Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Behavioral Medicine 6/2017

19.06.2017

Experiencing is believing: prior experience moderates the impact of self-based and socially-based cues in the context of blood donation

verfasst von: Lindsay Roberts, Jason P. Rose, Erin A. Vogel

Erschienen in: Journal of Behavioral Medicine | Ausgabe 6/2017

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Abstract

Two studies explored how self-based cues (i.e., self-efficacy), socially-based cues (i.e., perceived social norms), and prior blood donation experience differentially influence behavioral intentions. In Study 1, undergraduate students (N = 766) completed an online study that evaluated prior experiences, self-efficacy, perceived norms, and behavioral intentions in the context of blood donation. In Study 2, a community sample (N = 199) from a clinic waiting room completed similar measures. Across both studies, having high self-efficacy was a necessary and sufficient antecedent to high intentions, regardless of norm perception for donors. For non-donors, however, high self-efficacy was necessary but not sufficient; non-donors’ intentions were higher when giving blood was perceived to be normative, but far lower when it was not. When self-efficacy was low, the effects of experience and norms did not exert meaningful effects and donation intentions were quite low. These results demonstrate that the impact of self-based and socially-based cues on behavioral intentions may differ as a function of experience. The findings can inform public health initiatives and enhance the accuracy of theoretical models by directly examining experience as a moderator.
Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
In the current context, our operationalization of self-efficacy beliefs most closely corresponds to belief extremity (i.e., high perceptions of efficacy vs. low perceptions of efficacy), but this could similarly reflect the strength of the belief (i.e., stronger efficacy beliefs vs. weaker efficacy beliefs).
 
2
Percentage-based norm items were not included in the norms index and thus excluded from subsequent analyses. This was done for two main reasons. First, a substantial proportion of responses (ranging from 18 to 28%) for these items were missing prior to imputation. The pre- and post-imputation responses were highly variable, suggesting that participants may have been guessing when providing their estimates. Second, omitting these items from further analyses makes Study 1 more directly comparable to Study 2 (due to the absence of free-response items in Study 2). Therefore, our perceived norm indices for both studies contain only averaged and standardized Likert-type items. However, it is notable that even when these items remain in the model, the results do not differ from what is reported here. See the online supplementary material for descriptive statistics and regression analyses.
 
3
Initially, 137 participants (17% of the sample) indicated that they were ineligible to donate to the best of their knowledge. Since this was a relatively large proportion of the overall sample, we surmised that many participants were likely reporting personal preferences (e.g., fear of needles) rather than their actual eligibility status. Thus, we used both exclusion criteria to identify participants who were most likely accurately reporting their ineligibility.
 
4
After deleting the 44 cases with a large percentage of missing data, the overall incidence of remaining missing values was less than 5%. Cheema (2014) has suggested that expectation maximization imputation provides the greatest increase in accuracy for samples of this size in a multiple regression context (compared to other imputation methods).
 
5
Following prior work (e.g., Baldwin et al., 2006; Humphreys et al., 1998), we also qualitatively examined the efficacy × norms interaction in donors and non-donors separately. In both studies, the efficacy × norms interaction remained significant for non-donors but was non-significant for donors. See the online supplementary material for regression coefficients (Table 5), simple slope tests, and a discussion of these results.
 
6
In contrast to Study 1, multiple imputation was used in Study 2 due to the comparatively smaller sample size. In general, multiple imputation tends to outperform expectation maximization imputation when sample sizes are low (Cheema, 2014).
 
