Introduction
Assessment of memory in people with suspected Alzheimer’s disease (AD) has long focused on episodic memory, being its associative forms the most commonly targeted (
1). This practice stems from the shared view that early AD pathology affects the hippocampus, a medial temporal lobe region known to support the formation of episodic memory via associative representations (
2). Our understanding of memory decline in AD has increased considerably since traditional memory tests used to aid its diagnosis were developed. A hypothetical model of memory decline in AD rooted in Braak’s pathological stages (
3) suggests that hippocampal atrophy, resulting from the accumulation of neurofibrillary tangles, appears rather late in the disease continuum. There is first a sub-hippocampal stage during which, regions of the anterior temporal lobe network are targeted by the disease. Such regions (e.g., entorhinal and perirhinal cortex) are involved in context-free memory functions such as familiarity-based recognition. Regions of this network are affected by AD earlier than the hippocampus (
4), spared in normal aging (
5), and involved in context-free memory (
3). Therefore, tests that tax the functional integrity of this network will more likely detect AD-related impairments in its preclinical stages. Recent evidence supports this notion. For example, Norton et al. (
6) focused on the entorhinal cortex and inferior temporal lobe as the former is thought to be the first location of tau buildup in AD while the latter represents the best proxy of early tau spreading to neocortex. The authors hypothesize that deficits in Visual Short-Term Memory Binding (VSTMB) would be more likely related to tau deposition in such regions given the evidence confirming its early decline in preclinical AD. It is worth noting that such regions are those thought to underpin context-free memory functions (
3,
7) of which VSTMB is a clear example.
VSTMB (
8) is a cognitive function that supports the integration and temporary retention of object’s features such as shape and color into unified representations. The function does not rely on the integrity of the hippocampus (
9), is affected by AD prior to its hippocampal stages (
10), and has proved insensitive to normal aging (
11,
12). The VSTMB task (VSTMBT) is seemingly indexing very early neuropathological changes associated to the AD continuum. For instance, VSTMB correlates with Amyloid-β (Aβ) deposits in individuals who are in the preclinical stages of familial AD (i.e., E280A-PSEN1 mutation (
6)) and in those expressing the early prodromal stages of sporadic AD (
13) before any overt neurodegeneration is observed. The ability of memory tests to index AD pathology is a topic of ongoing research (
14). Within the context, tests that assess the ability to hold bindings of items with own identity in memory (i.e., associative memory tests such as the Selective Reminding Test, FCSRT (
15,
16)) or bindings of distinct features (shape, colors) which make up objects’ identity (i.e., conjunctive memory tests), have become increasingly popular (
1). Accrued evidence suggests that conjunctive and associative forms of memory binding are dissociable (
9,
17) with the former being supported by regions of the anterior medial temporal lobe network and the latter by regions of the posterior medial temporal lobe network (
3,
7,
18). Although both forms of memory are affected in the early stages of AD, recent evidence suggests that the temporal pattern (i.e., “when”) of such impairments can now be detected with sensitive neuropsychological tests (
19‐
21).
There is an urgent need for cognitive tests that can help detect the transition from normal to abnormal aging and monitor disease progression. Meeting such needs is proving challenging. Based on traditional neuropsychological and clinical assessments we have been allocating older adults who do not provide signals of AD (or other dementias) to control groups. Evidence has accrued suggesting that older adults who are still asymptomatic may be accumulating AD-related pathology and some show significant resilience to such changes (
22). It will be ideal to identify memory tests which (
1) are sensitive and specific to AD, (
2) correlate with the accumulation of abnormal proteins in the brain linked to the development of AD dementia, and (
3) are not sensitive to the brain changes that accompany normal aging. The VSTMB test seems to hold these properties. However, such a test has never been used to investigate if among those still healthy older adults there are individuals who show VSTMB decline that can be accounted for by the accumulation of AD related brain pathology. This was the aim of the present study. Based on the above reviewed evidence we predicted that cognitively unimpaired older adults with selective VSTMB impairment would also display a significant increase of Amyloid-β in their brains.
Discussion
The present study was set out to investigate the hypothesis that cognitively unimpaired older adults who present with VSTMB impairments would display increased brain Aβ deposits relative to those whose VSTMB remain preserved. This hypothesis proved valid. We also found that such an association occurred when neither measures of grey matter integrity nor standard neuropsychological tests could identify differences between these groups. These findings have important implications for our understanding of the boundaries between normal and pathological cognitive aging and for the preclinical detection of Alzheimer’s disease. We discuss such implications in turn.
VSTMB has been found to remain preserved across the lifespan (
11,
12,
44) and to be unaffected by the level of education of those assessed (
45). The hypothesis that VSTMB impairments in asymptomatic older adults would reflect early Aβ pathology stemmed from recent studies in individuals at high risk of AD. VSTMB impairments have been found in middle-age adults who would inevitably develop familial AD due to the mutation E280A-PSEN1 (
10) and who were otherwise completely asymptomatic. Aβ deposits in such carriers reach a plateau at the mean age of 35 (
46), which is when VSTMB impairments were first observed (
10). The association between Aβ and VSTMB impairments in asymptomatic carriers of the mutation E280A-PSEN1 becomes apparent before evidence of tau pathology or neurodegeneration (
6). Interestingly, such an association also characterizes individuals at risk of late-onset sporadic AD (i.e., mild cognitive impairment) (
13). The still scarce yet converging evidence suggests that VSTMB deficits might be associated to the earliest pathological changes that underpin the transition from normal aging to AD, that is, β-amyloidopathy.
Disentangling normal and pathological cognitive aging is a challenge that neuropsychological tests are currently facing (
20). There is growing concern about the reliability of norms or control groups as the available neuropsychological tests currently used to ascertain normality are outdated and do not detect the earliest cognitive deficits caused by neurodegenerative disease. The traditional neuropsychological tests used in our study proved insensitive to the increased Aβ observed in older adults with poor VSTMB functions. In fact, participants enrolled in this study were recruited relying on strict inclusion criteria for normal cognitive aging. Yet, almost half of them presented with a behavioral VSTMB profile compatible with that consistently observed in individuals with or at risk of AD (
10,
47,
48) (Figure
2). In fact, the performance of the HBC group on the baseline conditions was numerically superior to that of the LBC group, which our manipulation (i.e., classification into LBC and HBC) would not predict. That is, our manipulation did anticipate that relative to LBC, HBC would show significantly poorer performance on the Shape-Color Binding Condition. This is the dissociation previously observed in people with or at risk of AD (
10,
30,
31) and our data confirmed this prediction (see Figure
2). However, such manipulation would warrant neither equivalent performance on single feature conditions nor a significant Group x Condition Interaction. These findings therefore grant us confidence that HBC did present with the typical binding profile previously identified in population with or at risk of AD dementia.
It is worth noting that our neuropsychological assessment battery included the Selective Reminding Test, which has been considered a preclinical cognitive marker for AD (
1). As we highlighted in the Introduction, these two forms of memory binding have proved dissociable in patients with AD or at risk of this type of dementia. The differential sensitivity of the forms of memory binding assessed by the VSTMBT and SRT to the transition from normal aging to AD has been recently noted (
20). The observation that VSTMB impairments were associated to increased deposits of Aβ in brain regions known to support visual object processing and memory contributes novel insights into the earliest neurocognitive changes that will likely characterize such a transition. We discuss such links next.
VSTMB appears to be linked to the functions of the visual ventral stream (
49). Cortico-cortical connections along this pathway support object unitization and identity formation. However, the occipitotemporo-medialtemporal pathway plays a key role in memory (
20,
49). This pathway consists of projections from the cortical components to various structures within the medial temporal lobe including the perirhinal cortex which projects in turn to both the entorhinal corte and to regions of the hippocampus. These regions o the anterior temporal network are thought to suppor familiarity-based recognition, a function known t support performance on change detection tasks suc as the VSTMBT. VSTMB remains preserved in patient with hippocampal damage (
9), and is affected in patient at risk of AD who still perform normally on memor tests that tax the function of the hippocampus (
10,
30,
50) This suggests that pathology during the transentorhina stage of AD, which appears prior to the hippocampa stage (
3), might be the one the VSTMBT is detecting Accrued evidence seems to support this notion. While th hippocampus undergoes substantial atrophy as we grow older, the volume of the perirhinal and entorhinal appear to be unaffected by age (
5). Interestingly, these region are targeted by AD before the hippocampus (
4,
51). This would explain why VSTMB has been consistently found to be insensitive to normal aging and sensitive to AD in its subhippocampal transentorhinal stage.
Evidence gathered to date suggests that the above sequence of neuropathological events is seemingly driven by tau pathology i.e., deposits of neuro-fibrillary tangles (NFT) in regions of the anterior network of the medial temporal lobe. In fact, a hypothetical model that maps the earliest memory impairments detectable in AD to the underlying neuropathology (
3) suggests that NFT in the sub-hippocampal stages of AD may account for the type of deficit we find with the VSTMBT (context-free memory impairments). Our data suggests that increased Aβ in the same regions of such a network can also disrupt such a memory function. Taking together the evidence above reviewed and that drawn from our own study we feel compelled to suggest that the VSTMBT appears to be indexing Aβ pathology in the very early stages of the AD continuum, seemingly before tau pathology becomes apparent. Studies using animal models have confirmed that tau is not necessary for Aβ to induce memory impairments (
52). In fact, tau pathology in humans seems to account for stages where the abnormal brain structure-function relationships become detectable. Accrued wisdom suggests that this may be too late when it comes to dementia prevention.
One may question whether the dichotomization approach used in this study is a reliable methodology to explore the association of cognition and AD pathology (both moving along a continuum) in cognitively unimpaired older adults. While dichotomization is often necessary and clinically useful, it often carries some challenges (see for example Morris (
53)) regarding the impact of cut-off scores of functional scales on MCI/AD diagnosis). Notwithstanding such challenges, Forno et al. (
21) recently confirmed that such a methodological approach allowed them to identify subtle cognitive impairments in people at risk of AD who had been otherwise undetected. In the context of the present study, we observed at a whole-group level analysis (see Supplementary Figure
1 and
2, correlations and whole-brain voxel-wise analyses) that increased Aβ in ROIs known to be nodes of the VSTMB network significantly predicted increase in the memory binding cost. It is worth noting that classical procedures to classify participants based on Aβ -PET would have faced limitations in the current sample as our cognitively unimpaired older adults were largely subthreshold (see (
6) for evidence from clinical samples). Taken together these findings suggest that our approach to dichotomize the sample should not be a factor undermining the reliability of our results.
The findings here reported come from a relatively small cross-sectional sample of healthy older adults. Future studies will be needed to validate these results in larger longitudinal samples. Lending support to this suggestion, Parra et al. (
19) recently observed that the VSTMBT is a reliable predictor of progression from normal to pathological aging, as defined by the very early stages of MCI. The authors suggested that it is at this stage when the test stands the best chance to identify those will progress to AD dementia (
20). Taken together these and our results suggest that the novel memory marker here investigated could help identify those presymptomatic older adults who are currently missed by available cognitive screening procedures.
In fact, evidence from CSF/PET Amyloid findings in asymptomatic adults suggests that preclinical and prodromal AD may be more prevalent than previously estimated (
54). This might have important implications for clinical trial recruitment strategies and for the development of normative samples. Regarding the latter, some have already suggested the need of biomarker adjusted normative data to reliably separate normal and pathological aging trajectories (
55). An alternative would be to rely on theory-driven function-specific cognitive tests capable of unveiling the earliest manifestation of AD. The VSTMBT seems to be a promising candidate. Dementia prevention entails both early detection and effective treatments, and both are currently lacking (
20). The results here presented grant us confidence to suggest that the VSTMBT can be considered a promising screening tool to help identify individuals who can be good candidates for AD prevention trials.
Conflicts of interest: Dr. Parra reports other from ViewMind, outside the submitted work. Dr. Parra work was supported by Alzheimer’s Society. Dr. Gazes and Dr. Habeck reports grants from National Institute of Aging, during the conduct of the study. Dr. Stern reports personal fees from Lilly, personal fees from Eisai, outside the submitted work; In addition, Dr. Stern has a patent Columbia University licenses the Dependence Scale, and in accordance with university policy, Dr. Stern is entitled to royalties through this license. pending.
Ethical standards: All the procedures followed in this study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Internal Review Board of the College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000.