Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Review of Aging and Physical Activity 1/2018

Open Access 01.12.2018 | Review Article

Exercise or physical activity and cognitive function in adults with type 2 diabetes, insulin resistance or impaired glucose tolerance: a systematic review

verfasst von: Ren Ru Zhao, Anthony J. O’Sullivan, Maria A. Fiatarone Singh

Erschienen in: European Review of Aging and Physical Activity | Ausgabe 1/2018

Abstract

Background

Diabetes is an important risk factor for cognitive impairment. Although some studies suggest that physical exercise can minimize age-related cognitive declines or improve brain morphology or function, benefits in diabetes or impaired glucose tolerance are unclear. Therefore, our aim was to evaluate the efficacy of exercise or physical activity on cognition in adults with type 2 diabetes, insulin resistance or impaired glucose tolerance.

Methods

An electronic search for studies published from the earliest record until February 2017 was conducted using Medline, EMBASE, SPORTDiscus, CINAHL, and PsycINFO. Any experimental or observational study designs were included, as long as they were conducted in individuals of any age with type 2 diabetes, insulin resistance or impaired glucose tolerance, and they directly examined exercise/physical activity effects on cognitive outcomes or the relationship between changes in cognition and changes in either insulin resistance and glucose homeostasis. Study quality was assessed using the PEDro scale; data on participant and intervention characteristics and outcomes were extracted.

Results

Six studies enrolling 2289 participants met the eligibility criteria. Quality was modest and effect sizes variable and mostly small or negligible. Overall, four of the six studies (67%) reported significant benefits of greater exercise/physical activity participation for some aspects of cognition, but only 26% of cognitive outcomes were significant across all trials. Clinical improvements in insulin resistance/glucose homeostasis were related to improvements in cognitive function in three studies. Overall results were inconsistent, with benefits varying across exercise types and cognitive domains.

Conclusions

Literature does not provide evidence that physical activity or exercise interventions contribute to a better cognitive function in patients with type 2 diabetes or impaired glucose tolerance. Large-scale, long-term, robust randomized controlled trials are required to determine if exercise improves cognition in this high-risk cohort, and to investigate putative mechanistic links between cognition, body composition, metabolism, and inflammation in diabetes and related metabolic syndromes.
Abkürzungen
AD
Alzheimer’s disease
AOS
Anthony J O’Sullivan
BDNF
Brain-derived neurotropic factor
BMI
Body mass index
CI
Confidence intervals
ES
Effect size
HbA1c
Glycosylated hemoglobin
HOMA
Homeostatic Model of Assessment
IGF-1
Insulin-like growth factor-1
IGT
Impaired glucose tolerance
IR
Insulin resistance
JOBA
Intensity horseback riding therapeutic equipment
METs
Metabolic equivalents
MFS
Maria Fiatarone Singh
MMSE
Mini-Mental State Exam
NRCTs
Non-randomized controlled trials
OGTT
Oral glucose tolerance test
PEDro
Physiotherapy Evidence Database scale
PRISMA
Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews
PRT
Progressive resistance training
RCTs
Randomized controlled trials
RRZ
Ren Ru Zhao
SDs
Standard deviations
SLUMS
Saint Louis University Mental Status
T2D
Type 2 diabetes
TMTB
Trail Making Test B
VO2peak
Peak oxygen uptake
VS
Versus

Background

Diabetes is projected to affect 435 million adults by 2030 [1], and as the prevalence rises with age from 12% in people aged 65 to 70 to 15% in those over age 80, older adults will continue to be disproportionately affected [2]. Type 2 diabetes (T2D) accounts for 85% to 90% of patients with diabetes, is a progressive disease manifested by a decline in insulin sensitivity with insulin deficiency that results in sustained hyperglycemia [3], and is the leading cause of cardiovascular disease, kidney disease, vision loss, and neuropathy [4]. Patients with T2D may present with cognitive deficits, associated with reduced performance in multiple domains of cognitive function [58]. Higher levels of glycosylated hemoglobin (HbA1c) have been negatively associated with cognitive performance in middle-aged and older individuals [5, 9]. Additionally, in older patients with T2D, cognitive deficits in working memory and attention have been observed in the hyperglycemic state during a glucose clamp [10].
Diet and exercise represent the initial treatment approaches to slow progression of metabolic disturbances associated with pre-diabetes and to assist with pharmacological treatment in established T2D. Increased physical activity has clear beneficial physiological effects for older adults with T2D or glucose intolerance or insulin resistance [11, 12], and more recently has been shown to benefit cognition as well [1315]. Epidemiological evidence consistently links physical exercise with better cognitive performance [13], lower risk for dementia, and reduced pathological changes in the central nervous system [1416]. Positive effects of aerobic exercise on cognition have been well documented in animal models and in aging clinical populations [1719]. There is evidence from cross-sectional and prospective brain imaging studies to suggest that aerobic exercise may reduce brain atrophy in older adults, changes that are most striking for brain regions that support executive control processes and memory [17, 20]. Experimental studies have more recently reported benefits of resistance training on cognitive function in older adults [2123]. Furthermore, regular physical exercise also has potential therapeutic effects on glucose regulation and cardiovascular health, both of which may threaten cognitive integrity when compromised [22, 24, 25]. Although it has been shown that exercise can enhance cognitive function, most studies have been in healthy older adults, and thus the applicability of these findings to older adults at high risk for cognitive decline is less well defined. Indeed, there are recent reviews of the cognitive benefits of physical activity [26, 27], but there is no systematic review of the effects of exercise on cognitive function in people with T2D, insulin resistance (IR) or impaired glucose tolerance (IGT) to our knowledge. Given the increased risk for dementia posed by T2D, it is important to define the utility of exercise or physical activity for this outcome in this cohort specifically.
Therefore, our objective was to systematically review the literature to identify the relative efficacy of various modes of structured exercise or habitual physical activity level in individuals of any age with T2D or IR or IGT on any measure of cognition, including attention, visual-spatial performance, memory, information processing speed, executive control processes, or global cognitive function. Our secondary aim was to identify the potential mechanisms underlying any cognitive benefits by examining relationships between changes in metabolism, body composition, markers of cerebral pathology including amyloid deposition, and cognitive changes after exposure to chronic exercise or physical activity in this cohort.

Methods

Systematic review protocol

A systematic review consistent with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) was conducted [28]. An electronic search was originally performed in June 2014 and updated in February 2017 using database Medline (1946 - Second week February 2017), CINAHL (1982- Second week February 2017), EMBASE (1974-February 07, 2017), SPORTDiscus (1967- Second week February 2017), and PsycINFO (1967- January week 5 2017). The exercise intervention search terms were: [exercise] [training] [physical] [aerobic] [physical capacity] [aerobic capacity] [physical performance] [physical endurance] [motor activity] [resistance] [weight lifting] [strength] [power training] [strength training] [weight-training] [resistance exercise], combined with “or”. Population terms, combined with “or”, were [diabetes] [insulin] [glucose], Cognitive outcomes terms, combined with “or”, were: [Cognition] [Cognitive] [Memory] [Brain] [Mental] [Neurological] [Neuropsychological]. Intervention, Population, and Cognitive outcome terms were combined with “AND” and then searched in “All Fields” with the limits human and English language. Reference lists of retrieved papers and review articles were hand-searched for any additional papers.

Eligibility criteria

Studies were selected from the initial search if they met all of the following criteria:
1)
Population: Persons diagnosed with T2D or IR or IGT with a valid measure of IR or glucose homeostasis, including fasting glucose or insulin, HbA1c, Homeostatic Model of Assessment-insulin resistance (HOMA-IR) or Homeostatic Model of Assessment 2-insulin resistance (HOMA2-IR), glucose clamp, intravenous glucose tolerance test, oral glucose tolerance test variables or meal tolerance test.
 
2)
Intervention: Physical exercise training consistent with the definition of the American College of Sports Medicine: planned, structured, and repetitive physical activity which has as a final or intermediate objective, the improvement or maintenance of physical fitness [29]. Studies of less than twelve weeks’ duration did not meet criteria for chronic training and were excluded. No limitations were imposed based on modality, dose, or intensity. In experimental studies, the exercise intervention did not need to be fully supervised but must have been prescribed and quantifiable, including self-administered questionnaires or activity monitors [30, 31]. If the study was observational, then it had to report the level of physical activity of the cohort by either objective or subjective measures. The design of the study must have been such that the independent effects of exercise or physical activity could be analyzed. In the case of multiple interventions (e.g., diet + exercise), one group must have been treated with diet/other intervention, another with diet/other intervention + exercise, and both groups must have included individuals with T2D, IGT or IR.
 
3)
Outcomes: Any validated neuropsychological test of cognition reported at baseline and follow-up after exposure to exercise or physical activity. The physiological/metabolic profile was included in the secondary aim of examining relationships between changes in physiological/metabolic profile and changes in cognition, studies must have directly analyzed the effects of changes in IR or other metabolic outcomes on the changes in cognitive function outcomes. Such analyses must have included metabolic, body composition or fitness changes as independent variables using simple or multiple regression models unless the independent effects of changes in these parameters could be determined from the statistical models presented.
 
4)
Control group: For experimental studies, any kind of control group was eligible, including no contact, no treatment, waiting list, attention control, sham exercise, or alternative active treatment. For observational studies, cohorts had to be stratified by level of reported or observed physical activity, with the least active stratum considered the control/low exposure group, and may have included participants who did not have metabolic disorders in the overall study, as long as the subset with the metabolic disease was analyzed separately.
 
5)
Study design: randomized controlled trial (RCT), non-randomized controlled trial (NRCT), uncontrolled trial (UCT), observational study.
 
6)
Full-length article published in a peer-reviewed English language journal.
 

Data extraction and quality assessment

One author (RRZ) conducted the search and extracted all data. After eliminating duplicates, all papers identified by the search strategy were screened by the author, first by title and then by the abstract. Two authors (RRZ and AOS) determined final eligibility by reading the full text of potentially relevant studies. Quality assessment of eligible trials was independently rated on the quality rating Physiotherapy Evidence Database scale (PEDro) [32]. An additional item (“Exercise supervised: yes/no”) was included to identify this important component of exercise, providing a final possible score of 12. Any discrepancies were resolved by consensus, or by a third author (MFS) when necessary. Baseline and follow-up data were extracted, and cohort characteristics, intervention (training type, delivery, volume, intensity and duration), and outcome measures were reported along with relationships and statistical significance levels. Authors were contacted for missing data whenever possible.

