Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BioPsychoSocial Medicine 1/2018

Open Access 01.12.2018 | Short report

Exaggerated envy and guilt measured by economic games in Japanese women with anorexia nervosa

verfasst von: Masanori Isobe, Michiko Kawabata, Ema Murao, Tomomi Noda, Noriko Matsukawa, Ryosaku Kawada, Teruhisa Uwatoko, Toshiya Murai, Shun’ichi Noma, Hidehiko Takahashi

Erschienen in: BioPsychoSocial Medicine | Ausgabe 1/2018

Abstract

Background

Anorexia nervosa (AN) patients are assumed to express high levels of guilt and envy. Ultimatum game (UG) is a standard behavioral task that focuses on interpersonal behavior when splitting a sum of money between two players. UG studies consistently demonstrate that people tend to decrease their inequity in outcomes, one explanation being that economically irrational decision-making may partly arise from the emotions guilt and envy. We assumed that AN patients would perform excessively fair in UG, reflecting high guilt and envy.

Methods

We utilized UG to investigate the characteristics of guilt and envy among 24 Japanese AN patients and 22 age-matched healthy controls (HC). The relation between the outcome of UG and decision strategy confirmed by post-experimental questionnaires was analyzed.

Results

As proposer, AN offered a larger amount to the responder compared with HC (p = 0.002) while, on the other hand, as responder, AN demanded much higher allocation to accept the offer compared with HC (p = 0.026). Regarding the strategy as responder, AN put more emphasis on fairness and less emphasis on monetary reward compared with HC (p = 0.046, p = 0.042, respectively).

Conclusions

The results indicate that Japanese AN patients demonstrate strong preference for fairness, with high guilt and high envy. High sensitivity to guilt and envy of AN patients can affect not only their own behavior concerning eating attitude and body shape, but also decision-making in interpersonal situations. Behavioral experimental settings among social situations will enable us to evaluate and help actual decision-making in the real life of patients.
Abkürzungen
AN
Anorexia nervosa
ANBP
Anorexia nervosa, binge-eating/purging type
ANR
Anorexia nervosa, restricting type
BMI
Body mass index
EDE-Q
Eating Disorder Examination-questionnaire 6.0
HC
Healthy controls
SCID
Structural clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders
UG
Ultimatum game

Introduction

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is characterized by distorted body image and excessive dieting [1]. Individuals with AN feel guilt for “indulgent” eating or loss of control in an eating setting [2], and they feel envy when they see someone very slim [3]. In this manner, those with AN are assumed to express high levels of guilt and envy [4, 5]. However, these emotional reactions can be observed not only in food/body contexts but also in more general situations. Adolescents with AN showed more increased guilt than healthy ones [6], and increased envy may have roots deep in the psychopathology of AN [7].
To understand these emotions, economics games, which can assess decision-making in social situations [8] and provide a quantitative value to examining the psychopathology by predicting optimal adaptation to a changing environment [9], have been used in psychiatric populations [10, 11]. In particular, ultimatum game (UG), focusing on interpersonal behavior when splitting a sum of money between two players, is one of the most widely used tasks, as guilt and envy are addressed with this task [12]. The proposer suggests the distribution amount, and then the responder decides to accept or reject the proposal. If the responder accepts, the proposed distribution is the final allocation for the two players. If the responder rejects the proposal, both players receive nothing. Although theory predicts that the responder accepts all offers and that the proposer offers the smallest amount [13], behavioral economics consistently demonstrates that people do not necessarily maximize their allocation [14]. It has been assumed that people tend to minimize inequity in outcomes [11]. One explanation for such behavior is that guilt and envy play roles in the game. Rejecting low offers in UG implies envy, which is the preference to prevent the opposing person earning more than oneself, and offering equal allocation is derived from the desire to minimize guilt of having too much [15]. Given that both guilt and envy could be measured in the two different roles of UG (proposer, responder), we decided to use UG with the aim of quantifying these emotions in social situations among AN patients. We assumed that behaviors with exaggerated envy and guilt could be detected in AN patients, resulting in a high rejection rate of low offers and egalitarian allocation.

