related to performance of WAIS Digit Symbol Test: a functional imaging study. Neurosci Lett 463:1–5CrossRefPubMed Usui N, Haji T, Maruyama M et al (2009) Cortical areas related to performance of WAIS Digit Symbol Test: a functional imaging study. Neurosci Lett 463:1–5CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Bie-Olsen LG, Kjaer TW, Pedersen-Bjergaard U et al (2009) Changes of cognition and regional cerebral activity during acute hypoglycemia in normal subjects: a H2 15O positron emission tomographic study. J Neurosci Res 87:1922–1928CrossRefPubMed Bie-Olsen LG, Kjaer TW, Pedersen-Bjergaard U et al (2009) Changes of cognition and regional cerebral activity during acute hypoglycemia in normal subjects: a H2 15O positron emission tomographic study. J Neurosci Res 87:1922–1928CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenthal JM, Amiel SA, Yaguez L et al (2001) The effect of acute hypoglycemia on brain function and activation: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Diabetes 50:1618–1626CrossRefPubMed Rosenthal JM, Amiel SA, Yaguez L et al (2001) The effect of acute hypoglycemia on brain function and activation: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Diabetes 50:1618–1626CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Schafer RJ, Page KA, Arora J, Sherwin R, Constable RT (2012) BOLD response to semantic and syntactic processing during hypoglycemia is load-dependent. Brain Lang 120:1–14CrossRefPubMed Schafer RJ, Page KA, Arora J, Sherwin R, Constable RT (2012) BOLD response to semantic and syntactic processing during hypoglycemia is load-dependent. Brain Lang 120:1–14CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Teh MM, Dunn JT, Choudhary P et al (2010) Evolution and resolution of human brain perfusion responses to the stress of induced hypoglycemia. NeuroImage 53:584–592CrossRefPubMed Teh MM, Dunn JT, Choudhary P et al (2010) Evolution and resolution of human brain perfusion responses to the stress of induced hypoglycemia. NeuroImage 53:584–592CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Teves D, Videen TO, Cryer PE, Powers WJ (2004) Activation of human medial prefrontal cortex during autonomic responses to hypoglycemia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:6217–6221CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Teves D, Videen TO, Cryer PE, Powers WJ (2004) Activation of human medial prefrontal cortex during autonomic responses to hypoglycemia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:6217–6221CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Watanabe Y, Funahashi S (2012) Thalamic mediodorsal nucleus and working memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:134–142CrossRefPubMed Watanabe Y, Funahashi S (2012) Thalamic mediodorsal nucleus and working memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:134–142CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Yee LT, Roe K, Courtney SM (2010) Selective involvement of superior frontal cortex during working memory for shapes. J Neurophysiol 103:557–563CrossRefPubMed Yee LT, Roe K, Courtney SM (2010) Selective involvement of superior frontal cortex during working memory for shapes. J Neurophysiol 103:557–563CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Zarahn E, Rakitin B, Abela D, Flynn J, Stern Y (2005) Positive evidence against human hippocampal involvement in working memory maintenance of familiar stimuli. Cereb Cortex 15:303–316CrossRefPubMed Zarahn E, Rakitin B, Abela D, Flynn J, Stern Y (2005) Positive evidence against human hippocampal involvement in working memory maintenance of familiar stimuli. Cereb Cortex 15:303–316CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat van Duinkerken E, Schoonheim MM, Steenwijk MD et al (2014) Ventral striatum, but not cortical volume loss, is related to cognitive dysfunction in type 1 diabetic patients with and without microangiopathy. Diabetes Care 37:2483–2490CrossRefPubMed van Duinkerken E, Schoonheim MM, Steenwijk MD et al (2014) Ventral striatum, but not cortical volume loss, is related to cognitive dysfunction in type 1 diabetic patients with and without microangiopathy. Diabetes Care 37:2483–2490CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat International Hypoglycaemia Study Group (2017) Glucose concentrations of less than 3.0 mmol/l (54 mg/dl) should be reported in clinical trials: a joint position statement of the American Diabetes Association and the European Association for the Study of Diabetes. Diabetologia 60:3–6CrossRef International Hypoglycaemia Study Group (2017) Glucose concentrations of less than 3.0 mmol/l (54 mg/dl) should be reported in clinical trials: a joint position statement of the American Diabetes Association and the European Association for the Study of Diabetes. Diabetologia 60:3–6CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Mitrakou A, Ryan C, Veneman T et al (1991) Hierarchy of glycemic thresholds for counterregulatory hormone secretion, symptoms, and cerebral dysfunction. Am J Phys 260:E67–E74CrossRef Mitrakou A, Ryan C, Veneman T et al (1991) Hierarchy of glycemic thresholds for counterregulatory hormone secretion, symptoms, and cerebral dysfunction. Am J Phys 260:E67–E74CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Stuss DT (2011) Functions of the frontal lobes: relation to executive functions. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 17:759–765CrossRefPubMed Stuss DT (2011) Functions of the frontal lobes: relation to executive functions. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 17:759–765CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Wyatt JP, Illingworth RN, Graham CA, Hogg K (2012) Oxford handbook of emergency medicine. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRef Wyatt JP, Illingworth RN, Graham CA, Hogg K (2012) Oxford handbook of emergency medicine. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Kenny C (2014) When hypoglycemia is not obvious: diagnosing and treating under-recognized and undisclosed hypoglycemia. Prim Care Diabetes 8:3–11CrossRefPubMed Kenny C (2014) When hypoglycemia is not obvious: diagnosing and treating under-recognized and undisclosed hypoglycemia. Prim Care Diabetes 8:3–11CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Sprague JE, Arbelaez AM (2011) Glucose counterregulatory responses to hypoglycemia. Pediatr Endocrinol Rev 9:463–473PubMedPubMedCentral Sprague JE, Arbelaez AM (2011) Glucose counterregulatory responses to hypoglycemia. Pediatr Endocrinol Rev 9:463–473PubMedPubMedCentral
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Brands AM, Biessels GJ, de Haan EH, Kappelle LJ, Kessels RP (2005) The effects of type 1 diabetes on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Diabetes Care 28:726–735CrossRefPubMed Brands AM, Biessels GJ, de Haan EH, Kappelle LJ, Kessels RP (2005) The effects of type 1 diabetes on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Diabetes Care 28:726–735CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Effects of hypoglycaemia on working memory and regional cerebral blood flow in type 1 diabetes: a randomised, crossover trial
verfasst von
Michael Gejl
Albert Gjedde
Birgitte Brock
Arne Møller
Eelco van Duinkerken
Hanne L. Haahr
Charlotte T. Hansen
Pei-Ling Chu
Kirstine L. Stender-Petersen
Jørgen Rungby
Publikationsdatum
29.11.2017
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Diabetologia / Ausgabe 3/2018
Print ISSN: 0012-186X
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-0428
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00125-017-4502-1

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2018

Diabetologia 3/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Innere Medizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Innere Medizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.