Data analysis

A quantitative meta-analysis of studies was not carried out due to the heterogeneity of exercise interventions, outcomes assessed and measurement tools used and the paucity of studies identified. The systematic review compared exercise with no exercise or strata of physical activity at the end of the intervention/observation period. Measurements are presented as mean +/− standard deviation (SD) and significance was set at p < 0.05 unless otherwise indicated, as original study papers reported. Relative effect size (ES) (mean change Treatment – mean change Control) ÷ Pooled baseline SD was calculated for controlled trials where possible [33]. Effect sizes were interpreted according to the method of Cohen as: ‘trivial’ (≤ 0.20), ‘small’ (≥ 0.20 to <0.50), ‘moderate’ (≥ 0.50 to <0.80) and ‘large’ (≥ 0.80) [34].

Results

Studies retrieved

Figure 1 displays the detailed results of the search process at each step. The combined search identified 7238 potentially eligible studies while hand searching identified a further two studies; this was reduced to 19 after reviewing titles and abstracts, which were reviewed in full to determine suitability. Six studies [3540] met criteria for this review. This included three RCTs [35, 38, 40], one NRCT [39], one prospective cohort study (which also included a cross-sectional analysis) [37] and one cross-sectional study [36].

Study quality assessment

An evaluation of the study quality checklist items based on a modified PEDro is summarized in Table 1. Overall, the quality of the three RCTs [35, 38, 40] included in this review was moderate, receiving scores of 7 or 9 out of 12. Common limitations in quality were: lack of blinded assessors, lack of therapists blinded to hypotheses, the absence of intention-to-treat analysis, and insufficient available information regarding baseline matching between groups in all trials [35, 38, 40]. As expected, the NRCT [39], prospective [37], and cross-sectional [36, 37] studies were of a lower standard on the PEDro scale [36, 37, 39], with an average scoring 4/12 (range 4 to 6) [36, 37, 39].
Table 1
Study quality of included trials according to a modified PEDro scale
Criterion
Watson
et al.
2006 [35]
Colberg
et al.
2008 [36]
Devore
et al.
2009 [37]
Baker
et al.
2010 [38]
Yanagawa
et al.
2011 [39]
Lehtisalo
et al.
2016 [40]
Study Design
RCT
CS
CS
LS
RCT
NRCT
RCT
Eligibility Criteria
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Random allocation
Y
N/A
N/A
N/A
Y
N
Y
Concealed allocation
N
N/A
N/A
N/A
N
N
N
Group baseline similarity
Y
N
N
N
Y
Y
Y
Blinding of subjects
N
N/A
N/A
N/A
N
N
N
Blinding of therapists
N
N/A
N/A
N/A
Y
N
N
Blinding of assessors
N
N/A
N/A
N/A
N
N
N
Measures of one key outcome in at least 85% of participants
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
At least one key outcomes analyzed with ‘intention to treat’
N
N
N
N
Y
N
N
Point measures, and measures of variability for at least one key outcome
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Between group statistical comparisons reported for at least one key outcome
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Full supervision of exercise intervention
Y
N
N
N
Y
Y
Y
Final score (/ 12)
7
4
4
4
9
6
7
Points are only awarded when a criterion is clearly satisfied within a modified PEDro Scale. PEDro Scale Physiotherapy Evidence Database Scale, CS cross-sectional study, PS prospective study, RCT randomized controlled trial, NRCT non-randomized controlled trial, N/A, not applicable, Y = 1 N or N/A = 0

Study design and participant characteristics

A summary of participant and study characteristics can be found in Table 2. Total of 2289 participants were enrolled across 6 studies, ranging from 16 to 1550 participants, with median 71. Pooled sample size amongst RCTs was 578 (398 in exercise intervention arms, ranging from 15 to 364) [35, 38, 40], while 16 participants were enrolled in 1 NRCT [39]. The average age of cohorts across all studies was 64 years, ranging from 56 to 74 years. Sex breakdown was provided in 5/6 study cohorts. Overall, one study included women only [37], while another one did not report sex [35]. The remaining 4 studies [36, 37, 39, 40] were comprised of mixed cohorts.
Table 2
Participant characteristics of exercise on cognition in individuals with T2D or IGT
Citation
Inclusion criteria
Condition (N)
Age, years (SD)
% Female
Medications
Watson
et al.
2006 [35]
Glucose tolerance criteria for prediabetes 7.8 mmol/L ≥ 2-h glucose <11.1 mmol/L)
Normal cognitive assessment
Exercise group: exercise
plus AHAS2ED (15)
BMI: 24.7 ± 3.2 kg/m2
Control group:
AHAS1ED (13)
BMI:26.7 ± 4.3 kg/m2
58.0 ± 9.7
60.6 ± 9.0
Not reported
Not reported
Colberg
et al.
2008 [36]
American Diabetes Association criteria for the diagnosis of type 2 diabetes. Cutoff point for MMSE score > 24 and SLUMS <20 for high school educated, <15 for less educated
Diabetes group (74)
HbA1c, % 6.6 ± 0.2
Control group (71)
HbA1c, % 5.2 ± 0.0
55.5 ± 1.0
57.5 ± 1.3
66.2
Not reported
Devore
et al.
2009 [37]
American Diabetes Association criteria for diagnosis of type 2 diabetes
Diabetes duration: 1–15 year
Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (modified forms of the Mini-Mental State Examination
High physical activity
group (512)
BMI (43%) ≥ 30 kg/m2
Moderate physical
activity group (520)
BMI (33%) ≥ 30 kg/m2
Low physical activity
group (518)
BMI (32%) ≥ 30 kg/m2
No control group
74.0 ± 2.3
74.0 ± 2.4
74.0 ± 2.3
100
Insulin therapy
Oral hypoglycemia
Anti-hypertension/ anti-hypoglycemia
Baker
et al.
2010 [38]
Glucose tolerance criteria for prediabetes (7.8 mmol/L ≥ 2-h glucose <11.1 mmol/L) and for diagnosed type 2 diabetes (2-h glucose ≥1.1 mmol/L)
Normal cognitive assessment
Exercise group: aerobic exercise (19), 2-h OGTT glucose levels:184 ± 46 mg/dL
BMI:30.6 ± 3.9 kg/m2
Control group: stretching (9)
2-h OGTT glucose levels: 163 ± 31.8 mg/dL
BMI: 30.1 ± 7.3 kg/m2
71.0 ± 7.5
66.0 ± 6.0
64.3
Anti-hypertension
Yanagawa et al.
2011 [39]
World Health Organization criteria for diagnosis of type 2 diabetes euglycemic clamp)
Normal cognitive assessment
Exercise group (9)
HbA1c, % 7.29 ± 0.56
BMI: 23.31 ± 2.80 kg/m2
Control group: walking (7)
HbA1c, % 7.14 ± 0.61
BMI: 22.31 ± 2.03 kg/m2
71.6 ± 3.8
70.1 ± 3.7
31.2
Not reported
Lehtisalo et al.
2016 [40]
Glucose tolerance criteria for prediabetes 7.8 mmol/L ≥ 2-h glucose <11.1 mmol/L
Normal cognitive assessment
Exercise group:
Mixing exercise (364)
Control group:
Health advice (158)
55.1 ± 6.8
55.3 ± 7.8
67.1
Not reported
AHAS1ED American Heart Association Step 1 Eucaloric Diet, AHAS2ED American Heart Association Step 2; Eucaloric Diet, MMSE Mini-mental State Examination, SLUMS Saint Louis University Mental Status Examination, OGTT oral glucose tolerance test, HbA1c glycosylated hemoglobin, BMI Body mass index, SD Standard deviation

Measures of glucose homeostasis and insulin resistance

One study utilized fasting glucose ≥7 mmol/L, or insulin resistance 100 to 125 mg/dL for diagnosis of type 2 diabetes [36]. The hyperinsulinemic euglycemic clamp (the gold standard measurement for IR) was only used in two studies [38, 39], while only two studies used an oral glucose tolerance test to assess glucose tolerance and insulin sensitivity [35, 40]. One study failed to report any measure of insulin or glucose metabolism [37].

Measures of body composition

Body composition was reported in all but one cross-sectional study [36]. One study used computerized tomography to measure intra-abdominal fat [35] and two studies assessed percent body fat [38, 39]. The remaining two studies reported that participants were overweight or obese, with body mass index (BMI) mean of ≥25 kg/m [37, 40].

Neuropsychological assessment

Cognitive function was assessed by neuro-psychological examination [35, 3840]. One study measured cognitive status with the Mini-mental State Exam (MMSE) [36], with a cutoff point for MMSE scores > 24 and Saint Louis University Mental Status (SLUMS) scores <20 for high school educated, <15 for less education inclusive. One study used modified forms of the MMSE [37], and although MMSE scores were inclusionary criteria for the study, the actual scores (global cognitive scores) were not provided [37].