Method

Participants

The AN patient group consisted of 24 Japanese female patients who were referred to the Department of Psychiatry of Kyoto University Hospital. Each patient fulfilled the criteria for AN based on the Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID) Patient Edition. Twelve patients were classified as the restricting subtype (ANR) and 12 as the binge-eating/purging subtype (ANBP). All the patients were also screened with the SCID Patient edition about any psychiatric comorbidity, and none were found to be comorbid with any other psychiatric disorders. Predicted premorbid IQ was measured by the Japanese Version of the National Adult Reading Test short form (JART) [16], and eating behavior was measured by Eating Disorder Examination-questionnaire 6.0 (EDE-Q). The HC group consisted of 22 Japanese healthy female individuals age-matched to the AN group. They were evaluated using the SCID Non-patient Edition, and were found to have no history of psychiatric disease. All participants of both groups were physically healthy at the time of participation, and had no history of neurological injury or disease, severe medical disease, or substance abuse.

Ultimatum game

Participants acted as both proposer and responder in 20 trials each. The participants were told that the total sum was 1000 Japanese Yen (JPY; approximately 9 US Dollars), and that they had to divide this sum between self and the partner. All the tasks were performed by computer. First, they played the role of responder. Prior to the task, they were told that “the preceding participants had proposed the distribution in advance,” and they were asked to accept or to reject the distribution. The proposed amounts were 100, 200, 300, 400 and 500 JPY, and each offer was proposed four times. Next, they were asked to provide 20 proposals of distribution amount as proposer for a latter unknown participant. The participants were told that one trial would be randomly chosen through all the tasks, and they would be paid according to its allocation. Therefore, they received no feedback from a responder. We defined “60:40” and “50:50” as “fair offer”, and “90:10”, “80:20” and “70:30” as “unfair offer” based on a previous study [17].

Analysis of UG data

We compared the acceptance rates of “fair offer” and “unfair offer” in terms of a responder. The smallest acceptable offered amount of each participant was also compared between groups. The mean distribution amount of proposed offers was adopted as a proposer index.

Post-experimental questionnaires for strategy

Post-experimental questionnaires to confirm comprehension and strategy of the experiments were performed. In the questionnaires, the decision strategies selected according to findings of previous studies were queried using a 7-point Likert scale (1: “very low” - 7: “very high”) in the following six items: fairness, hostility, reputation, high-mindedness, disregard aversion, and monetary gain [1821]. Comparing the distribution of the strategy in individuals, subtraction of the mean score of the six items from each raw score was separately calculated for all three roles in order to attenuate the variance of the scoring method. Between-group differences were examined using two-tailed independent sample t-tests (p < 0.05). Because we selected these items on the basis of the hypotheses, we did not perform any correction for multiple comparisons. Correlation analyses between the offered amount and each strategy were also examined using Pearson’s correlation coefficients with significance defined as p < 0.05. The main purpose of these correlation analyses was to disclose that participants properly understood the task. We predicted that participants who emphasize monetary gain would keep higher amounts for themselves, and that those who emphasize fairness would allocate the amount more fairly.

Results

Demographics

There was no significant difference in IQ between the HC and AN groups (Table 1). Body mass index (BMI) was much lower (p < 0.001) and the global score and subscales of the EDE-Q [22] were significantly higher in the AN group.
Table 1
Comparisons of the demographics and clinical characteristics in patients with anorexia nervosa and healthy controls
 
Mean (SD, range) or Number
p value
HC
AN
participants
22
24
 
ANR:12
ANBP:12
age
34.59 (9.7, 20–54)
35.9 (10.0, 20–56)
.65
BMI (kg/m2)
21.8 (3.4, 16.5–28.2)
14.3 (2.7, 10.1–20.7)
< .001
 
ANR:
12.4 (1.7, 10.1–13.8)
ANBP:
16.1 (2.3, 13.7–20.7)
 