7
Bandura (1997) has argued that the efficacy-behavior relationship is far from unidirectional; self-efficacy is both a cause and a consequence of experience in a given domain, and perceptions of efficacy also alter memories of past performance.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.CrossRef Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ajzen, I. (2002). Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 665–683.CrossRef Ajzen, I. (2002). Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 665–683.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Albarracín, D., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (2000). The cognitive impact of past behavior: Influences on beliefs, attitudes, and future behavioral decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 5–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Albarracín, D., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (2000). The cognitive impact of past behavior: Influences on beliefs, attitudes, and future behavioral decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 5–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Bagozzi, R. P. (1981). Attitudes, intentions, and behavior: A test of some key hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 607–627.CrossRef Bagozzi, R. P. (1981). Attitudes, intentions, and behavior: A test of some key hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 607–627.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Baldwin, A. S., Rothman, A. J., Hertel, A. W., Linde, J. A., Jeffery, R. W., Finch, E. A., et al. (2006). Specifying the determinants of the initiation and maintenance of behavior change: An examination of self-efficacy, satisfaction, and smoking cessation. Health Psychology, 25, 626–634.CrossRefPubMed Baldwin, A. S., Rothman, A. J., Hertel, A. W., Linde, J. A., Jeffery, R. W., Finch, E. A., et al. (2006). Specifying the determinants of the initiation and maintenance of behavior change: An examination of self-efficacy, satisfaction, and smoking cessation. Health Psychology, 25, 626–634.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman.
Zurück zum Zitat Bednall, T. C., & Bove, L. L. (2011). Donating blood: A meta-analytic review of self-reported motivators and deterrents. Transfusion Medicine Reviews, 25, 317–334.CrossRefPubMed Bednall, T. C., & Bove, L. L. (2011). Donating blood: A meta-analytic review of self-reported motivators and deterrents. Transfusion Medicine Reviews, 25, 317–334.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bednall, T. C., Bove, L. L., Cheetham, A., & Murray, A. L. (2013). A systematic review and meta-analysis of antecedents of blood donation behavior and intentions. Social Science and Medicine, 96, 86–94.CrossRefPubMed Bednall, T. C., Bove, L. L., Cheetham, A., & Murray, A. L. (2013). A systematic review and meta-analysis of antecedents of blood donation behavior and intentions. Social Science and Medicine, 96, 86–94.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bentler, P. M., & Speckart, G. (1979). Models of attitude–behavior relations. Psychological Review, 86, 452–464.CrossRef Bentler, P. M., & Speckart, G. (1979). Models of attitude–behavior relations. Psychological Review, 86, 452–464.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Boulware, L. E., Ratner, L. E., Ness, P. M., Cooper, L. A., Campbell-Lee, S., LaVeist, T. A., et al. (2002). The contribution of sociodemographic, medical, and attitudinal factors to blood donation among the general public. Transfusion, 42, 669–678.CrossRefPubMed Boulware, L. E., Ratner, L. E., Ness, P. M., Cooper, L. A., Campbell-Lee, S., LaVeist, T. A., et al. (2002). The contribution of sociodemographic, medical, and attitudinal factors to blood donation among the general public. Transfusion, 42, 669–678.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Brug, J., Lechner, L., & De Vries, H. (1995). Psychosocial determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption. Appetite, 25, 285–296.CrossRefPubMed Brug, J., Lechner, L., & De Vries, H. (1995). Psychosocial determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption. Appetite, 25, 285–296.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Buunk, B. P., & Bakker, A. B. (1995). Extradyadic sex: The role of descriptive and injunctive norms. Journal of Sex Research, 32, 313–318.CrossRef Buunk, B. P., & Bakker, A. B. (1995). Extradyadic sex: The role of descriptive and injunctive norms. Journal of Sex Research, 32, 313–318.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Callero, P. L., & Piliavin, J. A. (1983). Developing a commitment to blood donation: The impact of one’s first experience. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 13, 1–16.CrossRef Callero, P. L., & Piliavin, J. A. (1983). Developing a commitment to blood donation: The impact of one’s first experience. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 13, 1–16.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Camerer, C. F., & Lovallo, D. (1999). Overconfidence and excess entry: An experimental approach. American Economic Review, 89, 306–318.CrossRef Camerer, C. F., & Lovallo, D. (1999). Overconfidence and excess entry: An experimental approach. American Economic Review, 89, 306–318.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chan, D. K. S., & Fishbein, M. (1993). Determinants of college women’s intentions to tell their partners to use condoms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 1455–1470.CrossRef Chan, D. K. S., & Fishbein, M. (1993). Determinants of college women’s intentions to tell their partners to use condoms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 1455–1470.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cheema, J. R. (2014). Some general guidelines for choosing missing data handling methods in educational research. Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods, 13, 53–75.CrossRef Cheema, J. R. (2014). Some general guidelines for choosing missing data handling methods in educational research. Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods, 13, 53–75.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the theory of planned behavior: A review and avenues for further research. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1429–1464.CrossRef Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the theory of planned behavior: A review and avenues for further research. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1429–1464.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Conner, M., Martin, E., Silverdale, N., & Grogan, S. (1996). Dieting in adolescence: An application of the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 1, 315–325.CrossRef Conner, M., Martin, E., Silverdale, N., & Grogan, S. (1996). Dieting in adolescence: An application of the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 1, 315–325.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dawson, J. F., & Richter, A. W. (2006). Probing three-way interactions in moderated multiple regression: Development and application of a slope difference test. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 917–926.CrossRefPubMed Dawson, J. F., & Richter, A. W. (2006). Probing three-way interactions in moderated multiple regression: Development and application of a slope difference test. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 917–926.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers. Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers.
Zurück zum Zitat Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1978). On the predictive validity of attitudes: The roles of direct experience and confidence. Journal of Personality, 46, 228–243.CrossRef Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1978). On the predictive validity of attitudes: The roles of direct experience and confidence. Journal of Personality, 46, 228–243.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ferguson, E., & Bibby, P. A. (2002). Predicting future blood donor returns: Past behavior, intentions, and observer effects. Health Psychology, 21, 513–518.CrossRefPubMed Ferguson, E., & Bibby, P. A. (2002). Predicting future blood donor returns: Past behavior, intentions, and observer effects. Health Psychology, 21, 513–518.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.CrossRef Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat France, C. R., France, J. L., Roussos, M., & Ditto, B. (2004). Mild reactions to blood donation predict a decreased likelihood of donor return. Transfusion and Apheresis Science, 30, 17–22.CrossRefPubMed France, C. R., France, J. L., Roussos, M., & Ditto, B. (2004). Mild reactions to blood donation predict a decreased likelihood of donor return. Transfusion and Apheresis Science, 30, 17–22.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat France, C. R., Rader, A., & Carlson, B. (2005). Donors who react may not come back: Analysis of repeat donation as a function of phlebotomist ratings of vasovagal reactions. Transfusion and Apheresis Science, 33, 99–106.CrossRefPubMed France, C. R., Rader, A., & Carlson, B. (2005). Donors who react may not come back: Analysis of repeat donation as a function of phlebotomist ratings of vasovagal reactions. Transfusion and Apheresis Science, 33, 99–106.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Giles, M., Mcclenahan, C., Cairns, E., & Mallet, J. (2004). An application of the theory of planned behaviour to blood donation: The importance of self-efficacy. Health Education Research, 19, 380–391.CrossRefPubMed Giles, M., Mcclenahan, C., Cairns, E., & Mallet, J. (2004). An application of the theory of planned behaviour to blood donation: The importance of self-efficacy. Health Education Research, 19, 380–391.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gillespie, T. W., & Hillyer, C. D. (2002). Blood donors and factors impacting the blood donation decision. Transfusion Medicine Reviews, 16, 115–130.CrossRefPubMed Gillespie, T. W., & Hillyer, C. D. (2002). Blood donors and factors impacting the blood donation decision. Transfusion Medicine Reviews, 16, 115–130.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Glynn, S. A., Kleinman, S. H., Schreiber, G. B., Zuck, T., Mc Combs, S., Bethel, J., et al. (2002). Motivations to donate blood: Demographic comparisons. Transfusion, 42, 216–225.CrossRef Glynn, S. A., Kleinman, S. H., Schreiber, G. B., Zuck, T., Mc Combs, S., Bethel, J., et al. (2002). Motivations to donate blood: Demographic comparisons. Transfusion, 42, 216–225.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Godin, G., & Kok, G. (1996). The theory of planned behavior: A review of its applications to health-related behaviors. American Journal of Health Promotion, 11, 87–98.CrossRefPubMed Godin, G., & Kok, G. (1996). The theory of planned behavior: A review of its applications to health-related behaviors. American Journal of Health Promotion, 11, 87–98.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Godin, G., Sheeran, P., Conner, M., & Germain, M. (2008). Asking questions changes behavior: Mere measurement effects on frequency of blood donation. Health Psychology, 27, 179–184.CrossRefPubMed Godin, G., Sheeran, P., Conner, M., & Germain, M. (2008). Asking questions changes behavior: Mere measurement effects on frequency of blood donation. Health Psychology, 27, 179–184.