Intervention characteristics

Tables 3 provides an overview of the exercise interventions among the 4 experimental studies. For the cross-sectional and prospective studies, physical activity exposure was assessed by self-report questionnaires (Table 4).
Table 3
Intervention and control characteristics of exercise on cognition in individuals with T2D or IGT
Citation
Intervention modality
Intensity
Volume
(minutes)
Frequency
(days/week)
Duration
(weeks)
Control condition
Watson
et al.
2006 [35]
Aerobic exercise plus Diet control
Supervised
50% of HRR over
12 weeks increasing to 70% of HRR from 13 to 26 weeks
60
3
52
Stretching
plus diet
Baker
et al.
2010 [38]
Aerobic exercise,
treadmill
Supervised
Workload gradually
increased to 75–85% of HRR over 1–6 weeks
45–60
4
26
Stretching or balance
Yanagawa
et al.
2011 [39]
Aerobic exercise
(JOBA)
Supervised
55–69% heart rate reserve, or 40–59% of VO2peak
45
4
12
Usual level of activity
Lehtisalo
et al.
2016 [40]
Brisk walking, skiing,
jogging, swimming,
bicycling, gymnastics,
resistance training, and
ball games
Supervised
Moderate-vigorous
30
7
156
No any exercise
health advice
JOBA horseback riding simulation equipment, VO 2peak peak oxygen uptake, HRR heart rate reserve, T2D type 2 diabetes, IGT impaired glucose tolerance
Table 4
Intervention and control characteristics of physical activity on cognition in individuals with T2D or IGT
Citation
Physical activity assessment method
Physical activity category a
Low active
Moderate active
High active
Colberg et al.
2008 [36]
Modified (diabetes)
a version of the HAPAQ
(One-year recall)
Regular exercise, % 55.4 b
Days of exercise/week 2.2 ± 0.3
City blocks walked/day1.2 ± 1.5
Stairs climbed/day flights
0.5 ± 0.6
Usual walking pace 2.0 ± 0.1
Usual exertion 0.9 ± 0.2
Weekday, hours/day
Vigorous activity 0.8 ± 0.2
Moderate activity 3.0 ± 0.3
Light activity 5.5 ± 0.4
Sit activity 7.1 ± 0.5
Sleep/recline activity 7.6 ± 0.2
Weekend, hours/day
Vigorous activity 1.0 ± 0.2
Moderate activity 3.2 ± 0.3
Light activity 4.5 ± 0.3
Sit activity 7.1 ± 0.4
Sleep/recline activity 8.2 ± 0.2
 
Regular exercise, % 71.8 b
Days of exercise/week 2.2 ± 0.3
City blocks walked/day 0.3 ± 2.4
Stairs climbed/day flights
0.6 ± 0.8
Usual walking pace 0.5 ± 0.1
Usual exertion 4.3 ± 0.2
Weekday, hours/day
Vigorous activity 0.8 ± 0.1
Moderate activity 3.2 ± 0.4
Light activity 6.9 ± 0.3
Sit activity 5.4 ± 0.4
Sleep/recline activity 7.6 ± 0.2
Weekend, hours/day
Vigorous activity 0.2 ± 0.2
Moderate activity 4.2 ± 0.3
Light activity 5.4 ± 0.3
Sit activity 5.3 ± 0.4
Sleep/recline activity 7.9 ± 0.2
Devore et al.
2009 [37]
SAPAQ-CS
SAPAQ-PS
3.38 (0.13–6.76)
10.70 (6.77–15.50)
24.39 (15.54–112.23)
aPhysical activity category is classified according to the HAPAQ and SAPAQ where possible (Devore reported exercise intensity as metabolic equivalents (METS) [37], while the study of Colberg et al. (2008) defined the intensity of aerobic activities according to exertion levels (e.g. usual exertion, usual walking pace, stair climbing) [36]
bRegular exercise was defined (using the HAPAQ) as participants engaging in at least 30 min of moderate aerobic exercise 3 times a week for a minimum of 1 year
SAPAQ Self-administered Physical Activity Questionnaire, HAPAQ Harvard Alumni Physical Activity Questionnaire [31], CS cross-sectional study, PS prospective study, T2D type 2 diabetes, IGT impaired glucose tolerance

Training modality

One trial tested the effect of isolated moderate-intensity aerobic exercise [38], one moderate- and high-intensity mixed training [40], and one prescribed low-intensity horseback riding therapeutic equipment (JOBA) compared to a maintaining usual level of activity (walking) control group [39]. Only one study investigated the effects of a combination of aerobic exercise and dietary restriction compared to a diet restriction plus stretching control group [35]. The single prospective cohort study compared the effects of highest habitual physical activity level to lowest physical activity level [37], while the cross-sectional study, derived from the same cohort, examined the difference between the highest and the lowest physical activity groups [37]. Finally, another cross-sectional compared the cognitive function of regular exercise in a sedentary diabetes group (controls) to exercise in a group with T2D [36].

Exercise dose and intensity

Exercise volume in experimental studies varied from 30 to 60 min per session, three to seven sessions per week, and duration of interventions ranged from 12 to 156 weeks. Two trials of aerobic exercise used exercise equipment at moderate [35, 38], or low [39] intensities (JOBA), whereas moderate- and high-intensity walking was prescribed in one mixed resistance and other exercise modalities [40]. In the prospective cohort and cross-sectional studies, for classification of habitual physical activity, the period of observation was 1 year [36] and 4.2 years of follow-up [37], respectively. Devore et al. (2009) reported exercise intensity as metabolic equivalents (METs) [37], while Colberg et al. (2008) [36] defined the intensity of aerobic activities according to exertion levels (e.g., usual exertion, usual walking pace, stair climbing).

Control conditions

Control conditions were highly variable and included diet [35], stretching or balance [38], usual activities [39], or health advice [40]. Three of the experimental studies reported supervision during the control condition [35, 38, 40]. The one prospective and two cross-sectional studies described the control condition as a low level of habitual aerobic exercise/physical activity, which was compared to the higher activity level stratum, all unsupervised [36, 37].

Outcome measurement

A total of 20 different cognitive outcome measures were administered (average 4/study, range: 2–7). Data from non-significant findings were not always provided [35, 38]. The general cognitive function was measured using MMSE, SLUMS, Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS), or overall global scores [36, 37, 39, 40]. The majority of studies administered standardized neuropsychological tests with attention/executive function the most frequently measured domain [35, 38, 39], followed by memory [35, 3739] information processing [39, 40], and global cognitive function [36, 37, 39]. Only four trials had a longitudinal follow-up after intervention at 12 weeks [39], 26 weeks [38], 52 weeks [35], or 36 months [40]. Supervision was removed during the maintenance phase in one study and participants were advised to continue exercise at home for the final 6 months [35]. The one prospective cohort study had follow-up over 4.2 years [37].
Four of the 6 studies (67%) reported significant benefits for at least one outcome [3538]. However, only 26% of cognitive outcomes were significant across all trials. Exercise was reported to result in improved global cognition [36, 37], executive function [38], and memory [39] in these 4 studies, which included 2 interventional and 2 observational studies.

Executive function/attention

Aerobic exercise vs. non-exercise controls

A summary of the results can be found in Table 5. Only one [38] out of four [35, 3840] studies reported a significantly greater improvement in three of five executive function tests in aerobic training compared to controls, with small effect size (ES) [(Trail Making Test B ES = 0.36, p = 0.04), (Task Switching ES = 0.39, p = 0.03), and Stroop Color-Word Interference ES = 0.38, p = 0.04)], and trends for improvement in Verbal Fluency (ES = 0.26, p = 0.11) and Self-Ordered Pointing Test (ES = 0.29, p = 0.10) over 6 months. [38] However, analysis of executive function showed no difference in a trial of low intensity JOBA exercise vs. controls [(Trail Making Test A ES: 0.46; 95% CI: -0.54, 1.46), (Trail Making Test B ES: 0.60; 95% CI: -0.41, 0.41), (Stroop ES: 0.14; 95% CI: -0.84, 1.13), (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised-Digit Symbol test ES: 0.38; 95% CI: -0.62, 1.38)] [39]. Similarly, there was no significant effect of exercise on executive function in two other aerobic exercise trials [35, 40].
Table 5
Effect of exercise interventions on cognition
Citation
Outcome measurement
Results
Calculation
Statistics
Domain
Method
Group
Pre-exercise
(Mean ± SD)
Post-exercise
(Mean ± SD)
Between group
Effect size
(95%, CIs)
Between
group
p-value
Watson
et al.
2006 [35]
Memory
Attention/Executive function
SR
BVR
TMT
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
6.30 ± 1.15*
6.25 ± 1.10*
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
3.65 ± 1.15*
5.05 ± 1.15*
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
1.35 (0.48, 2.22)
Unable to be calculated
Unable to be calculated
0.01
Not significant
Not significant
Baker
et al.
2010 [38]
Information processing and executive function
Memory
VF
TMTB
TS
SCWI
SOPT
SR
LR
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Unable to be calculated
Unable to be calculated
Unable to be calculated
Unable to be calculated
Unable to be calculated
Unable to be calculated
Unable to be calculated
0.11
0.04
0.03
0.04
0.10
Not significant
Not significant
Yanagawa et al.
2011 [39]
Memory
Information processing and executive function
Global cognitive function
WLIM
WLDM
WAIS-DST
TMTA
Stroop
TMTB
MMSE
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
0.59 (−0.42, 0.60)
0.08 (−0.90, 1.07)
0.38 (−0.62, 1.38)
0.46 (−0.54, 1.46)
0.14 (−0.84, 1.13)
0.60 (−0.41, 0.41)
0.02 (−0.97, 1.00)
0.31
1.00
0.79
0.53
0.27
0.27
0.96
Lehtisalo et al.
2016 [40]
Overall cognitive performance
Executive function
CERAD
TMTB
AE
CO
AE
CO
8.3 ± 9
Not reported
48.6 ± 9.6
Not reported
Unable to be calculated
Unable to be calculated
Unable to be calculated
Unable to be calculated
Not significant
Not significant
Data with an asterisk were estimated from graphic values provided using the Matlab software. Between-group and/or intra-group effect sizes (ESs) were calculated for both groups. Between group effect size = (Change in Treatment – Change in Control) / Pooled baseline SD [33]. Intra-group effect size = (post-exercise - pre-exercise) / pooled baseline SD [34]. Data are presented as mean ± SD and effect size and 95% confidence intervals (CIs), adjusted for age, BMI and years of education
SD standard deviation, ES effect size, CIs confidence intervals, AE aerobic exercise, CO control, SR Story Recall (Story Recall (immediate recall and delayed recall), BVR Benton Visual Retention, TMT Trail Making Test, SIT Stroop Interface Test, VF Verbal Fluency, TMTA Trail Making Test A, TMTB Trail Making Test B, TS Task Switching, SCWI Stroop Color-Word Interference, SOPT Self-Ordered Pointing Test, LR Learning Recall, WLIM Word List Immediate Memory a subtest of the Alzheimer’s disease Assessment Scale, WLDM Word List Delayed Memory a subtest of the Alzheimer’s disease Assessment Scale, DST Digit Symbol Test a subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination (maximal score = 0–30), CERAD Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease
The Finnish CERAD Battery is composed of 1) Verbal fluency (animals) 2) Modified Boston Naming test (15 words) 3) Mini-Mental State Exam 4) Word List Memory (ten words, three trials) 5) Constructional praxis 6) Word List Recall (delayed recall of the ten words) 7) Word List Recognition (recognition of the ten words out of 20 words) 8) Constructional praxis recall 9) Clock drawing