JART
105.3 (8.1)
102.2 (9.1)
.31
EDE-Q
 Global score
0.7 (0.6)
2.2 (1.8)
< .001
 Restraint
0.4 (0.7)
1.7 (1.9)
.006
 Eating Concern
0.1 (0.1)
2.0 (1.9)
< .001
 Shape Concern
1.3 (1.0)
2.9 (1.9)
.001
 Weight Concern
1.0 (0.9)
2.3 (1.8)
.003
Duration of Illness (years)
16.7 (9.3, 5–41)
 
ANR:
11.4 (6.5, 5–30)
ANBP:
22.0 (8.6, 6–41)
 
Two-tailed p-values of two-sample t-test were calculated in all analyses
SD standard deviation, HC Healthy Control, AN Anorexia Nervosa, ANR Anorexia Nervosa Restricting Type, ANBP Anorexia Nervosa Binge-Eating/Purging Type, BMI Body Mass Index, JART Japanese Version of the National Adult Reading Test short form, EDE-Q Eating Disorder Examination-questionnaire 6.0

Task performance

Participants were asked about their understanding of the task using a 7-point Likert scale (1: “Not at all” - 7: “Completely”). There was no significant difference between the 2 groups, indicating a similar understanding of tasks (HC mean: 6.77, AN mean: 6.44). No significant difference was detected in the acceptance rates of ‘fair offer’, and the AN group had a tendency to reject ‘unfair offer’ more frequently than HC (Table 2). The smallest acceptable offered amount by AN was significantly higher than that by HC. As proposer, the AN group offered a significantly higher amount to the opponent than HC. Decision-making strategies of the two roles are shown in Fig. 1a and b. When playing the role of responder, the AN group put more emphasis on fairness, and in contrast, less on monetary gain compared with the HC group (p = 0.046, p = 0.042, respectively) (Fig. 1a, b). The mean offered amount of the HC group showed positive correlation with emphasis on ‘fairness’ (r = 0.61, p = 0.003) and negative correlation with emphasis on ‘monetary gain’ (r = 0.47, p = 0.028), and that of the AN group showed no significant correlation with either item (‘fairness’: r = 0.31, p = 0.14; ‘monetary gain’: r = 0.33, p = 0.12) (Fig. 1c). The results of correlation analyses in the HC group were consistent with our assumption. We could not find any significant difference between the correlation coefficient of HC and that of AN, neither in ‘fairness’ nor in ‘monetary gain’ (‘fairness’: p = 0.22, ‘monetary gain’: p = 0.60).
Table 2
Comparison of the behavioral data of the ultimatum game in patients with anorexia nervosa and healthy controls
 
Mean (SD)
p value
HC
AN
UG responder
 Acceptance rate of ‘unfair offer’ (%)
70.8 (34.6)
51.0 (41.8)
.089
 Acceptance rate of ‘fair offer’ (%)
92.6 (14.2)
93.8 (16.9)
.81
 Minimum acceptable amount (JPY)
159.1 (85.4)
241.7 (147.2)
.026
UG proposer
 Offered amount (JPY)
314.8 (98.6)
402.1 (79.5)
.002
Two-tailed p-values of two-sample t-test were calculated in all analyses
HC Healthy Control, AN Anorexia Nervosa, UG Ultimatum Game, JPY Japanese Yen