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Graham, J. W., Cumsille, P. E., & Elek-Fisk, E. (2003). Methods for handling missing data. In J. A. Schinka & W. F. Velicer (Eds.), Handbook of Psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 87–114). New York, NY: Wiley. Graham, J. W., Cumsille, P. E., & Elek-Fisk, E. (2003). Methods for handling missing data. In J. A. Schinka & W. F. Velicer (Eds.), Handbook of Psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 87–114). New York, NY: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2005). First-and higher-order models of attitudes, normative influence, and perceived behavioural control in the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 513–535.CrossRefPubMed Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2005). First-and higher-order models of attitudes, normative influence, and perceived behavioural control in the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 513–535.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N., & Biddle, S. J. (2001). The influence of self-efficacy and past behaviour on the physical activity intentions of young people. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19, 711–725.CrossRefPubMed Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N., & Biddle, S. J. (2001). The influence of self-efficacy and past behaviour on the physical activity intentions of young people. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19, 711–725.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Humphreys, A. S., Thompson, N. J., & Miner, K. R. (1998). Intention to breastfeed in low-income pregnant women: The role of social support and previous experience. Birth, 25, 169–174.CrossRefPubMed Humphreys, A. S., Thompson, N. J., & Miner, K. R. (1998). Intention to breastfeed in low-income pregnant women: The role of social support and previous experience. Birth, 25, 169–174.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kallgren, C. A., Reno, R. R., & Cialdini, R. B. (2000). A focus theory of normative conduct: When norms do and do not affect behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1002–1012.CrossRef Kallgren, C. A., Reno, R. R., & Cialdini, R. B. (2000). A focus theory of normative conduct: When norms do and do not affect behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1002–1012.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kam, J. A., Matsunaga, M., Hecht, M. L., & Ndiaye, K. (2009). Extending the theory of planned behavior to predict alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana use among youth of Mexican heritage. Prevention Science, 10, 41–53.CrossRefPubMed Kam, J. A., Matsunaga, M., Hecht, M. L., & Ndiaye, K. (2009). Extending the theory of planned behavior to predict alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana use among youth of Mexican heritage. Prevention Science, 10, 41–53.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kidwell, B., & Jewell, R. D. (2003). The moderated influence of internal control: An examination across health-related behaviors. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 377–386.CrossRef Kidwell, B., & Jewell, R. D. (2003). The moderated influence of internal control: An examination across health-related behaviors. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 377–386.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lewis, M. A., & Neighbors, C. (2006). Social norms approaches using descriptive drinking norms education: A review of the research on personalized normative feedback. Journal of American College Health, 54, 213–218.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lewis, M. A., & Neighbors, C. (2006). Social norms approaches using descriptive drinking norms education: A review of the research on personalized normative feedback. Journal of American College Health, 54, 213–218.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Mann, T., Sherman, D., & Updegraff, J. (2004). Dispositional motivations and message framing: A test of the congruency hypothesis in college students. Health Psychology, 23, 330–334.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Mann, T., Sherman, D., & Updegraff, J. (2004). Dispositional motivations and message framing: A test of the congruency hypothesis in college students. Health Psychology, 23, 330–334.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Masser, B. M., White, K. M., Hyde, M. K., Terry, D. J., & Robinson, N. G. (2009). Predicting blood donation intentions and behavior among Australian blood donors: Testing an extended theory of planned behavior model. Transfusion, 49, 320–329.CrossRefPubMed Masser, B. M., White, K. M., Hyde, M. K., Terry, D. J., & Robinson, N. G. (2009). Predicting blood donation intentions and behavior among Australian blood donors: Testing an extended theory of planned behavior model. Transfusion, 49, 320–329.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat McMillan, B., & Conner, M. (2003). Applying an extended version of the theory of planned behavior to illicit drug use among students. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 1662–1683.CrossRef McMillan, B., & Conner, M. (2003). Applying an extended version of the theory of planned behavior to illicit drug use among students. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 1662–1683.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Montaño, D. E., & Kasprzyk, D. (2008). Theory of reasoned action, theory of planned behavior, and the integrated behavioral model. In K. Glanz, B. K. Rimer, & K. Viswanath (Eds.), Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 67–96). San Francisco, CA: Wiley. Montaño, D. E., & Kasprzyk, D. (2008). Theory of reasoned action, theory of planned behavior, and the integrated behavioral model. In K. Glanz, B. K. Rimer, & K. Viswanath (Eds.), Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 67–96). San Francisco, CA: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Norman, P., Conner, M., & Bell, R. (2000). The theory of planned behavior and exercise: Evidence for the moderating role of past behavior. British Journal of Health Psychology, 5, 249–261.CrossRef Norman, P., Conner, M., & Bell, R. (2000). The theory of planned behavior and exercise: Evidence for the moderating role of past behavior. British Journal of Health Psychology, 5, 249–261.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Notari, I. V., Edward, P., Zou, S., Fang, C. T., Eder, A. F., Benjamin, R. J., et al. (2009). Age-related donor return patterns among first-time blood donors in the United States. Transfusion, 49, 2229–2236.CrossRefPubMed Notari, I. V., Edward, P., Zou, S., Fang, C. T., Eder, A. F., Benjamin, R. J., et al. (2009). Age-related donor return patterns among first-time blood donors in the United States. Transfusion, 49, 2229–2236.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 54–74.CrossRef Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 54–74.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rivis, A., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Descriptive norms as an additional predictor in the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis. Current Psychology, 22, 218–233.CrossRef Rivis, A., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Descriptive norms as an additional predictor in the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis. Current Psychology, 22, 218–233.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rose, J. P., Geers, A. L., France, J. L., & France, C. R. (2014). Norm perception and communication for vasovagal symptoms in blood donation. Transfusion, 54, 2258–2266.CrossRefPubMed Rose, J. P., Geers, A. L., France, J. L., & France, C. R. (2014). Norm perception and communication for vasovagal symptoms in blood donation. Transfusion, 54, 2258–2266.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147–177.CrossRefPubMed Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147–177.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Schlegel, R. P., D’Avernas, J. R., Zanna, M. P., DeCourville, N. H., & Manske, S. R. (1992). Problem drinking: A problem for the theory of reasoned action? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 358–385.CrossRef Schlegel, R. P., D’Avernas, J. R., Zanna, M. P., DeCourville, N. H., & Manske, S. R. (1992). Problem drinking: A problem for the theory of reasoned action? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 358–385.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention–Behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. European Review of Social Psychology, 12, 1–36.CrossRef Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention–Behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. European Review of Social Psychology, 12, 1–36.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1998). Do intentions predict condom use? Meta-analysis and examination of six moderator variables. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 231–252.CrossRefPubMed Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1998). Do intentions predict condom use? Meta-analysis and examination of six moderator variables. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 231–252.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Sojka, B. N., & Sojka, P. (2008). The blood donation experience: Self‐reported motives and obstacles for donating blood. Vox Sanguinis, 94, 56–63.PubMed Sojka, B. N., & Sojka, P. (2008). The blood donation experience: Self‐reported motives and obstacles for donating blood. Vox Sanguinis, 94, 56–63.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Strecher, V. J., DeVellis, B. M., Becker, M. H., & Rosenstock, I. M. (1986). The role of self-efficacy in achieving health behavior change. Health Education & Behavior, 13, 73–92. Strecher, V. J., DeVellis, B. M., Becker, M. H., & Rosenstock, I. M. (1986). The role of self-efficacy in achieving health behavior change. Health Education & Behavior, 13, 73–92.
Zurück zum Zitat Weber, E. U., Blais, A. R., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15, 263–290.CrossRef Weber, E. U., Blais, A. R., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15, 263–290.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Williams-Piehota, P., Schneider, T. R., Pizarro, J., Mowad, L., & Salovey, P. (2004). Matching health messages to health locus of control beliefs for promoting mammography utilization. Psychology & Health, 19, 407–423.CrossRef Williams-Piehota, P., Schneider, T. R., Pizarro, J., Mowad, L., & Salovey, P. (2004). Matching health messages to health locus of control beliefs for promoting mammography utilization. Psychology & Health, 19, 407–423.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 231–248.CrossRef Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 231–248.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Yzer, M. (2007). Does perceived control moderate attitudinal and normative effects on intention? A review of conceptual and methodological issues. In I. Ajzen, D. Albarracín, & R. C. Hornik (Eds.), Prediction and change of health behavior: Applying the reasoned action approach (pp. 111–217). Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum. Yzer, M. (2007). Does perceived control moderate attitudinal and normative effects on intention? A review of conceptual and methodological issues. In I. Ajzen, D. Albarracín, & R. C. Hornik (Eds.), Prediction and change of health behavior: Applying the reasoned action approach (pp. 111–217). Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Metadaten
Titel
Experiencing is believing: prior experience moderates the impact of self-based and socially-based cues in the context of blood donation
verfasst von
Lindsay Roberts
Jason P. Rose
Erin A. Vogel
Publikationsdatum
19.06.2017
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Journal of Behavioral Medicine / Ausgabe 6/2017
Print ISSN: 0160-7715
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-3521
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-017-9862-y

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 6/2017

Journal of Behavioral Medicine 6/2017 Zur Ausgabe

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.