Memory

Aerobic exercise vs. non-exercise or aerobic exercise and diet restriction vs. diet restriction or high physical activity vs. low physical activity

A summary of the results can be found in Table 5. Four studies [35, 3739] examined the effects on memory and nonsignificant results were reported for the majority of outcomes. Aerobic training combined with diet control compared to a diet control group produced significant large effects on delayed memory (Story Recall Test ES: -1.26; 95% CI: -1.83, −0.70), but nonsignificant results on overall memory domain [35]. Similarly, nonsignificant results were found in studies of low intensity JOBA exercise [39] and moderate intensity treadmill exercise [38]. The cross-sectional analysis failed to find positive effects of high physical activity compared to low physical activity levels [37], and in longitudinal analyses from this same cohort, there was no relationship between physical activity level and memory in this prospective cohort study at the 4.2-year follow-up [37]. Thus, aerobic training and diet compared to diet only control has been shown to improve one memory test in one trial, and no data are available on direct comparisons of the efficacy of different exercise modalities for these outcomes.

Information processing speed

Aerobic exercise vs. non-exercise controls

Results are reported in Table 5. Few data on information processing speed were available, as this was reported after aerobic training in 2 only trials [39, 40]. No significant improvements were shown compared to non-exercise controls.

Global cognitive function

Aerobic exercise vs. non-exercise or higher physical activity vs. low physical activity

As reported in Table 6, aerobic exercise or high physical activity did not significantly improve any global cognitive outcome in a prospective study [37], one RCT [40], and one NRCT [39]. Two cross-sectional analyses reported significantly better global cognition in those with higher physical activity levels however [36, 37]. Similarly, sedentary time showed an inverse relationship with a global cognitive function on the SLUMS test which was used in the study of Colberg et al. [36].
Table 6
Effect of exercise /physical activity on cognition
Citation
Assessment time point
Outcome measurements
Results
Statistics
Domain
Method
Group
Physical activity levels
Between group mean difference (95%, CIs)
p-value
Colberg
et al.
2008
[36]
Cross section study
Global cognitive function
MMSE
SLUMS
AE
CO
AE
CO
AE
CO
weekday light activity: 5.5 ± 0.4
weekday light activity: 6.9 ± 0.3
weekday moderate activity: 3.0 ± 0.3
weekday moderate activity: 3.2 ± 0.4
weekday light activity: 5.5 ± 0.4
weekday light activity: 6.9 ± 0.3
Not reported
Not reported
Not reported
0.04
<0.01
0.04
Devore
et al.
2009 [37]
Cross section study
General cognitive function
Memory
TICS score
Global cognitive
Score
Immediate and delayed recalls
LPA
MPA
HPA
LPA
MPA
HPA
LPA
MPA
HPA
3.38 (0.13–6.76)
10.70 (6.77–15.50)
24.39(15.54–112.23)
3.38 (0.13–6.76)
10.70 (6.77–15.50)
24.39(15.54–112.23)
3.38 (0.13–6.76)
10.70 (6.77–15.50)
24.39(15.54–112.23)
0.37 (0.02, 0.72)
0.07 (0.01, 0.15)
0.06 (0.03, 0.1)
0.03
(HPA versus LPA)
0.06
(HPA
versus LPA)
0.20
(HPA
versus LPA)
Devore
et al.
2009 [37]
Prospective study
General cognitive function
Memory
TICS score
Global cognitive
Score
Immediate and delayed recalls
LPA
MPA
HPA
LPA
MPA
HPA
LPA
MPA
HPA
3.38 (0.13–6.76)
10.70 (6.77–15.50)
24.39(15.54–112.23)
3.38 (0.13–6.76)
10.70 (6.77–15.50)
24.39(15.54–112.23)
3.38 (0.13–6.76)
10.70 (6.77–15.50)
24.39(15.54–112.23)
0.21 (0.15, 0.57)
0.05 (0.03, 0.12)
0.07 (0.03, 0.16)
0.20
(HPA
versus LPA)
0.20
(HPA
versus LPA)
0.20
(HPA
versus LPA)
Data are presented as mean ± SD and mean difference and 95% confidence intervals (CIs), adjusted for age, education, disability indicators, and others
SLUMS Saint Louis University Mental Status exam, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, TICS Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status, LPA low physical activity, MPA moderate physical activity, HPA high physical activity, AE regular physical exercise in individuals with diabetes, CO regular physical exercise in normal individuals

Relationship between changes in measures of cognitive function and insulin resistance

Results are shown in Table 7. Improvements in delayed memory were correlated with improvements in glucose infusion rates (r = 0.64; p = 0.024) during a euglycemic clamp, with a trend for metabolic clearance rate (r = 0.575; p = 0.051) in those who performed JOBA exercise, as well as in the usual physical activity control group [39]. In another intervention study, improvements in delayed memory were related to greater reductions in 2-h OGTT insulin levels (r = −0.52; p < 0.05) in the aerobic exercise plus diet group only, while no relationship was seen in the diet and stretching control group [35]. Additionally, global cognitive function in participants reporting higher levels of physical activity was inversely related to HOMA-IR (r = −0.19, p = 0.02) and insulin level (r = −0.13, p = 0.03) in a cross-sectional study [36]. Thus, there is some evidence to suggest that improvements in insulin sensitivity are either potentially contributory to cognitive adaptations or at least markers for other beneficial mechanistic adaptations and are modified proportionally by exercise.
Table 7
Relationship between changes in cognition and insulin resistance
Citation
Cognition
Insulin resistance measurement
Time since last exercise bout
Relationship
r
p-value
Comments
Watson
et al.
2006 [35]
Delayed memory
Two-hour Oral glucose tolerance test insulin level (mmol/L)
26 weeks
Change in delayed
memory and change in two-hour Oral glucose tolerance test insulin level (mmol/L)
−0.52
0.047
Exercise group (Aerobic exercise plus American Heart Association Step 2 Eucaloric Diet only
Colberg
et al. 2008 [36]
Global cognitive function
Insulin (IU/mL)
Not
reported
Not
reported
Relationship between
HOMA-IR and
cognitive function
Relationship between insulin level and cognitive function
−0.19
−0.18
0.02
0.03
Diabetes exercise group
Yanagawa et al. 2011 [39]
Delayed memory
Euglycemic
Clamp (glucose
infusion rate (mL/kg/min)
Metabolic clearance
rate(mL/kg/min)
12 weeks
Change in word recall and change in glucose infusion rate
Change in word recall and change in metabolic clearance rate
0.64
0.575
0.024
0.051
Horseback riding simulation equipment (JOBA) exercise group

Relationship between changes in measures of cognitive function and glucose homeostasis

A summary of the results can be found in Table 8. Improvements in word recall were related to reductions in HbA1c (r = −0.627; p = 0.029) after JOBA intervention in one NRCT but were not observed in the control group [39]. Higher fasting blood glucose was directly related to worse executive function (r = 0.611; p = 0.035) in this same study [39]. However, no such relationships between cognition and glucose changes were reported in those who performed aerobic training or aerobic training plus diet control only, or in the control groups of two other studies [35, 38]. Thus, there is mixed support for improvements in glucose homeostasis as a mechanism or marker for cognitive benefits of exercise in cohorts with metabolic disease.
Table 8
Relationship between changes in cognition and glucose homeostasis
Citation
Cognition
Glucose level measurement
Time since last exercise bout
Relationship
r
P value
Comments
Yanagawa et al. 2011 [39]
Delayed memory
Executive function
HbA1c (%)
(glycosylated hemoglobin)
Fasting glucose level (mg/dl)
12 weeks
12 weeks
Change in word recall and change in HbA1c
Change in word recall and change in metabolic clearance rate
−0.627
0.611
0.029
0.035
Horseback riding simulation equipment (JOBA)
exercise group

The relationship between changes in measures of cognitive function and body composition

A summary of the results can be found in Table 9. Reductions in intra-abdominal fat (IAF) were correlated with improvements in delayed memory within control participants who received diet and stretching, while no significant relationship was observed in those who received aerobic training and dietary intervention [35]. No other studies reported body composition/cognition relationships, precluding generalized conclusions.
Table 9
Relationship between changes in cognition and body composition
Citation
Cognition
Body composition measurement
Time since last exercise bout
Relationship
r
P value
Comments
Watson
et al.
2006 [35]
Delayed memory
Intra-abdominal
fat (cm2)
26 weeks
Change in delayed
memory and change in intra-abdominal fat (IAF)
Not
reported
−0.62
Not
reported
0.024
Exercise group (exercise plus American Heart Association Step 2 Eucaloric Diet)
Control group (stretching plus American Heart Association Step 1 Eucaloric Diet)

Discussion

This systematic review identified only 3 RCTs, one NRCT, one cross-sectional and one prospective cohort study of exercise or physical activity exposure in individuals with T2D or IGT or IR investigating the relationship between exercise/physical activity and cognition. The quality of this very small literature was only moderate, and the majority of studies had insufficient power due to low sample size to detect small effects. Few cognitive outcomes (26%) were significant, thus providing no strong or consistent evidence that aerobic exercise or lifestyle intervention or higher levels of habitual physical activity improve cognition or are associated with less risk of cognitive impairment/decline in individuals with T2D or IGT or IR.
Abnormal glucose tolerance, a characteristic of prediabetes and T2D, has been related to an increased risk of cognitive impairment and dementia compared with healthy peers [4143]. Regular exercise has potential therapeutic effects on cognitive function compromised by T2D and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) pathology [4145]. Long term T2D often contributes to numerous harmful consequences for peripheral systems and neurophysiologic and structural changes in the brain that adversely affect cognition [44] and ultimately increase the risk of dementia [46]. Cerebral neuropathological changes are believed to begin in the early stages of diabetes, consistent with deficits in cognitive performance for adults with prediabetes as well [47, 48]. Exercise and lifestyle interventions may improve metabolic health as well as potentially attenuate neuropathology in adults at high risk for developing type 2 diabetes, thus ultimately both preventing type 2 diabetes and preserving cognitive function. However, there are currently no studies comparing the use of exercise for cognitive benefits at various stages of the disease process, and this is an important topic for future study. Theoretically, it would seem intuitive that halting the neuropathological changes in the brain at an early stage might be most beneficial, however, literature reviews in dementia [14] actually show that the effect size of exercise on cognition is greater in those with cognitive impairment and/or dementia than it is in older adults with normal cognition. Given that the adverse effects of hyperinsulinemia on neuronal structures and function can be addressed even after a neurotoxic insult has occurred in animal models [49], there would appear to be the rationale for the application of exercise along the entire spectrum of disease from impaired glucose tolerance to established, longstanding T2D.
Prior studies in cohorts without diabetes have mainly focused on aerobic exercise and linked it to improved executive function [19, 50, 51]. In our review, moderate-to-high intensity aerobic exercise was related to improvements in executive function in only 1 [38] of 4 studies [35, 3840], and additional evidence is needed to confirm this finding. Exercise may protect executive function by reducing brain atrophy [52], However, the failure to observe consistent beneficial effects of exercise on executive function as shown the other 3 trials is notable. The type of exercise prescribed (low intensity and short duration JOBA training) may have attenuated the response in one study [39]. It is likely that small sample size may have resulted in a type II error in the group with T2D or IGT [35, 39]. The other study [40] which reported no significant improvement in executive function after mixed aerobic training had a high dropout rate of 30% which may have precluded benefit. Additionally, this study included participants of younger ages with a relatively high level of cognitive function even at the end of the follow-up study, and thus even longer-term follow-up may have been needed in this early prediabetes stage due to ceiling effects on cognitive testing utilized. Thus, as there is only one positive study [38] investigating executive function and exercise, further investigation is required to confirm these findings. By contrast, no beneficial improvements were seen for information processing speed domain after aerobic exercise in older adults with T2D [39] or IGT [40]. These findings stand in contrast to large beneficial effects reported in two previous studies of aerobic exercise healthy cohorts [53]. Sample sizes in this review may have been too small to detect a significant improvement in information process speed, and thus replication in larger samples is indicated.
Only one study compared memory changes between exercise and diet control vs. diet control, and reported a significant, large benefit of the exercise on one memory outcome [35]. Although additional studies are needed, this finding is consistent with evidence in older adults without metabolic disease which suggests a moderate effect in favor of aerobic exercise combined with diet management [54, 55]. This may be explained by the fact that participants in the diet plus exercise group had greater improvement in metabolic parameters directly relevant to memory than did the diet only group. However, in the observational study of high physical activity vs. low physical activity in older individuals with T2D, this benefit for memory was not seen [37]. It is possible that long-standing diabetes, medication treatment, and/or disability may have attenuated the response in this cohort. Similarly, the current review did not find any consistent benefit of aerobic training compared to controls for memory in the other experimental studies of older adults with T2D [39] or IGT [38]. Memory tasks are supported by structural plasticity in the hippocampus [56] which has been observed after aerobic voluntary wheel running in rodents. In humans, the absence of exercise-induced memory benefits in most trials we reviewed may relate to the type of tests administered or the specific task demands that rely on patterns of structural plasticity in specific brain regions. Modality, dose, intensity, and duration of testing is another possible explanation. However, no consistent pattern has been demonstrated and further study is needed.
Only two cross-sectional studies and no experimental studies reported that high physical activity vs. low physical activity showed global cognitive function improvements, and thus it is not possible to draw conclusions about the efficacy of physical activity for global cognition in this cohort. Evidence in older people without T2D suggests a small effect in favor of high physical activity for global cognition [57]. Regular physical exercise participation may offset many of the increased risk factors for cognitive dysfunction frequently associated with diabetes, such as visceral obesity and hypertension [45]. Aerobic fitness itself is also linked to cognitive decline, and an increase of only 1.0 MET has been linked to a lower rate of global cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment and AD in older individuals, for example [58]. Moderate-to-vigorous intensity aerobic exercise is the most potent exercise modality for improving aerobic fitness, thus providing another rationale for its inclusion in exercise programs and guidelines for older adults with T2D. However, the analysis of the cross-sectional/observational studies has some limitations: the level of physical activity was self-reported in two studies, (with physical activity category classified according to the Harvard Alumni Physical Activity Questionnaire and Self-administered Physical Activity Questionnaire, whereas Devore [37] reported exercise intensity as metabolic equivalents, and Colberg [36] defined the intensity of aerobic activities according to exertion levels. Thus, actual physical activity/exercise intensity may not have been reliably captured. Although Devore et al. [37] demonstrated that the validity of such questionnaires is adequate, the results should be interpreted with caution. Among three studies which included patients with T2D [37, 39] or IGT [40], Devore [37] showed that subjects of T2D engaging in a higher level of physical activity per week had better global and verbal cognitive outcomes compared to the lower level of physical activity after adjusting age and education level. However, the initial results disappeared after further adjustments for all covariates. Similarly, among the two experimental studies (one RCT and one NRCT) included in this review, the Lehtisalo [40] did not find any improvement in global cognition in the T2DM or IGT patients after training. This cohort had a relatively young group of participants, with a relatively high level of cognitive function even at the end of their follow-up study. Another possibility is that there were also higher dropout rates after the long follow-up. Furthermore, in the NRCT [39], no positive effects of exercise on global cognition were reported in older adults with T2D. The relatively small sample size and low exercise training intensity and short duration may have contributed to the lack of benefit observed.
None of the trials focused on progressive resistance training (PRT). However, much more research is needed about such anabolic exercise due to its clinical relevance, including prevention of incident dementia and T2D in this high-risk cohort. First, reductions in adiposity and increases in muscle mass, which are targeted by PRT, have been specifically associated with improved insulin sensitivity, glucose control, and inflammation [5962], which are in turn associated with a decreased risk of cognitive decline. Secondly, many older adults with co-morbidities may not tolerate moderate-to-high intensity aerobic exercise but can tolerate high-intensity resistance training. Resistance training may also improve hypertension, dyslipidemia, insulin and glucose regulation, which are important comorbidities of T2D associated with cognitive impairment [25, 63]. Third, PRT results in a range of positive benefits for neurobiological outcomes in animal and human studies, such as increased insulin-like growth factor 1 (IGF-1) [18], increased brain-derived neurotrophic factor [6466], neurogenesis [66], functional plasticity [21], decreased inflammatory cytokines [67, 68], decreased cortisol response to stressors, and improved cognitive function [64, 69]. Specifically, PRT can increase IGF-1 levels, which may lead to improved neurogenesis and vessel remodeling in the brain [70]. Currently, the American College of Sports Medicine recommends that exercise programs for older adults include both aerobic and nonaerobic physical activities, such as resistance training, balance training, and stretching for optimal general health [29]. The inconsistent efficacy of isolated aerobic training for cognitive outcomes in this review suggests that for optimal cognitive outcomes in those with metabolic disease, such a combined prescription of exercise modalities may also be required, and requires specific investigation.
The small amount of evidence available suggests that better cognitive outcomes on executive function [39], memory [35], and global cognitive function [36] were partially explained by, or associated with, reductions in IR. In this context, it is notable that reduced insulin elevations were associated with better delayed memory in IGT patients after aerobic training and diet control in one study which was included this review, compared to diet alone. Although suggestive, we cannot conclude that the exercise alone would have had this same association. In another study in this review, improvements in HOMA2-IR were related to improvements in memory and executive function, even if no significant change in either measure was observed post-intervention. The intervention delivered during the negative trial was JOBA exercise [39], and it may be that the intervention was not of a sufficient intensity to induce significant improvements in delayed memory, information processing speed, executive function, or global cognitive function, nor provide robust metabolic benefit. However, what the results do suggest is that improvements in insulin resistance were related to improvements in cognitive function. In another study, the improvements of global cognitive function were explained in part by decreases in peripheral insulin resistance (HOMA-IR) in individuals who participated in more vigorous activities [36]. It is known that hyperinsulinemia is neurotoxic [71], and thus it is possible that insulin sensitivity and insulin signaling pathway improvements after exercise would favor neurogenesis, and thereby improved cognitive function. However, additional animal and human studies are required to determine the nature of the mechanistic links suggested by the relationships reported in this review.
The reductions in HbA1c and the reduction in fasting blood sugar were related to cognitive benefits in one NRCT study. [39] With the limited data available, it is difficult to conclusively determine the specific role of changes in glucose homeostasis on cognition. However, this study does suggest that improvements in glucose homeostasis are potentially related to improvements in memory and executive function of older adults with T2D.

Limitations of this review and future research

This review was limited by the number of RCTs available in this cohort. Cognitive impairment also introduces the problematic issue that potentially different etiological processes and subtypes may have been mixed within diabetes, insulin resistance or hyperglycemia cohorts included. Several methodological issues were noted in this emerging field. Negligible or nonsignificant effects were found in a high proportion of cognitive outcomes. For all outcomes, inadequate reporting of mean and SD and group mean difference precluded calculation of ES in many studies. Given our overall finding was for no significant effect, the unavoidable issue of negative publication bias should not have impinged upon the outcome of this review. Furthermore, the variety of different exercises modalities (intensity, volume, etc.) might be a limitation. Previous reviews have suggested that aerobic exercise and resistance training should form part of any lifestyle intervention aimed at improving the cognitive function and metabolic profile in healthy adults [16, 72]. However, less of the focus has been on cognitive adaptations for cognitively normal older adults with type 2 diabetes, insulin resistance, or glucose intolerance who are at increased risk of cognitive deficit. This is the first review to focus on neurocognitive adaptations in response to exercise in older adults with T2D, IR, or IGT, as well as attempt to identify any mechanistic links between changes in cognition and changes in metabolism, body composition, neurotrophic factors, and inflammation. In addition, this review and analysis were conducted only among persons using glucose-lowering drugs and/or insulin, as those are the published studies in diabetes with cognitive outcomes. Thus, we cannot clearly answer the question as to whether the relationship between exercise and cognition we observed would also be present in T2D treated only with lifestyle rather than medications for diabetes. The studies included did not systematically report all medication usage in participants, nor did they report subgroup analyses of cognitive adaptations in people on different medication regimens, such as those on insulin vs. oral hypoglycemics for example. Therefore, it was not possible to assess the influence of particular medications on cognitive responses to exercise. Future studies should provide more detail in this regard, and directly compare those on relevant medication regimens.
With only three RCT studies meeting the criteria for this review to date, further research is required to adequately address the relationship between the improvements in metabolic health and improvements in cognition in individuals with T2D. In particular, the data on cognitive function presented in these trials were secondary outcomes for all studies, and as such, the studies may have been underpowered, and there was not much discussion or exploration of the mechanistic links between the exercise and cognitive change. Many other putative mechanistic factors remain to be studied in this regard as well. Additionally, little is known regarding the persistence of therapeutic effects on cognition following the termination of exercise. The training regimes were relatively short and some lacked sufficient intensity to optimize neurophysiological or neuropsychological change. Dose-response relationships between exercise dose (both volume and intensity) and comparisons of different exercise modalities and cognitive outcomes in this cohort are completely lacking, and should also be a focus of the additional investigation. Furthermore, future studies will also need to use cognitive measures which are comprehensive and more sensitive to longitudinal change, and provide long-term follow up to assess the sustainability of any gains achieved during clinical trials. Arguably the most salient issue for the field is the expansion of outcomes to assess transfer of cognitive gains to activities of daily living, quality of life, and psychological well-being.

Conclusions

The limited data available suggest that aerobic exercise or lifestyle interventions may improve some aspects of cognition in older adults with T2D or IGT, including executive function, delayed memory, and global cognitive scores, but the effects are inconsistent and require further study. In the present review, exercise-induced improvements in insulin sensitivity and glucose levels were associated with the observed cognitive benefits, while there is insufficient evidence exploring any relationship with other physiological adaptations at this time. In addition, literature from other cohorts supports potential advantages for moderate-to-high intensity aerobic exercise, rather than the low-intensity aerobic training paradigms sometimes recommended clinically in T2D or IGT, but confirms the need for dose-response trials in this cohort. Furthermore, comparing study effects was difficult due to different neuropsychological tests used, and standardized cognitive test batteries would increase the ability to synthesize data across trials. Future research in this field should include high quality, robust, randomized controlled trials which enroll older adults with T2D or metabolic syndrome to confirm any beneficial effects of physical exercise on cognition in these cohorts specifically. Moreover, analyses of the relationships between exercise-induced improvements in insulin sensitivity, glucose levels, body composition, and other metabolic parameters that potentially mediate and/or moderate the cognitive adaptations are needed. Such investigations will lead to a better understanding of the underlying mechanisms and therefore to the design of interventions specifically targeting the specific needs of those with metabolic disease. This will promote optimal physical, cognitive and psychological health, and ultimately improve quality of life in such individuals.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge Dr.Yorgi Mavros who provided support with literature review.

Funding

All the authors did not receive any funding related to this manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.
The systematic review was a sub-study of the Graded Resistance Exercise And Type 2 Diabetes in Older adults, which was registered with the Australian Clinical Trials Registry (ACTRN12606000436572) on the 11th of October 2006. Ethical approval was obtained from Sydney South West Area Health Service the (Royal Prince Alfred Hospital Ethics Review Committee, Protocol No: X04–0096) on the 2nd of August 2006 and the University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee on the 4th of August 2006.
All authors provided consent for publication.

Competing interests

All authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

© Springer Medizin

Bis 11. April 2024 bestellen und im ersten Jahr 50 % sparen!

e.Med Innere Medizin

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Innere Medizin erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Innere Medizin, den Premium-Inhalten der internistischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten internistischen Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

© Springer Medizin

Bis 11. April 2024 bestellen und im ersten Jahr 50 % sparen!

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaw JE, Sicree RA, Zimmet PZ. Global estimates of the prevalence of diabetes for 2010 and 2030. Diabetes Res Clin Pract. 2010;87(1):4–14.CrossRefPubMed Shaw JE, Sicree RA, Zimmet PZ. Global estimates of the prevalence of diabetes for 2010 and 2030. Diabetes Res Clin Pract. 2010;87(1):4–14.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Rathmann W, Giani G. Global prevalence of diabetes: estimates for the year 2000 and projections for 2030. Diabetes Care. 2004;27(10):2568–9. author reply 69CrossRefPubMed Rathmann W, Giani G. Global prevalence of diabetes: estimates for the year 2000 and projections for 2030. Diabetes Care. 2004;27(10):2568–9. author reply 69CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat American Diabetes A. (2) Classification and diagnosis of diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2015;38 Suppl:S8-S16. American Diabetes A. (2) Classification and diagnosis of diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2015;38 Suppl:S8-S16.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhao Y, Ye W, Boye KS, Holcombe JH, Hall JA, Swindle R. Prevalence of other diabetes-associated complications and comorbidities and its impact on health care charges among patients with diabetic neuropathy. J Diabetes Complicat. 2010;24(1):9–19.CrossRefPubMed Zhao Y, Ye W, Boye KS, Holcombe JH, Hall JA, Swindle R. Prevalence of other diabetes-associated complications and comorbidities and its impact on health care charges among patients with diabetic neuropathy. J Diabetes Complicat. 2010;24(1):9–19.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Cukierman-Yaffe T, Gerstein HC, Williamson JD, Lazar RM, Lovato L, Miller ME, et al. Relationship between baseline glycemic control and cognitive function in individuals with type 2 diabetes and other cardiovascular risk factors: the action to control cardiovascular risk in diabetes-memory in diabetes (ACCORD-MIND) trial. Diabetes Care. 2009;32(2):221–6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cukierman-Yaffe T, Gerstein HC, Williamson JD, Lazar RM, Lovato L, Miller ME, et al. Relationship between baseline glycemic control and cognitive function in individuals with type 2 diabetes and other cardiovascular risk factors: the action to control cardiovascular risk in diabetes-memory in diabetes (ACCORD-MIND) trial. Diabetes Care. 2009;32(2):221–6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Strachan MW, Reynolds RM, Marioni RE, Price JF. Cognitive function, dementia and type 2 diabetes mellitus in the elderly. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2011;7(2):108–14.CrossRefPubMed Strachan MW, Reynolds RM, Marioni RE, Price JF. Cognitive function, dementia and type 2 diabetes mellitus in the elderly. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2011;7(2):108–14.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Cooray G, Nilsson E, Wahlin A, Laukka EJ, Brismar K, Brismar T. Effects of intensified metabolic control on CNS function in type 2 diabetes. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36(1):77–86.CrossRefPubMed Cooray G, Nilsson E, Wahlin A, Laukka EJ, Brismar K, Brismar T. Effects of intensified metabolic control on CNS function in type 2 diabetes. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36(1):77–86.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat SR-F J, Sa-Roriz TM, Rosset I, Camozzato AL, Santos AC, Chaves ML, et al. (Pre)diabetes, brain aging, and cognition. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2009;1792(5):432–43.CrossRef SR-F J, Sa-Roriz TM, Rosset I, Camozzato AL, Santos AC, Chaves ML, et al. (Pre)diabetes, brain aging, and cognition. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2009;1792(5):432–43.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Ryan CM, Geckle MO. Circumscribed cognitive dysfunction in middle-aged adults with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2000;23(10):1486–93.CrossRefPubMed Ryan CM, Geckle MO. Circumscribed cognitive dysfunction in middle-aged adults with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2000;23(10):1486–93.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Sommerfield AJ, Deary IJ, Frier BM. Acute hyperglycemia alters mood state and impairs cognitive performance in people with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2004;27(10):2335–40.CrossRefPubMed Sommerfield AJ, Deary IJ, Frier BM. Acute hyperglycemia alters mood state and impairs cognitive performance in people with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2004;27(10):2335–40.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Diabetes Prevention Program Research. G. The diabetes prevention program (DPP): description of lifestyle intervention. Diabetes Care. 2002;25(12):2165–71.CrossRef Diabetes Prevention Program Research. G. The diabetes prevention program (DPP): description of lifestyle intervention. Diabetes Care. 2002;25(12):2165–71.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Ahlskog JE, Geda YE, Graff-Radford NR, Petersen RC. Physical exercise as a preventive or disease-modifying treatment of dementia and brain aging. Mayo Clin Proc. 2011;86(9):876–84.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ahlskog JE, Geda YE, Graff-Radford NR, Petersen RC. Physical exercise as a preventive or disease-modifying treatment of dementia and brain aging. Mayo Clin Proc. 2011;86(9):876–84.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. 2003;14(2):125–30.CrossRefPubMed Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. 2003;14(2):125–30.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Etgen T, Sander D, Huntgeburth U, Poppert H, Forstl H, Bickel H. Physical activity and incident cognitive impairment in elderly persons: the INVADE study. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(2):186–93.CrossRefPubMed Etgen T, Sander D, Huntgeburth U, Poppert H, Forstl H, Bickel H. Physical activity and incident cognitive impairment in elderly persons: the INVADE study. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(2):186–93.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Larson EB, Wang L, Bowen JD, McCormick WC, Teri L, Crane P, et al. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and older. Ann Intern Med. 2006;144(2):73–81.CrossRefPubMed Larson EB, Wang L, Bowen JD, McCormick WC, Teri L, Crane P, et al. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and older. Ann Intern Med. 2006;144(2):73–81.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E, et al. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61(11):1166–70.CrossRefPubMed Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E, et al. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61(11):1166–70.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Cotman CW, Berchtold NC, Christie LA. Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30(9):464–72.CrossRefPubMed Cotman CW, Berchtold NC, Christie LA. Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30(9):464–72.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Colcombe SJ. Exercise, cognition, and the aging brain. J Appl Physiol (1985). 2006;101(4):1237–42.CrossRef Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Colcombe SJ. Exercise, cognition, and the aging brain. J Appl Physiol (1985). 2006;101(4):1237–42.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, et al. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(9):3316–21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, et al. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(9):3316–21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Voss MW, Khan KM, Handy TC. Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33(8):1690–8.CrossRefPubMed Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Voss MW, Khan KM, Handy TC. Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33(8):1690–8.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Nagamatsu LS, Handy TC, Hsu CL, Voss M, Liu-Ambrose T. Resistance training promotes cognitive and functional brain plasticity in seniors with probable mild cognitive impairment. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(8):666–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Nagamatsu LS, Handy TC, Hsu CL, Voss M, Liu-Ambrose T. Resistance training promotes cognitive and functional brain plasticity in seniors with probable mild cognitive impairment. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(8):666–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Suzuki T, Shimada H, Makizako H, Doi T, Yoshida D, Tsutsumimoto K, et al. Effects of multicomponent exercise on cognitive function in older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Neurol. 2012;12:128.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Suzuki T, Shimada H, Makizako H, Doi T, Yoshida D, Tsutsumimoto K, et al. Effects of multicomponent exercise on cognitive function in older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Neurol. 2012;12:128.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Gasparini L, Potential XH. Roles of insulin and IGF-1 in Alzheimer's disease. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26(8):404–6.CrossRefPubMed Gasparini L, Potential XH. Roles of insulin and IGF-1 in Alzheimer's disease. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26(8):404–6.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Helzner EP, Luchsinger JA, Scarmeas N, Cosentino S, Brickman AM, Glymour MM, et al. Contribution of vascular risk factors to the progression in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2009;66(3):343–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Helzner EP, Luchsinger JA, Scarmeas N, Cosentino S, Brickman AM, Glymour MM, et al. Contribution of vascular risk factors to the progression in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2009;66(3):343–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Gregory SM, Parker B, Thompson PD. Physical activity, cognitive function, and brain health: what is the role of exercise training in the prevention of dementia? Brain Sci. 2012;2(4):684–708.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gregory SM, Parker B, Thompson PD. Physical activity, cognitive function, and brain health: what is the role of exercise training in the prevention of dementia? Brain Sci. 2012;2(4):684–708.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Yau SY, Gil-Mohapel J, Christie BR, So KF. Physical exercise-induced adult neurogenesis: a good strategy to prevent cognitive decline in neurodegenerative diseases? Biomed Res Int. 2014;2014:403120.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Yau SY, Gil-Mohapel J, Christie BR, So KF. Physical exercise-induced adult neurogenesis: a good strategy to prevent cognitive decline in neurodegenerative diseases? Biomed Res Int. 2014;2014:403120.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Group P. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ. 2009;339:b2535.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Group P. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ. 2009;339:b2535.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
29.
Zurück zum Zitat American College of Sports M, Chodzko-Zajko WJ, Proctor DN, Fiatarone Singh MA, Minson CT, Nigg CR, et al. American College of Sports Medicine position stand. Exercise and physical activity for older adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2009;41:1510–30. American College of Sports M, Chodzko-Zajko WJ, Proctor DN, Fiatarone Singh MA, Minson CT, Nigg CR, et al. American College of Sports Medicine position stand. Exercise and physical activity for older adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2009;41:1510–30.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Paffenbarger RS Jr, Blair SN, Lee IM, Hyde RT. Measurement of physical activity to assess health effects in free-living populations. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1993;25(1):60–70.CrossRefPubMed Paffenbarger RS Jr, Blair SN, Lee IM, Hyde RT. Measurement of physical activity to assess health effects in free-living populations. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1993;25(1):60–70.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolf AM, Hunter DJ, Colditz GA, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Corsano KA, et al. Reproducibility and validity of a self-administered physical activity questionnaire. Int J Epidemiol. 1994;23(5):991–9.CrossRefPubMed Wolf AM, Hunter DJ, Colditz GA, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Corsano KA, et al. Reproducibility and validity of a self-administered physical activity questionnaire. Int J Epidemiol. 1994;23(5):991–9.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Maher CG, Sherrington C, Herbert RD, Moseley AM, Elkins M. Reliability of the PEDro scale for rating quality of randomized controlled trials. Phys Ther. 2003;83(8):713–21.PubMed Maher CG, Sherrington C, Herbert RD, Moseley AM, Elkins M. Reliability of the PEDro scale for rating quality of randomized controlled trials. Phys Ther. 2003;83(8):713–21.PubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris SB, DeShon RP. Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychol Methods. 2002;7(1):105–25.CrossRefPubMed Morris SB, DeShon RP. Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychol Methods. 2002;7(1):105–25.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Watson GS, Reger MA, Baker LD, McNeely MJ, Fujimoto WY, Kahn SE, et al. Effects of exercise and nutrition on memory in Japanese Americans with impaired glucose tolerance. Diabetes Care. 2006;29(1):135–6.CrossRefPubMed Watson GS, Reger MA, Baker LD, McNeely MJ, Fujimoto WY, Kahn SE, et al. Effects of exercise and nutrition on memory in Japanese Americans with impaired glucose tolerance. Diabetes Care. 2006;29(1):135–6.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Colberg SR, Somma CT, Sechrist SR. Physical activity participation may offset some of the negative impact of diabetes on cognitive function. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2008;9(6):434–8.CrossRefPubMed Colberg SR, Somma CT, Sechrist SR. Physical activity participation may offset some of the negative impact of diabetes on cognitive function. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2008;9(6):434–8.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Devore EE, Kang JH, Okereke O, Grodstein F. Physical activity levels and cognition in women with type 2 diabetes. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;170(8):1040–7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Devore EE, Kang JH, Okereke O, Grodstein F. Physical activity levels and cognition in women with type 2 diabetes. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;170(8):1040–7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Baker LD, Frank LL, Foster-Schubert K, Green PS, Wilkinson CW, McTiernan A, et al. Aerobic exercise improves cognition for older adults with glucose intolerance, a risk factor for Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;22(2):569–79.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Baker LD, Frank LL, Foster-Schubert K, Green PS, Wilkinson CW, McTiernan A, et al. Aerobic exercise improves cognition for older adults with glucose intolerance, a risk factor for Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;22(2):569–79.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Yanagawa M, Umegaki H, Uno T, Oyun K, Kawano N, Maeno H, et al. Association between improvements in insulin resistance and changes in cognitive function in elderly diabetic patients with normal cognitive function. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2011;11(3):341–7.CrossRefPubMed Yanagawa M, Umegaki H, Uno T, Oyun K, Kawano N, Maeno H, et al. Association between improvements in insulin resistance and changes in cognitive function in elderly diabetic patients with normal cognitive function. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2011;11(3):341–7.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Lehtisalo J, Lindstrom J, Ngandu T, Kivipelto M, Ahtiluoto S, Ilanne-Parikka P, et al. Association of Long-Term Dietary fat Intake, exercise, and weight with later cognitive function in the Finnish diabetes prevention study. J Nutr Health Aging. 2016;20(2):146–54.CrossRefPubMed Lehtisalo J, Lindstrom J, Ngandu T, Kivipelto M, Ahtiluoto S, Ilanne-Parikka P, et al. Association of Long-Term Dietary fat Intake, exercise, and weight with later cognitive function in the Finnish diabetes prevention study. J Nutr Health Aging. 2016;20(2):146–54.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Luchsinger JA. Type 2 diabetes, related conditions, in relation and dementia: an opportunity for prevention? J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;20(3):723–36.CrossRefPubMed Luchsinger JA. Type 2 diabetes, related conditions, in relation and dementia: an opportunity for prevention? J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;20(3):723–36.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Biessels GJ, Deary IJ, Ryan CM. Cognition and diabetes: a lifespan perspective. Lancet Neurol. 2008;7(2):184–90.CrossRefPubMed Biessels GJ, Deary IJ, Ryan CM. Cognition and diabetes: a lifespan perspective. Lancet Neurol. 2008;7(2):184–90.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Yuan XY, Wang XG. Mild cognitive impairment in type 2 diabetes mellitus and related risk factors: a review. Rev Neurosci. 2017;28(7):715–23.CrossRefPubMed Yuan XY, Wang XG. Mild cognitive impairment in type 2 diabetes mellitus and related risk factors: a review. Rev Neurosci. 2017;28(7):715–23.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Manschot SM, Biessels GJ, de Valk H, Algra A, Rutten GE, van der Grond J, et al. Metabolic and vascular determinants of impaired cognitive performance and abnormalities on brain magnetic resonance imaging in patients with type 2 diabetes. Diabetologia. 2007;50(11):2388–97.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Manschot SM, Biessels GJ, de Valk H, Algra A, Rutten GE, van der Grond J, et al. Metabolic and vascular determinants of impaired cognitive performance and abnormalities on brain magnetic resonance imaging in patients with type 2 diabetes. Diabetologia. 2007;50(11):2388–97.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Ryan JP, Fine DF, Rosano C. Type 2 diabetes and cognitive impairment: contributions from neuroimaging. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2014;27(1):47–55.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ryan JP, Fine DF, Rosano C. Type 2 diabetes and cognitive impairment: contributions from neuroimaging. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2014;27(1):47–55.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Kanaya AM, Davidowitz N, Barrett-Connor E, Krueger K. Diabetes, impaired fasting glucose, and development of cognitive impairment in older women. Neurology. 2004;63(4):658–63.CrossRefPubMed Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Kanaya AM, Davidowitz N, Barrett-Connor E, Krueger K. Diabetes, impaired fasting glucose, and development of cognitive impairment in older women. Neurology. 2004;63(4):658–63.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Peila R, Rodriguez BL, White LR, Launer LJ. Fasting insulin and incident dementia in an elderly population of Japanese-American men. Neurology. 2004;63(2):228–33.CrossRefPubMed Peila R, Rodriguez BL, White LR, Launer LJ. Fasting insulin and incident dementia in an elderly population of Japanese-American men. Neurology. 2004;63(2):228–33.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Luchsinger JA. Adiposity, hyperinsulinemia, diabetes and Alzheimer's disease: an epidemiological perspective. Eur J Pharmacol. 2008;585(1):119–29.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Luchsinger JA. Adiposity, hyperinsulinemia, diabetes and Alzheimer's disease: an epidemiological perspective. Eur J Pharmacol. 2008;585(1):119–29.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Biessels GJ, Reagan LP. Hippocampal insulin resistance and cognitive dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(11):660–71.CrossRefPubMed Biessels GJ, Reagan LP. Hippocampal insulin resistance and cognitive dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(11):660–71.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Hoffman BM, Cooper H, Strauman TA, Welsh-Bohmer K, et al. Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: a meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom Med. 2010;72(3):239–52.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Hoffman BM, Cooper H, Strauman TA, Welsh-Bohmer K, et al. Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: a meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom Med. 2010;72(3):239–52.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Angevaren M, Aufdemkampe G, Verhaar HJ, Aleman A, Vanhees L. Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2008;3:CD005381. Angevaren M, Aufdemkampe G, Verhaar HJ, Aleman A, Vanhees L. Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2008;3:CD005381.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Weinstein AM, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Chaddock L, Szabo A, White SM, et al. The association between aerobic fitness and executive function is mediated by prefrontal cortex volume. Brain Behav Immun. 2012;26(5):811–9.CrossRefPubMed Weinstein AM, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Chaddock L, Szabo A, White SM, et al. The association between aerobic fitness and executive function is mediated by prefrontal cortex volume. Brain Behav Immun. 2012;26(5):811–9.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Peiffer R, Darby LA, Fullenkamp A, Morgan AL. Effects of acute aerobic exercise on executive function in older women. J Sports Sci Med. 2015;14(3):574–83.PubMedPubMedCentral Peiffer R, Darby LA, Fullenkamp A, Morgan AL. Effects of acute aerobic exercise on executive function in older women. J Sports Sci Med. 2015;14(3):574–83.PubMedPubMedCentral
54.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Xie C, Wang X, Zhou C, Xu C, Chang YK. Exercise and dietary program-induced weight reduction is associated with cognitive function among obese adolescents: a longitudinal study. PeerJ. 2017;5:e3286.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Xie C, Wang X, Zhou C, Xu C, Chang YK. Exercise and dietary program-induced weight reduction is associated with cognitive function among obese adolescents: a longitudinal study. PeerJ. 2017;5:e3286.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Cotman CW, Berchtold NC. Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 2002;25(6):295–301.CrossRefPubMed Cotman CW, Berchtold NC. Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 2002;25(6):295–301.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Hamer M, Chida Y. Physical activity and risk of neurodegenerative disease: a systematic review of prospective evidence. Psychol Med. 2009;39(1):3–11.CrossRefPubMed Hamer M, Chida Y. Physical activity and risk of neurodegenerative disease: a systematic review of prospective evidence. Psychol Med. 2009;39(1):3–11.CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Angevaren M, Vanhees L, Wendel-Vos W, Verhaar HJ, Aufdemkampe G, Aleman A, et al. Intensity, but not duration, of physical activities is related to cognitive function. Eur J Cardiovasc Prev Rehabil. 2007;14(6):825–30.CrossRefPubMed Angevaren M, Vanhees L, Wendel-Vos W, Verhaar HJ, Aufdemkampe G, Aleman A, et al. Intensity, but not duration, of physical activities is related to cognitive function. Eur J Cardiovasc Prev Rehabil. 2007;14(6):825–30.CrossRefPubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Daly RM, Miller EG, Dunstan DW, Kerr DA, Solah V, Menzies D, et al. The effects of progressive resistance training combined with a whey-protein drink and vitamin D supplementation on glycaemic control, body composition and cardiometabolic risk factors in older adults with type 2 diabetes: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials. 2014;15:431.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Daly RM, Miller EG, Dunstan DW, Kerr DA, Solah V, Menzies D, et al. The effects of progressive resistance training combined with a whey-protein drink and vitamin D supplementation on glycaemic control, body composition and cardiometabolic risk factors in older adults with type 2 diabetes: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials. 2014;15:431.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Mavros Y, Kay S, Simpson KA, Baker MK, Wang Y, Zhao RR, et al. Reductions in C-reactive protein in older adults with type 2 diabetes are related to improvements in body composition following a randomized controlled trial of resistance training. J Cachexia Sarcopenia Muscle. 2014;5(2):111–20.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Mavros Y, Kay S, Simpson KA, Baker MK, Wang Y, Zhao RR, et al. Reductions in C-reactive protein in older adults with type 2 diabetes are related to improvements in body composition following a randomized controlled trial of resistance training. J Cachexia Sarcopenia Muscle. 2014;5(2):111–20.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Misra A, Alappan NK, Vikram NK, Goel K, Gupta N, Mittal K, et al. Effect of supervised progressive resistance-exercise training protocol on insulin sensitivity, glycemia, lipids, and body composition in Asian Indians with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2008;31(7):1282–7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Misra A, Alappan NK, Vikram NK, Goel K, Gupta N, Mittal K, et al. Effect of supervised progressive resistance-exercise training protocol on insulin sensitivity, glycemia, lipids, and body composition in Asian Indians with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2008;31(7):1282–7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Peake JM, Kukuljan S, Nowson CA, Sanders K, Daly RM. Inflammatory cytokine responses to progressive resistance training and supplementation with fortified milk in men aged 50+ years: an 18-month randomized controlled trial. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2011;111(12):3079–88.CrossRefPubMed Peake JM, Kukuljan S, Nowson CA, Sanders K, Daly RM. Inflammatory cytokine responses to progressive resistance training and supplementation with fortified milk in men aged 50+ years: an 18-month randomized controlled trial. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2011;111(12):3079–88.CrossRefPubMed
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Craft S. Insulin resistance and Alzheimer's disease pathogenesis: potential mechanisms and implications for treatment. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2007;4(2):147–52.CrossRefPubMed Craft S. Insulin resistance and Alzheimer's disease pathogenesis: potential mechanisms and implications for treatment. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2007;4(2):147–52.CrossRefPubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Cassilhas RC, Lee KS, Fernandes J, Oliveira MG, Tufik S, Meeusen R, et al. Spatial memory is improved by aerobic and resistance exercise through divergent molecular mechanisms. Neuroscience. 2012;202:309–17.CrossRefPubMed Cassilhas RC, Lee KS, Fernandes J, Oliveira MG, Tufik S, Meeusen R, et al. Spatial memory is improved by aerobic and resistance exercise through divergent molecular mechanisms. Neuroscience. 2012;202:309–17.CrossRefPubMed
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Cassilhas RC, Lee KS, Venancio DP, Oliveira MG, Tufik S, de Mello MT. Resistance exercise improves hippocampus-dependent memory. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2012;45(12):1215–20.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cassilhas RC, Lee KS, Venancio DP, Oliveira MG, Tufik S, de Mello MT. Resistance exercise improves hippocampus-dependent memory. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2012;45(12):1215–20.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Yarrow JF, White LJ, McCoy SC, Borst SE. Training augments resistance exercise induced elevation of circulating brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). Neurosci Lett. 2010;479(2):161–5.CrossRefPubMed Yarrow JF, White LJ, McCoy SC, Borst SE. Training augments resistance exercise induced elevation of circulating brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). Neurosci Lett. 2010;479(2):161–5.CrossRefPubMed
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Parise G, Phillips SM, Kaczor JJ, Tarnopolsky MA. Antioxidant enzyme activity is up-regulated after unilateral resistance exercise training in older adults. Free Radic Biol Med. 2005;39(2):289–95.CrossRefPubMed Parise G, Phillips SM, Kaczor JJ, Tarnopolsky MA. Antioxidant enzyme activity is up-regulated after unilateral resistance exercise training in older adults. Free Radic Biol Med. 2005;39(2):289–95.CrossRefPubMed
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Ogawa K, Sanada K, Machida S, Okutsu M, Suzuki K. Resistance exercise training-induced muscle hypertrophy was associated with reduction of inflammatory markers in elderly women. Mediat Inflamm. 2010;2010:171023.CrossRef Ogawa K, Sanada K, Machida S, Okutsu M, Suzuki K. Resistance exercise training-induced muscle hypertrophy was associated with reduction of inflammatory markers in elderly women. Mediat Inflamm. 2010;2010:171023.CrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Kirschbaum C, Wolf OT, May M, Wippich W, Hellhammer DH. Stress- and treatment-induced elevations of cortisol levels associated with impaired declarative memory in healthy adults. Life Sci. 1996;58(17):1475–83.CrossRefPubMed Kirschbaum C, Wolf OT, May M, Wippich W, Hellhammer DH. Stress- and treatment-induced elevations of cortisol levels associated with impaired declarative memory in healthy adults. Life Sci. 1996;58(17):1475–83.CrossRefPubMed
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Cotman CW, Engesser-Cesar C. Exercise enhances and protects brain function. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2002;30(2):75–9.CrossRefPubMed Cotman CW, Engesser-Cesar C. Exercise enhances and protects brain function. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2002;30(2):75–9.CrossRefPubMed
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Iglay HB, Thyfault JP, Apolzan JW, Campbell WW. Resistance training and dietary protein: effects on glucose tolerance and contents of skeletal muscle insulin signaling proteins in older persons. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85(4):1005–13.PubMed Iglay HB, Thyfault JP, Apolzan JW, Campbell WW. Resistance training and dietary protein: effects on glucose tolerance and contents of skeletal muscle insulin signaling proteins in older persons. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85(4):1005–13.PubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Exercise or physical activity and cognitive function in adults with type 2 diabetes, insulin resistance or impaired glucose tolerance: a systematic review
verfasst von
Ren Ru Zhao
Anthony J. O’Sullivan
Maria A. Fiatarone Singh
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2018
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
European Review of Aging and Physical Activity / Ausgabe 1/2018
Print ISSN: 1813-7253
Elektronische ISSN: 1861-6909
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s11556-018-0190-1

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

European Review of Aging and Physical Activity 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Innere Medizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Innere Medizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.