Discussion

We detected significant differences in behavior and strategy between Japanese female AN and matched HC in UG. The results of UG indicate that excessive offer amount and excessive rejection rate reflect guilt and envy, respectively [23]. The proposer of UG has to decide the distribution, the conflict being between self-profit and preference for fairness [18, 23]. As decisive factors for the responder’s behavior, altruistic punishment and inequity aversion underlie the rejection of an unfair offer [24]. The results of the AN group indicate that the higher distribution amount as proposer and the lower accepting behavior as responder may reflect a strong preference for fairness. The decision-making strategy of responders also showed that AN patients placed great emphasis on fairness.
One explanation for the strong preference of the AN patients for fairness might be linked to the social emotions guilt and envy. In UG, individuals with more guilt were found to propose fair allocation, and those with more envy tended to reject unfair offer [25]. Eating disorder patients are believed to demonstrate high sensitivity to guilt and envy, which are not limited to eating settings [4, 5]. Therefore, exaggerated guilt and envy of AN patients can affect not only their own behavior concerning eating attitude and body shape, but also decision-making in interpersonal situations.
Another explanation for the strong preference for fairness is the fact that AN patients show psychological inflexibility motivated by demand of certainty and fear of social rejection and negative evaluation by others [26]. This inflexibility leads to over-reliance on social norms and rules for behavior to reduce ambiguity/risk and mistakes. In fact, the risk-aversive trait was detected in a decision-making study of AN [27]. AN patients may have chosen ‘excessive’ fair allocation as a more certain condition in UG in order to avoid blame. AN patients may stick to the normative principle that people ‘should’ follow and consequently make inflexible decisions in interpersonal situations. This notion might partly explain the result that, despite the exaggerated emphasis on fairness, it was not positively correlated with the mean offered amount by the proposer in AN, whereas HC showed a rational positive correlation between them.
Several limitations of the current study need to be considered. First, as the number of participants was limited, we must exercise caution regarding interpretation of the results until similar behavioral experiments can be performed with larger samples of AN patients. Second, for a better understanding of the decision-making process linked to the psychopathology of AN, the development of decision-making experiments based on other than monetary rewards is strongly awaited. Third, we selected the items of post-experimental questionnaires with strong hypothesis on the basis of previous studies and the results were reasonable and in agreement with our hypothesis. However, unfortunately, we did not perform correction for multiple comparisons. Future replication studies are recommended.
Given that poor decisions are related to poor real-life outcomes, intervention targeting impaired decision-making may improve clinical outcomes and quality of life [28]. For instance, neuroimaging studies have demonstrated the neural correlates of guilt and envy [e.g., medial prefrontal cortex [29, 30] and anterior cingulate [31], respectively], and malfunctions of these midline structures were repeatedly reported in AN [32]. Further investigations of economic behavioral experiments concurrent with neuroimaging are urgently required. Moreover, neuromodulation targeting these emotions also merits further investigation. In conclusion, behavioral economics tools are useful for the evaluation of the altered emotions and decision-making that are rooted deep in the psychopathology of AN.

Acknowledgements

Funding

This work was supported by grants-in-aid for scientific research A (24243061), C (15 K09864), and on Innovative Areas (23120009, 16H06572), from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan (MEXT); Grants-in-Aid for Young Scientists A (23680045) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS), a Japan-US Brain Research Cooperation Program, and the Takeda Science Foundation. A part of this study is the result of the Strategic Research Program for Brain Sciences (JP18dm0107151) by the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development and the Joint Usage/Research Program of the Medical Institute of Developmental Disabilities Research, Showa University.

