Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Journal of Nutrition 6/2016

Open Access 07.01.2016 | Review

Examining techniques for measuring the effects of nutrients on mental performance and mood state

verfasst von: Mark Hamer, Louise Dye, E. Siobhan Mitchell, Sophie Layé, Caroline Saunders, Neil Boyle, Jeroen Schuermans, John Sijben

Erschienen in: European Journal of Nutrition | Ausgabe 6/2016

Abstract

Purpose

Intake of specific nutrients has been linked to mental states and various indices of cognitive performance although the effects are often subtle and difficult to interpret. Measurement of so-called objective variables (e.g. reaction times) is often considered to be the gold standard for assessing outcomes in this field of research. It can, however, be argued that data on subjective experience (e.g. mood) are also important and may enrich existing objective data. The aim of this review is to evaluate methods for measuring mental performance and mood, considering the definition of subjective mood and the validity of measures of subjective experience.

Methods

A multi-stakeholder expert group was invited by ILSI Europe to come to a consensus around the utility of objective and subjective measurement in this field, which forms the basis of the paper. Therefore, the present review reflects a succinct overview of the science but is not intended to be a systematic review.

Results

The proposed approach extends the traditional methodology using standard ‘objective’ measurements to also include the consumers’ subjective experiences in relation to food. Specific recommendations include 1) using contemporary methods to capture transient mood states; 2) using sufficiently sensitive measures to capture effects of nutritional intervention; 3) considering the possibility that subjective and objective responses will occur over different time frames; and 4) recognition of the importance of expectancy and placebo effects for subjective measures.

Conclusions

The consensus reached was that the most informative approach should involve collection and consideration of both objective and subjective data.
Abkürzungen
ANCOVA
Analysis of covariance
ANOVA
Analysis of variance
DHA
Docosahexaenoic acid
EMA
Ecological momentary assessment
ESM
Experience sampling methods
fMRI
Functional magnetic resonance imaging
POMS
Profile of mood states
PUFA
Polyunsaturated fatty acid
RVIP
Rapid visual information processing
TSST
Trier social stress test

Introduction

Nutrition plays an important role in brain function throughout the entire lifespan. Appropriate nutrient intake allows normal brain growth and neurodevelopment in early life and reduces the risk of cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease in older age [1, 2]. Our knowledge about nutrition has greatly evolved over the last 50 years, and in developed countries, food is no longer seen uniquely as a survival necessity and a primary need, but is associated with promotion of mental fitness [35]. The effects of nutrients on cognitive performance—mental processes (e.g. attention, memory, reasoning, problem solving, decision-making) related to the processing of information and application of knowledge—have been widely examined usually by employing objective outcomes measures such as reaction time and accuracy. The effects of nutrition on subjective measures of psychological functions, such as self-reported mood, have received relatively less attention. The aim of this review is to provide a comprehensive overview of the theoretical frameworks that link subjective experience of mood to cognition and, from this understanding, identify the most promising methods to measure the individual experience of mood and its relationship to cognitive function. This will be illustrated with examples from studies that have examined the effects of nutritional interventions on cognitive performance and mood. However, the effects of nutritional manipulations are often subtle and inconsistently reported (e.g. Halyburton et al. [6]; Hellhammer et al. [7]; Rogers et al. [8]). Therefore, this review will also draw upon examples from the stress, cognition and mood literature, since stress induction [e.g. the use of sensory overload (excessive noise) or performing a task under social evaluation to provoke a psychophysiological stress response] is both robust and reliable in the provocation of mood change and moderation of cognitive function [9, 10]. Whilst stress response can be termed as any alteration to homoeostasis in an organism, for the purposes of this review, stress is defined as a mismatch between the demands of a task/context/stimuli and perceived ability to cope. If a context/stimuli is perceived as stressful, this will provoke a subjective response (e.g. negative affect/mood) and a physiological response (e.g. the release of the hormone cortisol to orchestrate the neuroendocrine response to stress). The main topics that will be addressed are: (1) how to best measure mental performance and mood, i.e. which measures are most reliable and validated under which conditions, (2) how placebo effects may affect subjective state and response to nutrient intervention, (3) the relationship between objective and subjective measures, (4) the ability to detect meaningful effects and elucidate their mechanism of action and (5) how nutrient effects on subjective experience, measured by standardised questionnaires, translate into everyday life and the clinical relevance of these. The proposed approach extends the traditional empirical approach of using solely standard “objective” measurements to also consider subjective state in a more integrated manner using advances in technology and appropriate analytical strategies. Such data on subjective experience may enrich our understanding and interpretation of objective measures and as such these recommendations are expected to help guide researchers from academia and industry when studying the effects that foods or specific product consumption can have on mood and mental performance.

Definition and measurement of subjective mood state

In behavioural sciences, measures of motor and cognitive behaviour (reaction time, number of words recalled) and physiological variables (salivary cortisol concentration, blood pressure, heart rate variability) are considered to be objective measures, in contrast to self-rating/self-reported measures of cognitive performance (e.g. participants’ self-rated perception of how accurately or quickly a task was performed) and mood, which are often referred to as subjective measures of an individual’s subjective experience. Mood can be described as a pervasive and predominant affective state and is commonly conceived to vary along orthogonal and bipolar dimensions of valence (positive vs negative) and arousal/activation (e.g. Russell [11]; Watson and Tellegen [12]). Mood can also be temporally separated into protracted (e.g. depressed mood) and transient, fluctuating affective states (e.g. a momentary state of increased vigour). As mood is inherently phenomenological, it is perceived as an inconsistent measure of the brain’s output, despite demonstrations that mood can be consistently measured. For example, specific drugs (e.g. Brown and Chandler [13]; Mula and Sander [14]) and stimuli (e.g. stress [15]) have been shown to have very consistent effects on mood, as measured by ratings on scales and questionnaires. In addition to well-being questionnaires, which often capture protracted, generalised affective mood states, there are several validated tools available to assess transient mood states which have been able to capture changes in mood as a result of a number of different nutritional interventions [16, 17]. For example, relative to placebo, a 30-day high-flavanol chocolate drink improved self-rated calmness and contentedness, measured by Bond–Lader mood scales [16] and the Profile of Mood States (POMS) captured changes in tension–anxiety scores following intervention with L-theanine under conditions of induced stress [17]. Table 1 provides examples of questionnaire/scale measures of subjective mood state and well-being and their potential suitability for nutritional interventions.
Table 1
Examples of measures of subjective mood state and well-being
Measure
Scales
Strengths and weaknesses
Reliability (Cronbach’s alpha [])
Bond–Lader visual analogue scale (BL-VAS) [69]
Subscales for calmness (encompassing individual questions on the extent of relaxation or calmness), contentedness (encompassing the emotions contented, tranquil, happy, amicable and gregarious) and alertness (encompassing alert, strong, well-coordinated, energetic, quick-witted, attentive, proficient, interested and clear-headed)
Using specifically selected subscales of common mood states, such as calmness and contentedness, it fails to capture a wide spectrum of other emotions, such as anger, or mind states that might not be viewed as strictly emotion, such as confusion or guilt
All scales:  = .77–.93 [70]
The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) [71]
Ten positive affects (interested, excited, strong, enthusiastic, proud, alert, inspired, determined, attentive, and active) and ten negative affects (distressed, upset, guilty, scared, hostile, irritable, ashamed, nervous, jittery, and afraid)
When measuring moods after food consumption or relating to nutrition, such diverse emotions as hostility or pride may not be useful. Simply asking such questions can induce differential emotions
All scales:  = .85–.90 [71]
The Profile of Mood States (POMS) [72]
A measure of six dimensions of transient and distinct mood states. The subscales are: tension/anxiety, vigour/activity, depression/dejection, fatigue/inertia, confusion/bewilderment, and anger/hostility. A total negative mood score can also be computed by adding up the five negative mood subscales and subtracting the positive scale vigour/activity
Widely used in cross-sectional studies and found to discriminate between different habitual diets; used in acute and chronic studies of intake of specific nutrients, vitamins, minerals and herbal supplements
Subscales:  = .63–.92; Total score:  = .75–.92 [73, 74]
World Health Organization Quality of Life Scale [75]
Comprises 26 items, which measure the following broad domains: physical health, psychological health, social relationships, and environment
Unlikely to pick up acute effects of food, but might reflect influence of habitual dietary patterns
Physical domain:  = .82; psychological domain:  = .81; social domain:  = .68; environmental domain:  = .80 [76]
The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) [77]
Comprises five items on satisfaction with life
Retrospective life experiences, may not be directly related to effects of diet
All scales:  = .61–.81 [77]
Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS) [78]
Four-item scale of global subjective happiness. Two items ask respondents to characterise themselves using both absolute ratings and ratings relative to peers, whereas the other two items offer brief descriptions of happy and unhappy individuals and ask respondents the extent to which each characterisation describes them
Measures only one dimension of mood, namely positive feelings
All scales:  = .74–.94 [78]
General Health Questionnaire (GHQ) [79]
Four subscales consisting of somatic symptoms, anxiety/insomnia, social dysfunction, and depression. (Multiple versions differing in number of items: GHQ-60, 30, 20, 28. 12)
Validated as a tool to assess mental health in large population samples; symptoms may be enduring and less likely to vary in relation to transient nutritional induced changes in subjective state
All versions:  ≥ .80 (e.g. Dale et al. [81], Carvalho et al. [80], Montazer et al. [82])
Moods can be linked to specific events, such as those caused by artificial mood inductions, but more commonly they occur somewhat independently of second-by-second, or minute-by-minute circumstances. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) or experience sampling methods (ESM) are alternative mood measurement tools which may more realistically capture sequential, transient states by requiring brief inputs of mood state during daily activities. EMA data can be captured, for example, via mobile devices such as smart phones, where participants are periodically asked to fill in short questionnaires. Psychological research has established that recollected effect may diverge from actual experience because it is influenced by errors in recollection, recall biases, focusing illusions and salient memory heuristics [18]. This “memory–experience gap” between life as it is remembered and life as it is experienced may be important to the processes through which the past impacts on future behaviour. Moreover, effects resulting from interventions with nutrients may not be noticeable after acute administrations commonly applied in laboratory settings, but may give rise to improved well-being over time, especially if the intervention has other benefits such as cardiovascular health. One recent study, which endeavoured to measure daily fluctuations in mood via cell phone questionnaires, reported an effect of a multivitamin supplement on alertness after 4-week supplementation [19]. Others have validated cardiovascular biomarkers against the mood reports acquired during daily life. For instance, Steptoe and colleagues found that systolic blood pressure correlated with positive effect in men during work hours [20].

Validity of measures of subjective states

There are numerous types of validity, but in this context, the most important is construct validity—are we actually measuring what we think we are measuring? Evaluation of construct validity involves demonstrating that the measure correlates with variables that are known to be related to the construct (purportedly measured by the instrument being evaluated or for which there are theoretical grounds for expecting it to be related). Thus, identifying the “gold standard” against which to validate is crucial. Employing the stress induction model to illustrate this point, the seminal work of Seyle [21] explained that many stressors placed upon us have neutral effects until, by our own thinking, we appraise the experience as subjectively negative or positive. Thus, when measuring stress in individuals, it is important to acknowledge that a self-report of stress will need to be evaluated against the baseline of an individual (i.e. personality, past experience, as well as physiological state). This explains why different people may have profoundly different reactions to the same basic source of stress. In other words, it is one’s own perception that is crucial. However, both physiological and subjective mood responses to stress are relatively stable individual traits and can thus be accurately and consistently measured after exposure to stressful stimuli. For example, good correlations between subjective and physiological stress increases are shown in participants after the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) [15]. Yet measurement of stress elicited by the TSST can be modulated by genetics, lifestyle, etc., and thus, validity of any subjective measurement will need to be evaluated in the context of these types of variables.
Contextual information is often missed when comparing measures; thus, validation of self-report with performance or physiological data must take into account large individual differences and also must control for as many external variables as possible. For example, measuring physical activity with objective (accelerometry) compared with self-report measures, not surprisingly produces very poor correlations [22] and is interpreted as reflecting recall bias inherent in self-report measures. However, an alternative explanation is that the two methods measure different things. Self-reports take into account the specific context, whereas accelerometry is a raw measure of movement. In summary, validity of subjective measures can be obtained via consistency of results after highly controlled stimuli such as the TSST and to some extent with physiological measures, when carefully controlled for “individual” factors.

Detection and interpretation of meaningful effects

A common problem in this field is how to interpret null effects. Detecting no effect on a measure of subjective state following a nutritional intervention could mean that the intervention really does not influence the mood parameter measured. Alternatively, it could reflect a lack of sensitivity of that measure to small or subtle changes in state or that the intervention prevented a degradation that would otherwise have been observed. An effect may also fail to be detected because of inadequate power which is also related to the magnitude of the experimental effect, or individual differences in the use of a scale which increase variability and decrease sensitivity of the measure [5]. Large inter- and intra-subject variability in subjective measures such as mood mean that larger sample sizes to detect the effects of a nutrient against this relatively noisy background may be required. Null or inconsistent effects may also be attributable to the use of insensitive experimental designs (e.g. between subjects) or inappropriate selection of baseline measures and the failure to include these in the statistical analysis. Two psychological measures of mood may claim to measure the same subjective state, but it may be that only one is sufficiently sensitive to detect effects of a nutritional manipulation. It is feasible that commonly employed measures may be inadequate to assess subjective state in relation to nutrient intervention and mask real effects. A lack of effect on some measures could be due to a subtle effect of a nutrient on only one component of the subjective state which when computed into a “scale score” is no longer detected. Many studies have simply administered “off the shelf” measures and report the factor scores or summed items based on previous validation studies or factor analyses which are not based on the same populations in the same state, e.g. following a nutrition intervention.
Appropriate analyses of data from studies where subjective state is the outcome variable pose some unique problems. Even where participants can be randomised to nutrient or placebo interventions, it cannot be assumed that baseline measures will not differ and hence change from baseline or inclusion of baseline as a covariate is the recommended statistical approach. In these randomised studies, both analysis of variance (ANOVA) on change from baseline and analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with baseline as a covariate will be unbiased [23]. In non-randomised studies where baseline differences may pre-exist and samples may be more heterogeneous, both methods may contradict each other since neither can be unbiased. True randomisation is therefore the preferred approach. Moreover, the overuse and over-interpretation of subgroup analyses poses further problems in this field [24], as does the splitting of samples using median splits [21] since these may exaggerate or even produce effects which are actually artefacts of the reduction of a continuous variable such as mood to a categorical group.

Expectancy and placebo effects

Placebo effects are a concern for all scientific research and well-designed clinical studies go to great lengths to avoid potential bias introduced by these effects by “blinding”. However, via expectations, placebos influence a variety of outcome measures including physiological responses. Despite continuing efforts to fully understand the psychological mechanisms of placebo effects, it is well recognised that expectation is a major contributor to study outcomes in response to treatment in intervention studies [25]. Expectancy effects can be described as changes in behaviour that occur in an experiment due to the anticipation of results that unconsciously affect the outcome. The observation that these effects may be one of the main mechanisms that underlie the placebo effect has been supported by neuroimaging studies which have shown that the placebo response is associated with widespread frontal and prefrontal activation which is consistent with the processing of expectation and executive function [26]. Most of our knowledge about placebo mechanisms comes from investigations of placebo effects in the domain of pain [27] where patients have repeatedly experienced significant pain relief from placebos when the placebo is described as an analgesic medication compared to when it is not [28]. This is an example of a verbal cue that can create a strong expectancy effect which results in behavioural, clinical or physiological changes [29]. Another example of this is where caffeine-associated stimuli such as the smell and taste of coffee have been shown to elicit both self-reported and physiological arousal [30] and where the expectation of having consumed caffeine can result in improvements in cognitive performance and mood [18]. In this particular study, participants were randomly allocated to both a drink (caffeinated coffee vs. decaffeinated coffee) and an expectancy (told caffeine vs. told decaffeinated) condition. Both caffeine and expectation of having consumed caffeine improved attention and psychomotor speed.
There are many other factors which influence expectation including previous experience of the intervention, knowledge of others’ accounts, influences such as the researcher and non-verbal and sensory cues such as the label of the product and the taste. For instance, Rogers et al. [31] showed that when energy drinks were labelled “energy drink” rather than “healthy and nutritious”, both the expected energy from the drink and the expectation of better performance on cognitive tasks were significantly greater. In this study, taste also resulted in expectation of better performance on the task; however, these expectancies did not carry through to influence actual performance, which is contradictory to Green et al.’s study [32]. They did find that expectancy effects were able to influence performance on a cognitive task. Green et al. [32] gave study participants either glucose or aspartame on two occasions in a counterbalanced manner. On one occasion, for each treatment, participants were misinformed about the treatment they were going to receive (glucose or aspartame). Improvements in the Bakan vigilance task were observed after glucose consumption, but only when participants were told they were going to receive glucose. No task improvements were observed when participants thought that they were consuming aspartame. Similar effects were observed by Stollery and Christian [33] who found a subtle improvement in a task of delayed free recall when participants believed they had consumed glucose. Whilst the authors concluded that the expectancy effects were small and unlikely to be confused with glucose enhancements, they did suggest that routine collection of product beliefs should be carried out in future studies. This is particularly important when testing commercial products, on potential consumers. Whilst typically substantial effort is invested to control for placebo effects (by having a placebo), expectancy effects are also important for subjective and objective state post-ingestion. Moreover, it is these effects reinforced by repeated exposure which will guide consumers into making a decision to purchase a product. Hence, expectancy effects also need to be measured in studies of nutrient interventions. These observations also emphasise the importance of an appropriate, well-matched placebo, particularly in experiments where the product could easily be recognised by the consumer, and post-study debriefing may be useful to assess the effectiveness of blinding of the treatment conditions (although this is rarely reported in published studies). It should also be noted that use of objective measures will not cancel or even ameliorate placebo effects. Thus, the expectancy of the study participants is an important factor in the design of nutritional intervention studies.

Relationships between subjective and objective mental performance measures

Whilst mood states are subjective in the sense that they occur in a person’s experience, manifestations of mood can also be observed objectively in physical actions, cognition and behaviour. Mood-influenced cognition is most often studied in patients with depression or those who have recovered from depression. In such studies, depression has been shown to correlate with worse performance in attention and memory tasks. The definition of clinical depression is defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM V) as a “depressed mood or a loss of interest or pleasure in daily activities for more than 2 weeks, where mood represents a change from the person’s baseline”. This rather qualitative definition is made quantitative in clinical intervention studies by establishment of threshold scores in depression checklists or questionnaires (such as the Beck Depression Inventory) which measure particular levels of impaired function in social, occupational and educational settings. Due to the concrete criteria of clinical depression, comparison of depressed vs healthy participant behaviour is a useful approach for understanding mood–cognition interactions. For instance, depressed study participants demonstrate attentional negative bias towards stimuli such as words or facial expressions as compared to non-depressed participants [34] and show a slowing down of response times compared to non-depressed participants. Interestingly, researchers have shown that participants with familial history of depression, but with no prior diagnosis of depression, also exhibit attentional bias towards negative stimuli that is greater than that of participants with no familial history of depression. These “emotional bias” cognition tests have been proposed as an early detection method to identify participants with a predisposition to depression. Similarly, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measurements of amygdala reactivity to negative stimuli appear to be greater in participants with family history of depression or carriers of putative “depression genes” such as the 5-HTTLPR short alleles or met MAO alleles [35].
With respect to healthy participants, the negative effect of poor mood on cognition has been demonstrated with similar paradigms. For example, after a stress induction to provoke negative mood, participants exhibit stronger reactions to negative stimuli reflected by altered reaction times or decision-making [36]. Poor mood has also been shown to affect attentional biases towards punishment-associated stimuli, as manifested by faster reaction time towards such stimuli. Impaired cognitive processing due to mood has also been observed via poorer performance on effortful versus automatic visual tasks in depressed subjects [37]. The above examples describe several areas where mood has an immediate effect on cognition; however, we also need to consider the direct effects of nutrients on mood and cognition and how they may not be measurable at the same time. Consideration needs to be given to the fact that there may be a temporal lag between ingestion of a nutrient and its effect on mood and cognition. For example, improvements in mood may have a motivating effect on subsequent cognitive performance. It is highly likely that effects of nutrients or dietary supplements on mood and cognition occur within different time frames and even at different doses of the nutrient and as such effects may be missed if subjective and objective measures are not considered concomitantly and independently. For example in an acute study by Scholey et al. [19], cognitive performance and self-reported mental fatigue were measured after two doses of cocoa flavanols. Both doses improved performance on the Serial Threes task, whilst only the low dose improved mood. In addition, the high acute dose improved response times in a Rapid Visual Information Processing Task (RVIP) but showed no effects on mood. It is plausible that the effects of a nutrient on subjective state in the longer term may produce acute changes in cognition or that such changes are not related temporally. Hence, it is advisable to consider acute on chronic studies and more frequent assessments to ensure that these effects and the relationships between acute and chronic effects in both subjective and objective outcomes are captured.

Linking subjective states to mechanisms of action

Demonstration of biological plausibility is crucial when linking the effects of specific nutrients to subjective mood states; however, there is a paucity of studies which have attempted to link these two types of outcome within the same study. We will use the example of n − 3 PUFA to describe potential mechanisms. Animals fed a diet containing low levels of n − 3 PUFA display impaired emotional and cognitive behaviour [3847]. Metabolism of monoamines [48], neuronal plasticity [49], neuroinflammation [50, 51] and stress response [52] are impaired in the brains of these animals. In humans, an impaired stress response has been repeatedly associated with low n − 3 PUFA [44, 5357]. Promising data obtained in humans and animal models show that docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) dietary supplementation lowers self-reported cognitive (e.g. feeling unease and fear) and physiological (e.g. sweating) anxiety symptoms [58], cortisol response to mental stress [55, 59] and to inflammatory stimuli [60], and peripheral and brain inflammation, especially in healthy older adults [50, 6165]. The activity of proinflammatory cytokines in the brain are involved in the development of somatic symptoms (fatigue, attention, etc.) known to alter mental performance and mood as described above [6668]. To conclude, preclinical studies have highlighted plausible biological mechanisms relating to neurobiological systems involved in mood and cognitive disorders, possibly underlying the effects of various nutrients on subjective mood state.

Recommendations and conclusions

In this paper, we argue that the subjective effects of foods on self-reported mood and cognition can enrich our understanding and interpretation of gold-standard “objective outcome measures” (cognitive test outcomes, e.g. reaction times, the number of words recalled and physiological markers, e.g. salivary cortisol concentrations) and should be considered both concomitantly and independently. Thus, we make the case that measures of subjective states are a valid and appropriate methodology for assessing the effects of nutritional intake on mood and mental performance. The proposed approach extends the traditional cognitive approach of using standard “objective” performance measurements to also include consumers’ subjective experiences in relation to food. Our specific recommendations are as follows:
  • Consideration of both traditional mood questionnaires or the use of ecological momentary assessment (EMA) or experience sampling methods (ESM), which may more realistically capture transient mood states. Identifying which are best and most reliable under which condition.
  • Use measures sufficiently sensitive to detect effects of a nutritional manipulation and to evaluate the magnitude and meaningfulness of changes demonstrated, e.g. by comparison with effects induced by drugs or alcohol or variations in mood which occur over the course of a day.
  • Consider the possibility that subjective and objective measures will occur within different time frames and in response to different doses. Acute on chronic studies may help capture transient effects on mood or cognitive function which may result in longer-term measureable benefits.
  • Recognition of the importance of expectancy and placebo effects when measuring subjective mood states and behaviour generally and selection of well-matched placebos.
  • Demonstrating biological plausibility is crucial when linking the effects of specific nutrients to subjective mood states and behaviour.

Acknowledgments

This work was conducted by an expert group of the International Life Sciences Institute, Europe (ILSI Europe). The expert group received funding from the ILSI Europe Nutrition and Mental Performance Task Force. Industry members of this task force are listed on the ILSI Europe Web site at www.​ilsi.​eu. For further information about ILSI Europe, please email info@ilsieurope.be or call +32 2 771 00 14. The opinions expressed herein and the conclusions of this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of ILSI Europe nor those of its member companies.

Author’s contribution

M. Hamer, L. Dye, E. Siobhan Mitchell, S. Layé, C. Saunders, N. Boyle and J. Sijben contributed to the conceptualisation and writing of the manuscript. J. Schuermans coordinated the work of the group and assisted with writing, condensing and formatting the draft.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

C. Saunders is a full-time employee of PepsiCo Inc. J. Sijben is a full-time employee of Nutricia Research. E. Siobhan Mitchell is a full-time employee of Nestlé Institute of Health Sciences. S. Layé, E. Siobhan Mitchell and J. Schuermans have no competing interests. M. Hamer has received funding from industry for research on physical activity and well-being. L. Dye and N. Boyle have received funding from various food companies for contract research on the effects of food on cognitive function and mood.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

Neuer Inhalt

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson JW, Johnstone BM, Remley DT (1999) Breast-feeding and cognitive development: a meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 70:525–535 Anderson JW, Johnstone BM, Remley DT (1999) Breast-feeding and cognitive development: a meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 70:525–535
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Singh B, Parsaik AK, Mielke MM, Erwin PJ, Knopman DS, Petersen RC, Roberts RO (2014) Association of Mediterranean diet with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis 39:271 Singh B, Parsaik AK, Mielke MM, Erwin PJ, Knopman DS, Petersen RC, Roberts RO (2014) Association of Mediterranean diet with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis 39:271
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Ordovas JM (2015) Genetics of brain and cognition and their interactions with dietary and environmental factors. In: Best T, Dye L (eds) Nutrition for brain health and cognitive performance, 1st edn. CRC Press, Boca Rotan, pp 41–74CrossRef Ordovas JM (2015) Genetics of brain and cognition and their interactions with dietary and environmental factors. In: Best T, Dye L (eds) Nutrition for brain health and cognitive performance, 1st edn. CRC Press, Boca Rotan, pp 41–74CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Gómez-Pinilla F (2008) Brain foods: the effects of nutrients on brain function. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:568–578CrossRef Gómez-Pinilla F (2008) Brain foods: the effects of nutrients on brain function. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:568–578CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat de Jager CA, Dye L, de Bruin EA, Butler L, Fletcher J, Lamport DJ, Latulippe ME, Spencer JP, Wesnes K (2014) Criteria for validation and selection of cognitive tests for investigating the effects of foods and nutrients. Nutr Rev 72:162–179CrossRef de Jager CA, Dye L, de Bruin EA, Butler L, Fletcher J, Lamport DJ, Latulippe ME, Spencer JP, Wesnes K (2014) Criteria for validation and selection of cognitive tests for investigating the effects of foods and nutrients. Nutr Rev 72:162–179CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Halyburton AK, Brinkworth GD, Wilson CJ, Noakes M, Buckley JD, Keogh JB, Clifton PM (2007) Low-and high-carbohydrate weight-loss diets have similar effects on mood but not cognitive performance. Am J Clin Nutr 86:580–587 Halyburton AK, Brinkworth GD, Wilson CJ, Noakes M, Buckley JD, Keogh JB, Clifton PM (2007) Low-and high-carbohydrate weight-loss diets have similar effects on mood but not cognitive performance. Am J Clin Nutr 86:580–587
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Hellhammer J, Waladkhani AR, Hero T, Buss C (2010) Effects of milk phospholipid on memory and psychological stress response. Br Food J 112:1124–1137CrossRef Hellhammer J, Waladkhani AR, Hero T, Buss C (2010) Effects of milk phospholipid on memory and psychological stress response. Br Food J 112:1124–1137CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Rogers PJ, Appleton KM, Kessler D, Peters TJ, Gunnell D, Hayward RC, Heatherley SV, Christian LM, McNaughton SA, Ness AR (2008) No effect of n − 3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid (EPA and DHA) supplementation on depressed mood and cognitive function: a randomised controlled trial. Br J Nutr 99:421–431CrossRef Rogers PJ, Appleton KM, Kessler D, Peters TJ, Gunnell D, Hayward RC, Heatherley SV, Christian LM, McNaughton SA, Ness AR (2008) No effect of n − 3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid (EPA and DHA) supplementation on depressed mood and cognitive function: a randomised controlled trial. Br J Nutr 99:421–431CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Lupien SJ, Maheu F, Tu M, Fiocco A, Schramek TE (2007) The effects of stress and stress hormones on human cognition: implications for the field of brain and cognition. Brain Cogn 65:209–237CrossRef Lupien SJ, Maheu F, Tu M, Fiocco A, Schramek TE (2007) The effects of stress and stress hormones on human cognition: implications for the field of brain and cognition. Brain Cogn 65:209–237CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C (2009) Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:434–445CrossRef Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C (2009) Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:434–445CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Russell JA (1980) A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 39:1161CrossRef Russell JA (1980) A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 39:1161CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Watson D, Tellegen A (1985) Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychol Bull 98:219CrossRef Watson D, Tellegen A (1985) Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychol Bull 98:219CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown ES, Chandler PA (2001) Mood and cognitive changes during systemic corticosteroid therapy. Prim Care Companion J Clin Psychiatry 3:17CrossRef Brown ES, Chandler PA (2001) Mood and cognitive changes during systemic corticosteroid therapy. Prim Care Companion J Clin Psychiatry 3:17CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Mula M, Sander JW (2007) Negative effects of antiepileptic drugs on mood in patients with epilepsy. Drug Saf 30:555–567CrossRef Mula M, Sander JW (2007) Negative effects of antiepileptic drugs on mood in patients with epilepsy. Drug Saf 30:555–567CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell J, Ehlert U (2012) Acute psychosocial stress: does the emotional stress response correspond with physiological responses? Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:1111–1134CrossRef Campbell J, Ehlert U (2012) Acute psychosocial stress: does the emotional stress response correspond with physiological responses? Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:1111–1134CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Pase MP, Scholey AB, Pipingas A, Kras M, Nolidin K, Gibbs A, Wesnes K, Stough C (2013) Cocoa polyphenols enhance positive mood states but not cognitive performance: a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. J Psychopharmacol 27:451–458CrossRef Pase MP, Scholey AB, Pipingas A, Kras M, Nolidin K, Gibbs A, Wesnes K, Stough C (2013) Cocoa polyphenols enhance positive mood states but not cognitive performance: a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. J Psychopharmacol 27:451–458CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Yoto A, Motoki M, Murao S, Yokogoshi H (2012) Effects of L-theanine or caffeine intake on changes in blood pressure under physical and psychological stresses. J Physiol Anthropol 31:28CrossRef Yoto A, Motoki M, Murao S, Yokogoshi H (2012) Effects of L-theanine or caffeine intake on changes in blood pressure under physical and psychological stresses. J Physiol Anthropol 31:28CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Dawkins L, Shahzad FZ, Ahmed SS, Edmonds CJ (2011) Expectation of having consumed caffeine can improve performance and mood. Appetite 57:597–600CrossRef Dawkins L, Shahzad FZ, Ahmed SS, Edmonds CJ (2011) Expectation of having consumed caffeine can improve performance and mood. Appetite 57:597–600CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Scholey AB, French SJ, Morris PJ, Kennedy DO, Milne AL, Haskell CF (2010) Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in acute improvements in mood and cognitive performance during sustained mental effort. J Psychopharmacol 24:1505–1514CrossRef Scholey AB, French SJ, Morris PJ, Kennedy DO, Milne AL, Haskell CF (2010) Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in acute improvements in mood and cognitive performance during sustained mental effort. J Psychopharmacol 24:1505–1514CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Steptoe A, Wardle J, Marmot M (2005) Positive affect and health-related neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory processes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:6508–6512CrossRef Steptoe A, Wardle J, Marmot M (2005) Positive affect and health-related neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory processes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:6508–6512CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Selye H (1936) Thymus and adrenals in the response of the organism to injuries and intoxications. Br J Exp Pathol 17:234 Selye H (1936) Thymus and adrenals in the response of the organism to injuries and intoxications. Br J Exp Pathol 17:234
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Hamer M, Kivimaki M, Steptoe A (2012) Longitudinal patterns in physical activity and sedentary behaviour from mid-life to early old age: a substudy of the Whitehall II cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health 66:1110–1115CrossRef Hamer M, Kivimaki M, Steptoe A (2012) Longitudinal patterns in physical activity and sedentary behaviour from mid-life to early old age: a substudy of the Whitehall II cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health 66:1110–1115CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Breukelen GJ (2006) ANCOVA versus change from baseline had more power in randomized studies and more bias in nonrandomized studies. J Clin Epidemiol 59:920–925CrossRef Van Breukelen GJ (2006) ANCOVA versus change from baseline had more power in randomized studies and more bias in nonrandomized studies. J Clin Epidemiol 59:920–925CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Pocock SJ, Assmann SE, Enos LE, Kasten LE (2002) Subgroup analysis, covariate adjustment and baseline comparisons in clinical trial reporting: current practice and problems. Stat Med 21:2917–2930CrossRef Pocock SJ, Assmann SE, Enos LE, Kasten LE (2002) Subgroup analysis, covariate adjustment and baseline comparisons in clinical trial reporting: current practice and problems. Stat Med 21:2917–2930CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Finniss DG, Kaptchuk TJ, Miller F, Benedetti F (2010) Biological, clinical, and ethical advances of placebo effects. Lancet 375:686–695CrossRef Finniss DG, Kaptchuk TJ, Miller F, Benedetti F (2010) Biological, clinical, and ethical advances of placebo effects. Lancet 375:686–695CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Benedetti F, Mayberg HS, Wager TD, Stohler CS, Zubieta JK (2005) Neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect. J Neurosci 25:10390–10402CrossRef Benedetti F, Mayberg HS, Wager TD, Stohler CS, Zubieta JK (2005) Neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect. J Neurosci 25:10390–10402CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Colloca L, Benedetti F (2005) Placebos and painkillers: is mind as real as matter? Nat Rev Neurol 6:545–552CrossRef Colloca L, Benedetti F (2005) Placebos and painkillers: is mind as real as matter? Nat Rev Neurol 6:545–552CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Wager TD, Atlas LY, Leotti LA, Rilling JK (2011) Predicting individual differences in placebo analgesia: contributions of brain activity during anticipation and pain experience. J Neurosci 31:439–452CrossRef Wager TD, Atlas LY, Leotti LA, Rilling JK (2011) Predicting individual differences in placebo analgesia: contributions of brain activity during anticipation and pain experience. J Neurosci 31:439–452CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Co Colloca L, Miller FG (2011) How placebo responses are formed: a learning perspective. Philos Trans Roy Soc B 366:1859–1869CrossRef Co Colloca L, Miller FG (2011) How placebo responses are formed: a learning perspective. Philos Trans Roy Soc B 366:1859–1869CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Flaten MA, Blumenthal TD (1999) Caffeine-associated stimuli elicit conditioned responses: an experimental model of the placebo effect. Psychopharmacology 145:105–112CrossRef Flaten MA, Blumenthal TD (1999) Caffeine-associated stimuli elicit conditioned responses: an experimental model of the placebo effect. Psychopharmacology 145:105–112CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Rogers PJ, Law J, Sanderson C (2012) Modelling the psychostimulant effects of an energy drink. Label and taste influence expectations, but only caffeine delivers. Appetite 59:634 Rogers PJ, Law J, Sanderson C (2012) Modelling the psychostimulant effects of an energy drink. Label and taste influence expectations, but only caffeine delivers. Appetite 59:634
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Green Green MW, Taylor MA, Elliman NA, Rhodes O (2001) Placebo expectancy effects in the relationship between glucose and cognition. Br J Nutr 86:173–179CrossRef Green Green MW, Taylor MA, Elliman NA, Rhodes O (2001) Placebo expectancy effects in the relationship between glucose and cognition. Br J Nutr 86:173–179CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Stollery B, Christian L (2013) Glucose and memory: the influence of drink, expectancy, and beliefs. Psychopharmacology 228:685–697CrossRef Stollery B, Christian L (2013) Glucose and memory: the influence of drink, expectancy, and beliefs. Psychopharmacology 228:685–697CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Murrough JW, Iacoviello B, Neumeister A, Charney DS, Iosifescu DV (2011) Cognitive dysfunction in depression: neurocircuitry and new therapeutic strategies. Neurobiol Learn Mem 96:553–563CrossRef Murrough JW, Iacoviello B, Neumeister A, Charney DS, Iosifescu DV (2011) Cognitive dysfunction in depression: neurocircuitry and new therapeutic strategies. Neurobiol Learn Mem 96:553–563CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Roiser JP, Sahakian BJ (2013) Hot and cold cognition in depression. CNS Spectr 18:139–149CrossRef Roiser JP, Sahakian BJ (2013) Hot and cold cognition in depression. CNS Spectr 18:139–149CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Hockey GR, John Maule A, Clough PJ, Bdzola L (2000) Effects of negative mood states on risk in everyday decision making. Cognition Emotion 14:823–855CrossRef Hockey GR, John Maule A, Clough PJ, Bdzola L (2000) Effects of negative mood states on risk in everyday decision making. Cognition Emotion 14:823–855CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Hammar A, Lund A, Hugdahl K (2003) Long-lasting cognitive impairment in unipolar major depression: a 6-month follow-up study. Psychiat Res 118:189–196CrossRef Hammar A, Lund A, Hugdahl K (2003) Long-lasting cognitive impairment in unipolar major depression: a 6-month follow-up study. Psychiat Res 118:189–196CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat DeMar JC, Ma K, Bell JM, Igarashi M, Greenstein D, Rapoport SI (2006) One generation of n − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid deprivation increases depression and aggression test scores in rats. J Lipid Res 47:172–180CrossRef DeMar JC, Ma K, Bell JM, Igarashi M, Greenstein D, Rapoport SI (2006) One generation of n − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid deprivation increases depression and aggression test scores in rats. J Lipid Res 47:172–180CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Fedorova I, Hussein N, Baumann MH, Di Martino C, Salem N Jr (2009) An n − 3 fatty acid deficiency impairs rat spatial learning in the Barnes maze. Behav Neurosci 123:196CrossRef Fedorova I, Hussein N, Baumann MH, Di Martino C, Salem N Jr (2009) An n − 3 fatty acid deficiency impairs rat spatial learning in the Barnes maze. Behav Neurosci 123:196CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Francès H, Coudereau JP, Sandouk P, Clément M, Monier C, Bourre JM (1996) Influence of a dietary α-linolenic acid deficiency on learning in the Morris water maze and on the effects of morphine. Eur J Pharmacol 298:217–225CrossRef Francès H, Coudereau JP, Sandouk P, Clément M, Monier C, Bourre JM (1996) Influence of a dietary α-linolenic acid deficiency on learning in the Morris water maze and on the effects of morphine. Eur J Pharmacol 298:217–225CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Lafourcade M, Larrieu T, Mato S, Duffaud A, Sepers M, Matias I, De Smedt-Peyrusse V, Labrousse VF, Bretillon L, Matute C (2011) Nutritional omega-3 deficiency abolishes endocannabinoid-mediated neuronal functions. Nat Neurosci 14:345–350CrossRef Lafourcade M, Larrieu T, Mato S, Duffaud A, Sepers M, Matias I, De Smedt-Peyrusse V, Labrousse VF, Bretillon L, Matute C (2011) Nutritional omega-3 deficiency abolishes endocannabinoid-mediated neuronal functions. Nat Neurosci 14:345–350CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Larrieu T, Madore C, Joffre C, Layé S (2012) Nutritional n − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids deficiency alters cannabinoid receptor signaling pathway in the brain and associated anxiety-like behavior in mice. J Physiol Biochem 68:671–681CrossRef Larrieu T, Madore C, Joffre C, Layé S (2012) Nutritional n − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids deficiency alters cannabinoid receptor signaling pathway in the brain and associated anxiety-like behavior in mice. J Physiol Biochem 68:671–681CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Levant B, Radel JD, Carlson SE (2006) Reduced brain DHA content after a single reproductive cycle in female rats fed a diet deficient in N − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids. Biol Psychiat 60:987–990CrossRef Levant B, Radel JD, Carlson SE (2006) Reduced brain DHA content after a single reproductive cycle in female rats fed a diet deficient in N − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids. Biol Psychiat 60:987–990CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat McNamara RK, Jandacek R, Rider T, Tso P, Cole-Strauss A, Lipton JW (2010) Omega-3 fatty acid deficiency increases constitutive pro-inflammatory cytokine production in rats: relationship with central serotonin turnover. Prostag Leukotr Ess 83:185–191CrossRef McNamara RK, Jandacek R, Rider T, Tso P, Cole-Strauss A, Lipton JW (2010) Omega-3 fatty acid deficiency increases constitutive pro-inflammatory cytokine production in rats: relationship with central serotonin turnover. Prostag Leukotr Ess 83:185–191CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Moranis A, Delpech JC, De Smedt-Peyrusse V, Aubert A, Guesnet P, Lavialle M, Joffre C, Layé S (2012) Long term adequate n − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid diet protects from depressive-like behavior but not from working memory disruption and brain cytokine expression in aged mice. Brain Behav Immun 26:721–731CrossRef Moranis A, Delpech JC, De Smedt-Peyrusse V, Aubert A, Guesnet P, Lavialle M, Joffre C, Layé S (2012) Long term adequate n − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid diet protects from depressive-like behavior but not from working memory disruption and brain cytokine expression in aged mice. Brain Behav Immun 26:721–731CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Rao JS, Ertley RN, Lee HJ, DeMar JC, Arnold JT, Rapoport SI, Bazinet RP (2007) n − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid deprivation in rats decreases frontal cortex BDNF via a p38 MAPK-dependent mechanism. Mol Psychiatry 12:36–46CrossRef Rao JS, Ertley RN, Lee HJ, DeMar JC, Arnold JT, Rapoport SI, Bazinet RP (2007) n − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid deprivation in rats decreases frontal cortex BDNF via a p38 MAPK-dependent mechanism. Mol Psychiatry 12:36–46CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Vancassel S, Aïd S, Pifferi F, Morice E, Nosten-Bertrand M, Chalon S, Lavialle M (2005) Cerebral asymmetry and behavioral lateralization in rats chronically lacking n − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids. Biol Psychiatry 58:805–811CrossRef Vancassel S, Aïd S, Pifferi F, Morice E, Nosten-Bertrand M, Chalon S, Lavialle M (2005) Cerebral asymmetry and behavioral lateralization in rats chronically lacking n − 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids. Biol Psychiatry 58:805–811CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Chalon S (2006) Omega-3 fatty acids and monoamine neurotransmission. Prostag Leukotr Ess 75:259–269CrossRef Chalon S (2006) Omega-3 fatty acids and monoamine neurotransmission. Prostag Leukotr Ess 75:259–269CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Zainuddin MSA, Thuret S (2012) Nutrition, adult hippocampal neurogenesis and mental health. Br Med Bull 103:89–114CrossRef Zainuddin MSA, Thuret S (2012) Nutrition, adult hippocampal neurogenesis and mental health. Br Med Bull 103:89–114CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Layé S (2010) Polyunsaturated fatty acids, neuroinflammation and well being. Prostag Leukotr Ess 82:295–303CrossRef Layé S (2010) Polyunsaturated fatty acids, neuroinflammation and well being. Prostag Leukotr Ess 82:295–303CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Mingam R, Moranis A, Bluthé R-M, De Smedt-Peyrusse V, Kelley KW, Guesnet P, Lavialle M, Dantzer R, Layé S (2008) Uncoupling of interleukin-6 from its signalling pathway by dietary n − 3-polyunsaturated fatty acid deprivation alters sickness behaviour in mice. Eur J Neurosci 28:1877–1886CrossRef Mingam R, Moranis A, Bluthé R-M, De Smedt-Peyrusse V, Kelley KW, Guesnet P, Lavialle M, Dantzer R, Layé S (2008) Uncoupling of interleukin-6 from its signalling pathway by dietary n − 3-polyunsaturated fatty acid deprivation alters sickness behaviour in mice. Eur J Neurosci 28:1877–1886CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Denis I, Potier B, Vancassel S, Heberden C, Lavialle M (2013) Omega-3 fatty acids and brain resistance to ageing and stress: body of evidence and possible mechanisms. Ageing Res Rev 12:579–594CrossRef Denis I, Potier B, Vancassel S, Heberden C, Lavialle M (2013) Omega-3 fatty acids and brain resistance to ageing and stress: body of evidence and possible mechanisms. Ageing Res Rev 12:579–594CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Lin PY, Huang SY, Su KP (2010) A meta-analytic review of polyunsaturated fatty acid compositions in patients with depression. Biol Psychiat 68:140–147CrossRef Lin PY, Huang SY, Su KP (2010) A meta-analytic review of polyunsaturated fatty acid compositions in patients with depression. Biol Psychiat 68:140–147CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Mocking RJ, Assies J, Bot M, Jansen EH, Schene AH, Pouwer F (2012) Biological effects of add-on eicosapentaenoic acid supplementation in diabetes mellitus and co-morbid depression: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS one 7:e49431CrossRef Mocking RJ, Assies J, Bot M, Jansen EH, Schene AH, Pouwer F (2012) Biological effects of add-on eicosapentaenoic acid supplementation in diabetes mellitus and co-morbid depression: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS one 7:e49431CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Assies J, Pouwer F, Lok A, Mocking RJ, Bockting CL, Visser I, Abeling NG, Duran M, Schene AH (2010) Plasma and erythrocyte fatty acid patterns in patients with recurrent depression: a matched case-control study. PLoS one 5:e10635CrossRef Assies J, Pouwer F, Lok A, Mocking RJ, Bockting CL, Visser I, Abeling NG, Duran M, Schene AH (2010) Plasma and erythrocyte fatty acid patterns in patients with recurrent depression: a matched case-control study. PLoS one 5:e10635CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Laugero KD, Smilowitz JT, German JB, Jarcho MR, Mendoza SP, Bales KL (2011) Plasma omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid status and monounsaturated fatty acids are altered by chronic social stress and predict endocrine responses to acute stress in titi monkeys. Prostag Leukotr Ess 84:71–78CrossRef Laugero KD, Smilowitz JT, German JB, Jarcho MR, Mendoza SP, Bales KL (2011) Plasma omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid status and monounsaturated fatty acids are altered by chronic social stress and predict endocrine responses to acute stress in titi monkeys. Prostag Leukotr Ess 84:71–78CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Mocking RJ, Ruhé HG, Assies J, Lok A, Koeter MW, Visser I, Bockting CL, Schene AH (2013) Relationship between the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal-axis and fatty acid metabolism in recurrent depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38:1607–1617CrossRef Mocking RJ, Ruhé HG, Assies J, Lok A, Koeter MW, Visser I, Bockting CL, Schene AH (2013) Relationship between the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal-axis and fatty acid metabolism in recurrent depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38:1607–1617CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Belury MA, Andridge R, Malarkey WB, Glaser R (2011) Omega-3 supplementation lowers inflammation and anxiety in medical students: a randomized controlled trial. Brain Behav Immun 25:1725–1734CrossRef Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Belury MA, Andridge R, Malarkey WB, Glaser R (2011) Omega-3 supplementation lowers inflammation and anxiety in medical students: a randomized controlled trial. Brain Behav Immun 25:1725–1734CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Delarue JOCP, Matzinger O, Binnert C, Schneiter P, Chiolero R, Tappy L (2003) Fish oil prevents the adrenal activation elicited by mental stress in healthy men. Diabetes Metab 29:289–295CrossRef Delarue JOCP, Matzinger O, Binnert C, Schneiter P, Chiolero R, Tappy L (2003) Fish oil prevents the adrenal activation elicited by mental stress in healthy men. Diabetes Metab 29:289–295CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Orr SK, Palumbo S, Bosetti F, Mount HT, Kang JX, Greenwood CE, Ma DW, Serhan CN, Bazinet RP (2013) Unesterified docosahexaenoic acid is protective in neuroinflammation. J Neurochem 127:378–393CrossRef Orr SK, Palumbo S, Bosetti F, Mount HT, Kang JX, Greenwood CE, Ma DW, Serhan CN, Bazinet RP (2013) Unesterified docosahexaenoic acid is protective in neuroinflammation. J Neurochem 127:378–393CrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Farzaneh-Far R, Harris WS, Garg S, Na B, Whooley MA (2009) Inverse association of erythrocyte n − 3 fatty acid levels with inflammatory biomarkers in patients with stable coronary artery disease: the Heart and Soul Study. Atherosclerosis 205:538–543CrossRef Farzaneh-Far R, Harris WS, Garg S, Na B, Whooley MA (2009) Inverse association of erythrocyte n − 3 fatty acid levels with inflammatory biomarkers in patients with stable coronary artery disease: the Heart and Soul Study. Atherosclerosis 205:538–543CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Ferrucci L, Cherubini A, Bandinelli S, Bartali B, Corsi A, Lauretani F, Martin A, Andres-Lacueva C, Senin U, Guralnik JM (2006) Relationship of plasma polyunsaturated fatty acids to circulating inflammatory markers. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 91:439–446CrossRef Ferrucci L, Cherubini A, Bandinelli S, Bartali B, Corsi A, Lauretani F, Martin A, Andres-Lacueva C, Senin U, Guralnik JM (2006) Relationship of plasma polyunsaturated fatty acids to circulating inflammatory markers. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 91:439–446CrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Kalogeropoulos N, Panagiotakos DB, Pitsavos C, Chrysohoou C, Rousinou G, Toutouza M, Stefanadis C (2010) Unsaturated fatty acids are inversely associated and n − 6/n − 3 ratios are positively related to inflammation and coagulation markers in plasma of apparently healthy adults. Clin Chim Acta 411:584–591CrossRef Kalogeropoulos N, Panagiotakos DB, Pitsavos C, Chrysohoou C, Rousinou G, Toutouza M, Stefanadis C (2010) Unsaturated fatty acids are inversely associated and n − 6/n − 3 ratios are positively related to inflammation and coagulation markers in plasma of apparently healthy adults. Clin Chim Acta 411:584–591CrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Belury MA, Porter K, Beversdorf D, Lemeshow S, Glaser R (2007) Depressive symptoms, n − 6: n − 3 fatty acids, and inflammation in older adults. Psychosom Med 69:217–224CrossRef Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Belury MA, Porter K, Beversdorf D, Lemeshow S, Glaser R (2007) Depressive symptoms, n − 6: n − 3 fatty acids, and inflammation in older adults. Psychosom Med 69:217–224CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Labrousse VF, Nadjar A, Joffre C, Costes L, Aubert A, Gregoire S, Bretillon L, Laye S (2012) Short-term long chain omega3 diet protects from neuroinflammatory processes and memory impairment in aged mice. PLoS one 7:e36861CrossRef Labrousse VF, Nadjar A, Joffre C, Costes L, Aubert A, Gregoire S, Bretillon L, Laye S (2012) Short-term long chain omega3 diet protects from neuroinflammatory processes and memory impairment in aged mice. PLoS one 7:e36861CrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Capuron L, Miller AH (2011) Immune system to brain signaling: neuropsychopharmacological implications. Pharmacol Ther 130:226–238CrossRef Capuron L, Miller AH (2011) Immune system to brain signaling: neuropsychopharmacological implications. Pharmacol Ther 130:226–238CrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Dantzer R, O’Connor JC, Freund GG, Johnson RW, Kelley KW (2008) From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nat Rev Neurol 9:46–56CrossRef Dantzer R, O’Connor JC, Freund GG, Johnson RW, Kelley KW (2008) From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nat Rev Neurol 9:46–56CrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Dantzer R, Heijnen CJ, Kavelaars A, Laye S, Capuron L (2014) The neuroimmune basis of fatigue. Trends Neurosci 37:39–46CrossRef Dantzer R, Heijnen CJ, Kavelaars A, Laye S, Capuron L (2014) The neuroimmune basis of fatigue. Trends Neurosci 37:39–46CrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Bond A, Lader M (1974) The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Br J Med Psychol 47:211–218CrossRef Bond A, Lader M (1974) The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Br J Med Psychol 47:211–218CrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Armitage CJ, Conner M, Norman P (1999) Differential effects of mood on information processing: evidence from the theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour. Eur J Soc Psychol 29:419–433CrossRef Armitage CJ, Conner M, Norman P (1999) Differential effects of mood on information processing: evidence from the theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour. Eur J Soc Psychol 29:419–433CrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Watson D, Clark LA (1999) The PANAS-X: manual for the positive and negative affect schedule-expanded form. University of Iowa Press, Iowa City Watson D, Clark LA (1999) The PANAS-X: manual for the positive and negative affect schedule-expanded form. University of Iowa Press, Iowa City
72.
Zurück zum Zitat McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF (1971) EITS manual for the profile of mood states. Educational and Industrial Testing Service, San Diego McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF (1971) EITS manual for the profile of mood states. Educational and Industrial Testing Service, San Diego
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Norcross JC, Guadagnoli E, Prochaska JO (1984) Factor structure of the profile of mood states (POMS): two partial replications. J Clin Psychol 40:1270–1277CrossRef Norcross JC, Guadagnoli E, Prochaska JO (1984) Factor structure of the profile of mood states (POMS): two partial replications. J Clin Psychol 40:1270–1277CrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Nyenhuis DL, Yamamoto C, Luchetta T, Terrien A, Parmentier A (1999) Adult and geriatric normative data and validation of the profile of mood states. J Clin Psychol 55:79–86CrossRef Nyenhuis DL, Yamamoto C, Luchetta T, Terrien A, Parmentier A (1999) Adult and geriatric normative data and validation of the profile of mood states. J Clin Psychol 55:79–86CrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat WHOQOL group (1995) The World Health Organization quality of life assessment (WHOQOL): position paper from the World Health Organization. Soc Sci Med 41:1403–1409CrossRef WHOQOL group (1995) The World Health Organization quality of life assessment (WHOQOL): position paper from the World Health Organization. Soc Sci Med 41:1403–1409CrossRef
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Skevington SM, Lotfy M, O’Connell KA (2004) The World Health Organization’s WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment: psychometric properties and results of the international field trial. A report from the WHOQOL group. Qual Life Res 13:299–310CrossRef Skevington SM, Lotfy M, O’Connell KA (2004) The World Health Organization’s WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment: psychometric properties and results of the international field trial. A report from the WHOQOL group. Qual Life Res 13:299–310CrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Diener ED, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S (1985) The satisfaction with life scale. J Pers Assess 49:71–75CrossRef Diener ED, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S (1985) The satisfaction with life scale. J Pers Assess 49:71–75CrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Lyubomirsky S, Lepper HS (1999) A measure of subjective happiness: preliminary reliability and construct validation. Soc Indic Res 46:137–155CrossRef Lyubomirsky S, Lepper HS (1999) A measure of subjective happiness: preliminary reliability and construct validation. Soc Indic Res 46:137–155CrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldberg DP (1972) The detection of psychiatric illness by questionnaire: a technique for the identification and assessment of non-psychotic psychiatric illness. Oxford University Press, London Goldberg DP (1972) The detection of psychiatric illness by questionnaire: a technique for the identification and assessment of non-psychotic psychiatric illness. Oxford University Press, London
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Carvalho HWd, Patrick CJ, Jorge MR, SrB Andreoli (2011) Validation of the structural coherency of the General Health Questionnaire. Rev Bras Psiquiatria 33:59–63CrossRef Carvalho HWd, Patrick CJ, Jorge MR, SrB Andreoli (2011) Validation of the structural coherency of the General Health Questionnaire. Rev Bras Psiquiatria 33:59–63CrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Dale B, Sævareid HI, Söderhamn O (2009) Testing and using Goldberg’s General Health Questionnaire: mental health in relation to home nursing, home help, and family care among older, care-dependent individuals. Int J Ment Health Nurs 18:133–143CrossRef Dale B, Sævareid HI, Söderhamn O (2009) Testing and using Goldberg’s General Health Questionnaire: mental health in relation to home nursing, home help, and family care among older, care-dependent individuals. Int J Ment Health Nurs 18:133–143CrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Montazeri A, Harirchi AM, Shariati M, Garmaroudi G, Ebadi M, Fateh A (2003) The 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12): translation and validation study of the Iranian version. Health Qual Life Outcomes 1:66CrossRef Montazeri A, Harirchi AM, Shariati M, Garmaroudi G, Ebadi M, Fateh A (2003) The 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12): translation and validation study of the Iranian version. Health Qual Life Outcomes 1:66CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Examining techniques for measuring the effects of nutrients on mental performance and mood state
verfasst von
Mark Hamer
Louise Dye
E. Siobhan Mitchell
Sophie Layé
Caroline Saunders
Neil Boyle
Jeroen Schuermans
John Sijben
Publikationsdatum
07.01.2016
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Journal of Nutrition / Ausgabe 6/2016
Print ISSN: 1436-6207
Elektronische ISSN: 1436-6215
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00394-015-1143-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 6/2016

European Journal of Nutrition 6/2016 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Innere Medizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Innere Medizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.