Availability of data and materials

Datasets analyzed in the current study are available upon reasonable request.
This study was approved by the Committee on Medical Ethics of Kyoto University, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat APA. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders : DSM-5, vol.: hardcover. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub; 2013. APA. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders : DSM-5, vol.: hardcover. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub; 2013.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Frank ES. Shame and guilt in eating disorders. Am J Orthop. 1991;61:303–6.CrossRef Frank ES. Shame and guilt in eating disorders. Am J Orthop. 1991;61:303–6.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Davies M. Need, greed and envy in anorexia nervosa and the challenge they present. Psychoanal Psychother. 2004;18:432–44.CrossRef Davies M. Need, greed and envy in anorexia nervosa and the challenge they present. Psychoanal Psychother. 2004;18:432–44.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Troop NA, Allan S, Treasure JL, Katzman M. Social comparison and submissive behaviour in eating disorder patients. Psychol Psychother. 2003;76:237–49.CrossRef Troop NA, Allan S, Treasure JL, Katzman M. Social comparison and submissive behaviour in eating disorder patients. Psychol Psychother. 2003;76:237–49.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Burney J, Irwin HJ. Shame and guilt in women with eating-disorder symptomatology. J Clin Psychol. 2000;56:51–61.CrossRef Burney J, Irwin HJ. Shame and guilt in women with eating-disorder symptomatology. J Clin Psychol. 2000;56:51–61.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Berghold KM, Lock J. Assessing guilt in adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychother. 2002;56:378–90.CrossRef Berghold KM, Lock J. Assessing guilt in adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychother. 2002;56:378–90.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Boris HN. The problem of anorexia nervosa. Int J Psychoanal. 1984;65(Pt 3):315–22.PubMed Boris HN. The problem of anorexia nervosa. Int J Psychoanal. 1984;65(Pt 3):315–22.PubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Rilling JK, Sanfey AG. The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annu Rev Psychol. 2011;62:23–48.CrossRef Rilling JK, Sanfey AG. The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annu Rev Psychol. 2011;62:23–48.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Sharp C, Monterosso J, Montague PR. Neuroeconomics: a bridge for translational research. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72:87–92.CrossRef Sharp C, Monterosso J, Montague PR. Neuroeconomics: a bridge for translational research. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72:87–92.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Takahashi H. Molecular neuroimaging of emotional decision-making. Neurosci Res. 2013;75:269–74.CrossRef Takahashi H. Molecular neuroimaging of emotional decision-making. Neurosci Res. 2013;75:269–74.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Fehr E, Schmidt KM. A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Q J Econ. 1999;114:817–68.CrossRef Fehr E, Schmidt KM. A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Q J Econ. 1999;114:817–68.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Ketelaar T, Tung AW. The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: an affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cogn Emot. 2003;17:429–53.CrossRef Ketelaar T, Tung AW. The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: an affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cogn Emot. 2003;17:429–53.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Güth W, Schmittberger R, Schwarze B. An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. J Econ Behav Organ. 1982;3:367–88.CrossRef Güth W, Schmittberger R, Schwarze B. An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. J Econ Behav Organ. 1982;3:367–88.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Camerer CF. Behavioral game theory : experiments in strategic interaction. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2003. Camerer CF. Behavioral game theory : experiments in strategic interaction. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2003.
15.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Matsuoka K, Uno M, Kasai K, Koyama K, Kim Y. Estimation of premorbid IQ in individuals with Alzheimer's disease using Japanese ideographic script (kanji) compound words: Japanese version of National Adult Reading Test. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;60:332–9.CrossRef Matsuoka K, Uno M, Kasai K, Koyama K, Kim Y. Estimation of premorbid IQ in individuals with Alzheimer's disease using Japanese ideographic script (kanji) compound words: Japanese version of National Adult Reading Test. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;60:332–9.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Csukly G, Polgár P, Tombor L, Réthelyi J, Kéri S. Are patients with schizophrenia rational maximizers? Evidence from an ultimatum game study. Psychiatry Res. 2011;187:11–7.CrossRef Csukly G, Polgár P, Tombor L, Réthelyi J, Kéri S. Are patients with schizophrenia rational maximizers? Evidence from an ultimatum game study. Psychiatry Res. 2011;187:11–7.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat van Dijk E, Vermunt R. Strategy and fairness in social decision making: sometimes it pays to be powerless. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2000;36:1–25.CrossRef van Dijk E, Vermunt R. Strategy and fairness in social decision making: sometimes it pays to be powerless. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2000;36:1–25.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Dana J, Cain DM, Dawes RM. What you don't know won't hurt me: costly (but quiet) exit in dictator games. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2006;100:193–201.CrossRef Dana J, Cain DM, Dawes RM. What you don't know won't hurt me: costly (but quiet) exit in dictator games. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2006;100:193–201.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Hashimoto H, Mifune N, Yamagishi T. To be perceived as altruistic: strategic considerations that support fair behavior in the dictator game. Lett Evolut Behav Sci. 2014;5:17–20. Hashimoto H, Mifune N, Yamagishi T. To be perceived as altruistic: strategic considerations that support fair behavior in the dictator game. Lett Evolut Behav Sci. 2014;5:17–20.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Oexl R, Grossman ZJ. Shifting the blame to a powerless intermediary. Exper Econ. 2013;16:306–12.CrossRef Oexl R, Grossman ZJ. Shifting the blame to a powerless intermediary. Exper Econ. 2013;16:306–12.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Fairburn CG. Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. Fairburn CG. Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders. New York: Guilford Press; 2008.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science. 2003;300:1755–8.CrossRef Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science. 2003;300:1755–8.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Halali E, Bereby-Meyer Y, Ockenfels A. Is it all about the self? The effect of self-control depletion on ultimatum game proposers. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:240.CrossRef Halali E, Bereby-Meyer Y, Ockenfels A. Is it all about the self? The effect of self-control depletion on ultimatum game proposers. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:240.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Krajbich I, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Denburg NL, Camerer CF. Economic games quantify diminished sense of guilt in patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2009;29:2188–92.CrossRef Krajbich I, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Denburg NL, Camerer CF. Economic games quantify diminished sense of guilt in patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2009;29:2188–92.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Merwin RM, Timko CA, Moskovich AA, Ingle KK, Bulik CM, Zucker NL. Psychological inflexibility and symptom expression in anorexia nervosa. Eat Disord. 2011;19:62–82.CrossRef Merwin RM, Timko CA, Moskovich AA, Ingle KK, Bulik CM, Zucker NL. Psychological inflexibility and symptom expression in anorexia nervosa. Eat Disord. 2011;19:62–82.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Adoue C, Jaussent I, Olie E, Beziat S, Van den Eynde F, Courtet P, Guillaume S. A further assessment of decision-making in anorexia nervosa. Eur Psychiatry. 2015;30:121–7.CrossRef Adoue C, Jaussent I, Olie E, Beziat S, Van den Eynde F, Courtet P, Guillaume S. A further assessment of decision-making in anorexia nervosa. Eur Psychiatry. 2015;30:121–7.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Caceda R, Nemeroff CB, Harvey PD. Toward an understanding of decision making in severe mental illness. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;26:196–213.CrossRef Caceda R, Nemeroff CB, Harvey PD. Toward an understanding of decision making in severe mental illness. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;26:196–213.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Takahashi H, Yahata N, Koeda M, Matsuda T, Asai K, Okubo Y. Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2004;23:967–74.CrossRef Takahashi H, Yahata N, Koeda M, Matsuda T, Asai K, Okubo Y. Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2004;23:967–74.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Gifuni AJ, Kendal A, Jollant F. Neural mapping of guilt: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Brain Imaging Behav. 2017;11:1164–78.CrossRef Gifuni AJ, Kendal A, Jollant F. Neural mapping of guilt: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Brain Imaging Behav. 2017;11:1164–78.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Takahashi H, Kato M, Matsuura M, Mobbs D, Suhara T, Okubo Y. When your gain is my pain and your pain is my gain: neural correlates of envy and schadenfreude. Science. 2009;323:937–9.CrossRef Takahashi H, Kato M, Matsuura M, Mobbs D, Suhara T, Okubo Y. When your gain is my pain and your pain is my gain: neural correlates of envy and schadenfreude. Science. 2009;323:937–9.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Esposito R, Cieri F, di Giannantonio M, Tartaro A. The role of body image and self-perception in anorexia nervosa: the neuroimaging perspective. J Neuropsychol. 2018;12:41–52.CrossRef Esposito R, Cieri F, di Giannantonio M, Tartaro A. The role of body image and self-perception in anorexia nervosa: the neuroimaging perspective. J Neuropsychol. 2018;12:41–52.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Exaggerated envy and guilt measured by economic games in Japanese women with anorexia nervosa
verfasst von
Masanori Isobe
Michiko Kawabata
Ema Murao
Tomomi Noda
Noriko Matsukawa
Ryosaku Kawada
Teruhisa Uwatoko
Toshiya Murai
Shun’ichi Noma
Hidehiko Takahashi
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2018
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BioPsychoSocial Medicine / Ausgabe 1/2018
Elektronische ISSN: 1751-0759
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13030-018-0138-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

BioPsychoSocial Medicine 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe