Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Palliative Care 1/2020

Open Access 01.12.2020 | Research article

Factors underlying surrogate medical decision-making in middle eastern and east Asian women: a Q-methodology study

verfasst von: Muhammad M. Hammami, Areej Al Balkhi, Sophia S. De Padua, Kafa Abuhdeeb

Erschienen in: BMC Palliative Care | Ausgabe 1/2020

Abstract

Background

It is not clear how lay people prioritize the various, sometimes conflicting, interests when they make surrogate medical decisions, especially in non-Western cultures. The extent such decisions are perspective-related is also not well documented.

Methods

We explored the relative importance of 28 surrogate decision-making factors to 120 Middle-Eastern (ME) and 120 East-Asian (EA) women from three perspectives, norm-perception (N), preference as patient (P), and preference as surrogate decision-maker (S). Each respondent force-ranked (one to nine) 28 opinion-items according to each perspective. Items’ ranks were analyzed by averaging-analysis and Q-methodology.

Results

Respondents’ mean (SD) age was 33.2 (7.9) years; all ME were Muslims, 83% of EA were Christians. “Trying everything possible to save patient,” “Improving patient health,” “Patient pain and suffering,” and/or “What is in the best interests of patient” were the three most-important items, whereas “Effect of caring for patient on all patients in society,” “Effect of caring for patient on patients with same disease,” and/or “Cost to society from caring for patient” were among the three least-important items, in each ME and EA perspectives. P-perspective assigned higher mean ranks to family and surrogate’s needs and burdens-related items, and lower mean rank to “Fear of loss” than S-perspective (p<0.001). ME assigned higher mean ranks to “Medical facts” and “Surrogate own wishes for patient” and lower mean rank to “Family needs” in all perspectives (p<0.001). Q-methodology identified models that were relatively patient’s preference-, patient’s religious/spiritual beliefs-, or emotion-dependent (all perspectives); medical facts-dependent (N- and S-perspectives), financial needs-dependent (P- and S-perspectives), and family needs-dependent (P-perspective).

Conclusions

1) Patient’s health was more important than patient’s preference to ME and EA women; society interest was least important. 2) Family and surrogate’s needs/ burdens were more important, whereas fear of loss was less important to respondents as patients than as surrogate decision-makers. 3) Family needs were more important to EA than ME respondents, the opposite was true for medical facts and surrogate’s wishes for patient. 4) Q-methodology models that relatively emphasized various surrogate decision-making factors overlapped the ME and EA women’ three perspectives.
Hinweise

Supplementary information

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12904-020-00643-9.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
ME
Middle Eastern
EA
East Asian
N-perspective or N
Norm-perception perspective
P-perspective or P
Preference-as-patient perspective
S-perspective or S
Preference-as-surrogate-decision-maker perspective
KFSH&RC
King Faisal Specialist Hospital & Research Center
ANOVA
Analysis of variance

Background

It is not clear how lay people prioritize the various, sometimes conflicting, interests when they make surrogate medical decisions, especially in non-Western cultures. In addition, the extent such decisions are perspective-dependent is not well documented.
According to extended autonomy/precedent consent, a prevalent thesis in Western cultures, surrogate decision-makers are expected to make substituted judgments based on stated or predicted patient’s wishes [1, 2]. However, surrogates may base their decisions, at least in part, on what they think is in the patient’s best interests; what they themselves would have wanted given the circumstances; their needs, burdens, and religious/ spiritual beliefs; and/or their family needs, including maintaining family cohesion. Further, surrogates’ decisions may be emotion-driven such as by fear of loss, feeling of guilt, and desire to pursue any chance of recovery [36]. Furthermore, the patient’s own wishes may be difficult to predict because they may be circumstance-dependent and may be influenced not only by consequences to the patient but also to the surrogate, family, friends, and society. A competent patient who wishes not to be intubated may prefer that the surrogate chooses to have him/her intubated if choosing otherwise would place a burden on the surrogate. Moreover, patients may believe that such decisions are not their right, responsibility, or even important to them and thus may not perceive deviations from their preferences as infarctions of their autonomy.
There are several views that disagree with the precedent consent thesis [720]. The current preference thesis holds it that one has no moral authority to control their incompetent self and promotes patient’s current interests such as avoiding pain and enjoying simple pleasures. The patient’s life-story narrative models [915] focus on respect for persons rather than barely on autonomy and on “authentic” rather than autonomous decisions, aiming to continue the life the patient has led (authentic life model), [14, 15] to promote the life the patient has valued (endorsed life model), [14] or to integrate substituted judgment and best interests standards (substituted interests model) [13, 14]. The family autonomy/familism notion [1618] argues that the family is the essential social unit and decision-maker, that sickness or end-of-life is a sharing process since they are experienced by the entire family, [18] that the values of patient’s autonomy/beneficence in such situations are related to their importance to someone who loves the patient, [19] and that family’s moral authority is warranted by trust and needs rather than prediction accuracy [18].
End-of-life priorities [21, 22] and factors underlying surrogate decision-making may be culture-dependent; religiosity and social outlook (on liberal-conservative continuum) contributed to the variance in physicians’ attitude toward end-of-life decision-making, [23] and while 85% of the US public expressed desire to be informed if they had < 1 year to live, only 49% of Japanese did [17].
Discrepancies between patients’ perspective and surrogates’ perspective have been noted both in decisions’ content [18, 2426] and in means of decision-making [27, 28]. Such discrepancies may be related to inadequate knowledge of the patient or to projecting surrogate’s own wishes rather than simulating patient’s wishes [29, 30]. However, they may also be related to the fact that priorities’ hierarchy may be perspective-dependent and that norm-perception may differ from individual preference. Thus, one would consider the relative importance of factors influencing surrogate decision-making from the patient’s perspective, surrogate decision-maker perspective, and norm perception perspective.
Study designs that use independent rating and score-averaging do not provide insight on relative importance [31] and may obscure individual priority structures, respectively [21, 22] In Q-methodology, a forced-ranking study design followed by factor analysis, respondents model their point of view by rank-ordering opinion statements along a fixed continuum of symmetrical distribution [32]. Q-methodology allows assessment of individual priority structures as well as grouping of similar-minded individuals [33].
Using Q-methodology, the primary aim of this study was to explore the relative importance of 28 factors in surrogate decision-making to Middle Eastern and East Asian women from three perspectives, norm-perception, patient’s perspective, and surrogate decision-maker perspective.

Methods

Design

This is a cross-sectional study on the relative importance of 28 surrogate decision-making factors to Middle-Eastern (ME) and East-Asian (EA) women from three perspectives: norm perception (N-perspective), preference as patient (P-perspective), and preference as surrogate decision-maker (S-perspective). A Q-methodology instrument was used because it is especially suited to examine relative importance. Briefly, Q-methodology involves construction/collection of a concourse of opinion statements related to the topic under study, sorting the statements into groups and collapsing them into a Q-set that adequately covers the various thematic domains, presenting the Q-set to respondents to model their point of view by rank-ordering the statements into piles (Q-sort) along a continuum defined by certain instructions, performing a special type of by-person exploratory factor analysis using the Q-sorts as variables, grouping of respondents who rank-ordered the statements into similar arrangements (i.e., loaded significantly on the same factor) into models, and interpreting the models [32, 33].

Sample / setting

Eligibility criteria were having a ME or EA ancestry, age > 18 years, >high school education, and ability to understand study aim and procedures as subjectively determined by study coordinator. Volunteers were recruited through advertisement within Riyadh, Saudi Arabia. The study involved both men and women; however, because of the extent of data, only the results of women are reported here. A sample size of 120 ME and 120 EA women was based on convenience and limitation of the Q-methodology program (maximum of 120 Q-sorts).

Protection of human subjects / ethical considerations

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki after approval of the Research Ethics Committee of the King Faisal Specialist Hospital and Research Center (KFSH&RC) and obtaining respondents’ written informed consent.

Data collection

Each respondent performed Q-sorting according to each of the three perspectives (3 Q-sorts), in three random same-day Q-sorting sessions. The sessions included writing comments on the 6 extreme selections and were separated by respondents’ completion of 1) respondents’ characteristics sheet and 2) a previously published social value scale questionnaire [23].
Each of the three Q-set consisted of 28 factors that may potentially underline surrogate medical decision-making (items), 16 of which were reported previously [5, 6] and the rest were identified from literature review [3, 1315]. The 28 items are listed in Additional file 1-Q-set items together with their abbreviated version that is used in the text. The items can be divided into eight domains, patient’s preference-centric (3 items plus one item shared with patient’s life-long narrative-centric), patient’s life-long narrative-centric (4 items plus one item shared with patient’s preference-centric), patient’s health-centric (4 items plus one item shared with surrogate’s emotion-centric), surrogate’s emotion-centric (2 items plus one item shared with patient’s health-centric), surrogate’s preference-centric (3 items), surrogate’s interest-centric (2 items plus one item shared with family-centric), family-centric (4 items plus one item shared with surrogate’s interest-centric), and society-centric (3 items). The wording of the 28 items (as well as the associated instructions) in the three Q-sets were modified to fit the three perspectives addressed in the study. The Q-sets were subjected to two cycles of pilot testing, validation, and revision as previously described [34]. Q-set items were randomly numbered.
Q-sets were given to respondents with sets of instructions and sorting sheets (Additional file 2-instructions and sorting sheets). Q-sorting requires respondents to arrange the items according to their subjective relative importance. In the current study, this was from 1 = least important to 9 = most important. Numbers of slots for each category were symmetrically distributed (Additional file 2-instructions and sorting sheets); for example, there was one slot under categories 1 and 9 and 2 slots under categories 2 and 8. The time spent in Q-sorting was recorded. Sorting sheets were checked for completion (i.e., each item number is used and only once) and were collected before respondents started the next Q-sorting. In case a mistake was found in copying items’ numbers onto the sorting sheet, respondents were notified to self-correct.
Respondents’ characteristics included age, nationality, employment status, self-rated religiosity, major religious affiliation, self-rated health, daily self-care status, pain in last month, pain interference with daily activities, life quality, life satisfaction, living arrangements, death in immediate family/close friends, and personal experience with surrogate decision-making.
The social value scale [23] consisted of three subscales: planning social change (subscale-1, 3 “conservative” and 3 “liberal” statements), free choice regarding medical ethical questions (subscale-2, 4 “conservative” and 3 “liberal” statements), and deciding good vs bad regarding personal welfare (subscale-3, 3 “conservative” and 3 “liberal” statements). Respondents were asked to score each statement from (1 = total disagreement to 5 = full agreement). To analyze responses, we multiplied “liberal” statement scores by − 1 and then calculated the average score for each subscale. Thus, score of subscale-1 could range from − 2 (extreme liberal) to 2 (extreme conservative), of subscale-2 from − 1.57 (extreme liberal) to 2.43 (extreme conservative), and of subscale-3 from − 2 (extreme liberal) to 2 (extreme conservative).

Data analysis

Q-sorts were analyzed separately for ME respondents and EA respondents and for each of the three perspectives, using PCQ for Windows (PCQ Software, Portland, OR, USA). Data analysis in Q-methodology requires a special program and involves exploring Q-sorts’ correlation, factor analysis (factor loading and rotation), factor scores computation, and factor interpretation. The PCQ program identified and used Q-sorts with a loading of ≥0.49 (p < 0.01) on a single factor to create model Q-sorts, which indicate how a respondent with 100% loading on a factor would have ordered the 28 items. Model Q-sorts were interpreted based on the 3 most important (i.e., items assigned to categories 8 and 9) and 3 least important items (i.e., items assigned to categories 1 and 2), respondents post-sorting comments, and relative importance of all items within and across models. Association between models and respondents’ characteristics was studied after grouping respondents who loaded significantly on one model and respondents with confounded loading who had higher loading on the same model. For averaging-analysis, items were considered “neutral” if their mean ranking score was >4 and <6. For Q-methodology, items were considered strongly-agreeable (most important), agreeable, disagreeable, or strongly-disagreeable (least important), if they received a model Q-set rank of 8–9, 7, 3, or 1–2, respectively. Bivariate analysis was performed using chi-square or Fisher Exact tests for categorical variables and ANOVA or t-test for continuous variables (IBM SPSS Statistics version 21 software). An unadjusted two-tailed p-value is reported and considered significant if <0.01.

Results

Table 1 summarizes respondents’ characteristics. All ME respondents were Muslims and 56% were Saudis, whereas 83% of EA respondents were Christians and 94% were Filipinos. ME respondents took less time in Q-sorting with a mean (95% confidence interval) time difference of − 6.2 (− 10.3 to − 2.1) minutes (p = 0.003); had more liberal score on social value subscale-1 with a mean difference of − 0.20 (− 0.32 to − 0.08) on a scale from − 2.0 to 2.0 (p = 0.001) and more conservative score on social value subscale-2 with a mean difference of 0.32 (0.19 to 0.46) on a scale of − 1.57 to 2.43 (p<0.001); and reported better self-rated health (p = 0.01), higher life quality (p = 0.001), lower life satisfaction (p<0.001), lower employment rate (p<0.001), more recent experience with surrogate decision-making (p = 0.009), and different living arrangement (p<0.001). Differences in other characteristics were not statistically significant. Responses to individual social value scale statements are presented in Table 1S (Additional file 3-social value scale).
Table 1
Characteristics of study respondents
 
Middle Eastern (n = 120)
East Asian (n = 120)
Age-mean (SD), yr.
32.7 (9.4)
33.7 (6.0)
Nationality- no. (%)
Saudi 
67 (56)
Filipino 
113 (94)
Syrian 
23 (19)
Others 
30 (25)
7 (6)
Employment status- no. (%)
Student  
23 (19)
0 (0)
Employed 
83 (69)
119 (99)
Self employed 
3 (3)
1 (1)
Not employed 
3 (3)
0 (0)
Self-rated religiosity- no. (%)
1 (least)  
2 (2)
3 (3)
2 
6 (5)
1 (1)
3 
65 (54)
52 (43)
4 
34 (28)
47 (39)
5 (most)  
13 (11)
17 (14)
Religious affiliation no. (%)
Islam 
120 (100)
19 (16)
Christianity 
0 (0)
99 (83)
Others 
0 (0)
2 (2)
Social value subscales-mean (SD)
1.Planning social change* 
−0.33 (0.52)
−0.14 (0.40)
2.Free choice regarding medical ethical questions** 
0.64 (0.57)
0.31 (0.48)
3.Deciding good vs bad regarding personal welfare*** 
0.01(0.48)
0.04 (0.50)
Self-rated health- no. (%)
Excellent 
36 (30)
18 (15)
Very good 
52 (43)
70 (58)
Good 
27 (23)
31 (26)
Fair 
4 (3)
1 (1)
Poor 
1 (1)
0 (0)
Daily self-care-no. (%)
Without help 
118 (98)
119 (99)
With some help 
2 (2)
1 (1)
Completely unable to do any housework 
0 (0)
0 (0)
Pain in last month-no. (%)
None 
34 (28)
48 (40)
A little bit 
57 (48)
46 (38)
Moderate 
22 (18)
19 (16)
Quite a bit 
4 (3)
7 (6)
Extreme 
3 (3)
0 (0)
Pain interference with daily activities-no. (%)
Not at all 
71 (59)
79 (66)
A little bit 
34 (28)
24 (20)
Moderately 
10 (8)
13 (11)
Quite a bit 
4 (3)
4 (3)
Extremely 
1 (1)
0 (0)
Current Quality of life-no. (%)
Excellent 
39 (33)
18 (15)
Very good 
56 (47)
81 (68)
Good 
25 (21)
19 (16)
Fair 
0 (0)
2 (2)
“If I could live my life over, I would change almost nothing.”-no. (%)
Strongly agree 
12 (10)
22 (18)
Agree 
25 (21)
56 (47)
Neither agree nor disagree 
23 (19)
31 (26)
Disagree 
33 (28)
10 (8)
Strongly disagree 
27 (23)
1 (1)
Living arrangements-no. (%)
With spouse 
46 (38)
54 (45)
With parent 
60 (50)
13 (11)
With child 
6 (5)
2 (2)
With other family members 
7 (6)
8 (7)
Alone 
1 (1)
43 (36)
Death in immediate family/close friends-no. (%)
Last year 
24 (20)
26 (22)
2–5 years ago 
55 (46)
46 (38)
None in last 5 years 
41 (34)
48 (40)
Personal experience with surrogate decision-making-no. (%)
Last year 
34 (28)
14 (12)
2–5 years ago 
21 (18)
21 (18)
Six or more years ago 
4 (3)
7 (6)
None 
61 (51)
78 (65)
Sorting time-mean (SD), min.
67.2 (14.5)
73.4 (17.6)
Percentages refer to number of responses and may not add to 100% due to rounding. *Subscale-1 ranges from −2.0 (most liberal) to 2.0 (most conservative). **Subscale-2 ranges from −1.57 (most liberal) to 2.43 (most conservative). ***Subcale-3 ranges from − 2.0 (most liberal) to 2.0 (most conservative). Responses to individual social value scale statements are presented in Table 1S under Additional file 3-social value scale

Averaging-analysis

Figure 1 shows mean ranking scores of the 28 items by ME and EA respondents from the N-perspective, P-perspective, and S-perspective. The items are grouped according to the underlying domains.
Overall, patient’s health was more important than other factors, including patient’s preference and life-long narrative; surrogate’s emotion, preference, and interest; family interests; and society interests. For ME respondents, of the items that received a mean ranking score >6, 5/5, 5/5, and 5/6 were related to patient’s health in the N-perspective, P-perspective, and S-perspective, respectively. For EA respondents, the corresponding ratios were 4/4, 3/4, and 3/5. The other items that received a mean score >6 were “26.Fear of loss of loved one” in ME and EA S-perspective and “9.Family needs” in EA P-perspective and S-perspective.
Notably: 1) In the patient’s preference domain, patients’ wishes were given about the same weights regardless of patients’ mental status. 2) In the patient’s life-long narrative domain, the weight given to “18.Life-long story of patient” was lower than the weight given to the other four related items, suggesting that it may have not been understood correctly by respondents. 3) In the patient’s health domain, “3.Trying everything possible to save patient” received more weight than “22.Medical facts.” 4) In the surrogate’s preference domain, there were no appreciable differences between the weights given to what surrogates want for themselves or wish for patients.

Comparing the three perspectives

There were significant (p<0.001 to p = 0.006) differences among the N-, P-, and S-perspectives in mean scores of several items, when ME and EA respondents were analyzed together, ME respondents analyzed alone, or EA respondents analyzed alone (Table 2). Of note: 1) Surrogate’s and family’s needs and burdens were given more weight in the P-perspective than the S-perspective. 2) Fear of loss, feeling of guilt, and patient’s interests were given more weight in the S-perspective than the P-perspective. 3) There was dissociation between the N-perspective and S-perspective in relation to importance of surrogate’s preference and family cohesion (S>N), and patient’s best interests (N>S).
Table 2
Surrogate decision-making items with significant differences in ranking scores among three perspectives
Item
All
Only ME
Only EA
“28.What is in the best interests of patient”
N>S>P
 
N>S>P
“26.Fear of loss of loved one”
S>N & P
S>P & N
S>N & P
“25.Feeling of guilt because not trying everything possible”
S & N>P
S & N>P
 
“8.What surrogate would have wanted if in similar situation”
S>N
S>N & P
 
“10.Surrogate own wishes for patient”
S>N
S>P & N
 
“14.Surrogate burden due to taking care of patient or disliked outcome”
P>N & S
P>N
P>N>S
“23.Surrogate needs”
P>N>S
P & N>S
P & N>S
“9.Family needs”
P>S>N
P>S & N
P & S>N
“20.Reaching family agreement to maintain family cohesion”
S & P>N
P & S>N
 
“27.Family burden due to taking care of patient or disliked outcome”
P>N & S
P>S
P>N & S
Items’ ranks were compared among the N, P, and S perspectives (norm perception, patient, and surrogate decision maker perspectives, respectively) in all respondents (All), only Middle Eastern (ME) respondents, and only East Asian (EA) respondents. Items with significant (p < 0.01) rank differences are shown, grouped according to the underlying domain. P values ranged from 0.006 to < 0.001

Comparing ME to EA women

Similarly, there were significant (p<0.001 to p = 0.008) differences between ME and EA respondents in mean ranking scores of several items (Table 3). In all three perspective, ME respondents were more influenced by patient’s health and less influenced by family needs. Further, ME respondents gave more weight to patient’s past wishes in the P-perspective. Furthermore, there was differential emphasis on the various aspects of the patient’s life-long narrative; ME respondents gave higher score to the life the patient has led (N-perspective) and lower score to the life the patient has valued (N-perspective and S-perspective) and religious or spiritual beliefs of patient (N-perspective). Finally, EA respondents gave more weight to society interests in the P-perspective and S-perspective.
Table 3
Surrogate decision-making items with significant differences in ranking scores between ME and EA women
Items
N-perspective
P-perspective
S-perspective
 
ME
EA
ME
EA
ME
EA
“15.What patient would have wanted based on past wishes”
  
x
 
x
 
“13.Religious or spiritual beliefs of patient”
 
x
    
“4.What promotes the life patient has valued”
 
x
   
x
“2.What continues the life patient has led”
x
     
“28.What is in the best interests of patient”
  
x
 
x
 
“12.Patient pain and suffering”
x
 
x
   
“22.Medical facts”
x
 
x
 
x
 
“3.Trying everything possible to save patient”
x
     
“8.What surrogate would have wanted if in similar situation”
    
x
 
“10.Surrogate own wishes for patient”
x
 
x
 
x
 
“14.Surrogate burden due to taking care of patient or disliked outcome”
    
x
 
“23.Surrogate needs”
 
x
 
x
  
“17.Financial concerns”
   
x
 
x
“9.Family needs”
 
x
 
x
 
x
“20.Reaching family agreement to maintain family cohesion”
 
x
   
x
“1.Effect of caring for patient on patients with same disease”
     
x
“5.Effect of caring for patient on all patients in society”
   
x
 
x
Items’ ranks were compared between Middle Eastern (ME) and East Asian (EA) women per perspective. N-, P-, and S- perspectives indicate norm perception, patient, and surrogate decision-maker perspectives, respectively. Items with significant (p < 0.01) rank differences are shown, grouped according to the underlying domain. P values ranged from 0.008 to < 0.001. “x” indicates the group with higher mean rank

Q-methodology analysis

Using a Q-methodology specific program (PCQ program), we extracted 6 factors for each of the three perspectives of ME and EA respondents. The program calculates idealized item scores (model Q-sort) for each factor. A model Q-sort indicates how a respondent with 100% loading on a factor would have ordered the 28 items. Factor characteristics and item scores are presented in Tables 2S to 7S (Additional file 4-factor characteristics and item scores).
The final step in Q-methodology is to interpret and name program-identified model Q-sorts and explore their association with respondents’ characteristics. Tables 4 and 5, list model names in each perspective of ME and EA respondents, respectively. Details underlying factors interpretation are presented in Additional file 5-Q-methodology technical report. The following provides an overall description of models in each perspective.
Table 4
Surrogate medical decision-making models in Middle Eastern women
Norm-perception (N-perspective) models
Model A: Surrogate’s burden-independent
Model B: Surrogate-independent
Model C: Emotion, patient’s life-long narrative, and family needs-independent
Model D: Patient’s endorsed-life narrative and Golden Rule-independent
Model E: Patient’s preference-dependent
Model F: Society’s interests-independent
Patient’s perspective (P-perspective) models
Model A: Emotion and patient’s life-long narrative-independent
Model B: Cost and society’s interests-independent
Model C: Emotion-dependent and society’s interests-independent
Model D: Religious/spiritual beliefs-independent and family needs-dependent
Model E: Emotion-independent and family needs-dependent
Model F: Patient’s preference-dependent
Surrogate’s perspective (S-perspective) models
Model A: Patient’s preference-dependent
Model B: Emotion and religious/spiritual beliefs-dependent
Model C: Society’s interests and religious/spiritual beliefs-independent
Model D: Patient’s life-long narrative-independent
Model E: Medical facts-dependent and religious/spiritual beliefs-independent
Model F: Society’s interests-independent
Models interpretation details are presented in Additional file 5 Q-methodology technical report
Table 5
Surrogate medical decision-making models in East Asian women
Norm-perception (N-perspective) models
Model A: Society’s interest and surrogate-independent
Model B: Religious/spiritual beliefs and surrogate-independent and emotion-dependent
Model C: Medical facts-dependent and patient’s preference independent
Model D: Financial needs and society’s interests-independent
Model E: Patient’s preference-dependent and emotion-independent
Model F: Patient’s religious/spiritual beliefs-dependent
Patient’s perspective (P-perspective) models
Model A: Society’s interests-independent
Model B: Religious/spiritual beliefs-independent
Model C: Financial needs-dependent and emotion-independent
Model D: Emotion-independent and family needs-dependent
Model E: Patient’s preference-independent and patient’s religious/spiritual beliefs-dependent
Model F: Patient’s preference-independent
Surrogate’s perspective (S-perspective) models
Model A: Religious/spiritual beliefs and financial needs-independent
Model B: Society’s interest-independent
Model C: Family burden-independent and religious/spiritual beliefs-dependent.
Model D: Surrogate’s preference-independent
Model E: Financial needs-dependent and patient’s authentic-life narrative-independent
Model F: Society’s interests-independent and religious/spiritual beliefs-dependent
Models interpretation details are presented in Additional file 5 Q-methodology technical report

Norm-perception models of ME women

There were three consensus items among the six models in this perspective, and all were related to patient’s health: “3.Trying everything possible to save patient,” “21.Improving patient health,” and “28.What is in the best interests of patient” (ranked 8 or 9, 7 or 8, and 7 or 8, respectively). However, ranks for “22.Medical facts” ranged from 6 to 9. Further, ranks of the six items with a mean score <4 on averaging-analysis varied from 1 to 6 for “7.Surrogate own religious or spiritual beliefs,” from 1 to 5 for “23.Surrogate needs,” from 2 to 6 for “9.Family needs,” from 1 to 5 for “18.Life-long story of patient,” from 1 to 7 for “16.Cost to society from caring for patient,” and from 3 to 5 for “5.Effect of caring for patient on all patients in society.” The results indicate more heterogeneity in what is considered unimportant compared to what is considered important.
Despite the overall emphasis on patient’s health, there was a relatively patient’s preference-dependent model (Table 4). We found no significant associations between respondent characteristics and loading on a given model in this perspective.

Patient’s perspective models of ME women

In this perspective, none of the patient’s health items received a consensus status. In fact, ranks for “28.What is in the best interests of patient” and “12.Patient pain and suffering” varied from 6 to 9. On the other hand, “1.Effects of caring for patient on patients with same disease” was a consensus item (ranked 3 or 4).
This perspective was notable for having two relatively family needs-dependent models, a relatively patient’s preference-dependent model, and a relatively emotion-dependent model (Table 4). Further, model loading was associated with age (p = 0.001), employment status (p = 0.006), and living arrangements (p<0.001). Models A and B respondents were older than models D and C respondents (mean (SD) age 38.9 (10.0), 36.4 (11.4), 25.9 (6.9), and 25.8 (3.5) years, respectively). Further, model C (unique in this perspective in being relatively emotion-dependent) was composed mainly of students (56% vs 0 to 22% in other models). In addition,
86% of model A (unique in being relatively patient’s life-long narrative-independent) respondents lived with a spouse (vs 0–33% in other models) and 89% of model D (unique in being relatively religious/spiritual beliefs-independent) respondents lived with a parent (vs 14–75% in other models).

Surrogate decision-maker perspective models of ME women

In this perspective, “1.Effects of caring for patient on patients with same disease” was a consensus item with low ranks (ranked 3 or 4), “8.What surrogate would have wanted if in similar situation” was a consensus item with intermediate ranks (ranked 5 or 6), and “21.Improving patient health” was a consensus item with high/very high ranks (ranked 7 or 8).
On the other hand, ranks of several items varied considerably; from 5 to 8 for “22.Medical facts,” from 4 to 9 for “26.Fear of loss of loved one,” from 2 to 7 for “17.Financial concerns” and “9.Family needs,” and from 1 to 5 for “18.Life-long story of patient.”
This perspective included a relatively emotion and religious/spiritual beliefs-dependent model, a relatively medical-facts-dependent model, and a relatively patient’s preference-dependent model (Table 4). Association with model loading was restricted to social value subscales-1 and subscale-2 scores and was of borderline significance (p = 0.02). Model A (unique in being relatively patient’s preference-dependent) had “more liberal” mean score than the relatively society’s interests-independent model F on subscale-1 (− 0.70 (0.69) vs − 0.14 (0.59) and “less conservative” mean score on subscale-1 (0.27 (0.71) vs 0.91 (0.61) on subscale-2.

Norm-perception models of EA women

“21.Improving patient health” was a consensus item (ranked 8 or 9) in this perspective. However, ranks of the other patient’s health related items varied from 5 to 9 for “28.What is in the best interests of patient” and from 6 to 9 for “3.Trying everything possible to save patient” and “12.Patient pain and suffering.” This perspective included relatively emotion-dependent, medical facts-dependent, patient’s preference-dependent, and patient’s religious/spiritual beliefs-dependent models (Table 5). Similar to the case in ME women N-perspective, there was no significant association between respondent characteristics and model loading.

Patient’s perspective models of EA women

In this perspective, “9.Family needs” was one of the four items with a mean score of >6 on averaging-analysis and was ranked 6 to 8. The other 3 items were again related to patient’s health. “21.Improving patient’s health” was a consensus item (ranked 8 or 9); however, ranks varied from 3 to 8 for “12.Patient’s pain and suffering” and from 5 to 9 for “3.Trying everything possible to save the patient.”
Notably, this perspective included relatively financial needs-dependent, family needs-dependent, and patient’s religious/spiritual beliefs-dependent models (Table 5). Further, there was significant (p = 0.007) association between model loading and social value subscale-3 scores. Model D (unique in being family needs-dependent) had “more conservative” mean score than models B and E (0.48 (0.61) vs − 0.14 (0.19) and − 0.11 (0.38), respectively). Model B was relatively religious/spiritual beliefs-independent, whereas model E was relatively patient’s religious/spiritual beliefs-dependent.

Surrogate decision-maker perspective models of EA women

In this perspective, “21.Improving patient health” was a consensus item (ranked 8 or 9). However, ranks of several items varied widely, including from 5 to 9 for “26.Fear of loss of loved one,” from 6 to 9 for “3.Trying everything possible to save patient” and “12.Patient pain and suffering,” from 1 to 6 for “16.Cost to society from caring for patient,” from 2 to 5 for “14.Surrogate burden due to taking care of patient or disliked outcome.”
There were two relatively religious/spiritual beliefs-dependent models and a relatively financial needs-dependent model in this perspective (Table 5). There was borderline significant (p = 0.04) association between factor loading and social value subscale-3 scores. Model E (unique in being relatively patient’s authentic-life narrative-independent) had more “liberal” score on subscale-3 than the relatively society’s interest-independent model B (− 0.13 (0.37) vs 0.30 (0.55), respectively).

Discussion

The primary aim of this study was to explore the relative importance of 28 factors in surrogate medical decision-making by ME and EA women. The factors covered the following areas: patient’s preference, life-long narrative, and health; surrogate’s emotion, preference, and interests; family interests; and society interests. Three perspectives were examined, norm-perception, patient’s perspective, and surrogate decision-maker perspective. Our main findings were: 1) Patient’s health was more important than patient’s preference to both ME and EA women in all three perspectives. 2) Respondents put more weight on family and surrogate’s needs/ burdens and less weight on fear of loss as patients than as surrogate decision-makers. 3) Family needs were more important to EA than ME women; the opposite was true for medical facts and surrogate’s wishes for patient. 4) ME and EA women could be assigned to heterogeneous but overlapping Q-methodology models that were associated with a few of respondents’ characteristics.

Importance of patient’s health

According to the substituted judgment standard, surrogate decisions should be based on patient’s stated or predicted preferences rather than what the surrogate thinks is in the patient’s best interests, which is supported by the results of a national survey of US physicians [2]. However, in the current study, patient’s health was more important than other studied factors, including patient’s preference, for both ME and EA respondents and from all the three perspectives. Consistently, a previous study using simple rating and dichotomization showed that 78% of surrogates (mostly white educated US women) focused more on patient’s well-being than patient’s preferences [6]. A predominance of consequentialist attitude over autonomy-based attitude was also observed in lay people approach to medical use of placebos [34] and organ donation, [35] providing support to the concept that “good” may be more fundamental than “right” and to the importance of “harm/care” as one of the psychological foundations of morality [36].
On the other hand, placing more weight on patient’s health than patient’s preference may be emotion-driven. Indeed, for our respondents, trying everything possible to save patient was more important than other patient’s health-related items such as medical facts and patient’s best interests (Figure). Substituted judgement is known to be associated with emotional demands, [18] and about a third of surrogates had negative emotions lasting several months [37]. Alternatively, the emphasis on patient’s health over patient’s preference may be related to beliefs of life sacredness.

Patient’s preference and life-long narrative

Overall, patient’s preference followed patient’s health in importance. Remarkably, current wishes (despite impaired mentation) were given similar weights to past wishes in most perspectives (Figure). The concept of prospective autonomy/ autonomy extension argues that desiring is only a basic notion, whereas valuing is a reflective appraisal that should be respected when previous and current desires conflict, [8] that autonomy is a right rather than necessarily a reflective capacity, that legitimate interests include not only experiential but also critical interests (such as interests in fulfilling religious commitments and interests in family welfare after death), and that personal identity/personhood is based on bodily continuity rather than psychological connectedness or continuity [7, 8]. In contrast, the current preference thesis takes into account only experiential interests, believes that preferences and interests do not survive loss of mental capacity, considers autonomy as reflective capacity/responsibility, and concludes that there is no moral authority for exercising control over one’s incompetent self and that one should instead promote the current interests of patients [7, 8]. The current preference thesis could provide yet another explanation for our respondents’ emphasis on patient’s health rather than patient’s preference.
Several of the items related to patient’s life-long narrative received similar weights as items related to patient’s preference. According to the narrative view, narratively well-structured lives have an aesthetic value, and life is to be concluded in a way that is best consistent with life-long themes [11, 14]. This view focuses on respect for persons rather than narrowly on respect for autonomy and on authentic decisions rather than autonomous decisions. In addition to taking into consideration patient’s prior wishes, it considers patient’s life-long dispositions, relationships, decisions, decision-making processes, and interests, balancing rather than prioritizing [9]. There are three closely-related versions of the life-story narrative model, authentic life, [10, 15] substituted interests, [13] and patient’s endorsed life [14]. The patient’s life-story is taken as evidence of what the patient would have wanted to continue, [15] of what the patient’s interests should be based upon, [13] or of what the patient have valued, respectively [14].
In most perspectives, our respondents put less weight on what continues the life the patient has led (authentic life) than on what promotes the life the patient has valued (endorsed life) or the way the patient used to make decisions (Figure). However, Q-methodology did identify both a relatively patient’s endorsed-life narrative-independent model (ME N-perspective) and a relatively patient’s authentic-life narrative-independent model (EA S-perspective). Finally, the fact that, in general, the importance of patient’s preference followed the importance of patient’s health appears to be most consistent with the substituted interests’ model, which integrates the substituted judgment and the best interests’ standards [13, 14]. Of note, “Life-long story of patient” was assigned much lower weight than the other items under the life-long narrative domain, suggesting that this item may have not been understood by our respondents as intended.

The Golden rule

Consistent with the Golden Rule, there were no overall appreciable differences between the importance of “What surrogate would have wanted if in similar situation” and the importance of “Surrogate own wishes for patient.” It has been argued that the Golden Rule, a fundamental concept of Judo-Christian and Islamic ethics, should be considered as a more practical alternative to substituted judgment [5]. Interestingly, the Golden Rule was more important to ME than EA respondents, ME respondents put significantly more weight on surrogate’s wishes for the patient in the N-, P-, and S-perspectives and more weight on what the surrogate would have done in a similar situation in the S-perspective. Finally, the observation that in the ME S-perspective, these two items were as important as items related to patient’s preference provides support to previous results showing that surrogates may more project their wishes than simulate patient’s wishes [29].

Differences among the three perspectives

In the current study, surrogate/family needs and burdens were given more weight in the P-perspective than in the S-perspective. Similarly, in a previous study, burden on family was the third most important end-of-life issue for patients but was not important for surrogates [38]. Several studies [26, 27, 29, 30, 38] and reviews [24, 25] have found that substituted judgment accuracy is low to moderate despite the fact that surrogates are more accurate than physicians in predicting patients’ preferences [25, 39]. Although this may be caused by inadequate knowledge of patient’s wishes and priorities, [29, 30] the current results suggest that it may be also role-dependent. This is supported by the fact that patient’ best interests and emotions (fear of loss of loved and feeling of guilt) were given more weight in the S-perspective than in the P-perspective.
There was also notable dissociation between the N-perspective and S-perspective. In the N-perspective, respondents put less weight on surrogate’s preference and family cohesion and more weight on patient’s best interest. Previous studies showed significant divergence between preference and perception of norm in regard to consenting for research on medical records and leftover tissue samples [40] but not in regard to disclosure of medical errors [41] or organ donation [42]. One can differentiate two kinds of norm perception. Perception of what is commonly approved/ disapproved (injunctive norm) and perception of what is commonly done (descriptive norm). Our instrument was designed to explore the perception of injunctive norm. The observed dissociation between the N- and S-perspectives suggests that social desirability bias did not have a substantial influence on our respondents.

Differences between ME to EA respondents

End-of-life preferences may be culture and sex [21, 22, 43] as well as religiosity [23] dependent. We observed two notable differences between ME and EA respondents. First, EA respondents were more family-centric, whereas ME respondents were more patient-centric. In a previous study on attitude to organ donation, there was also stronger familism orientation in Christians/Philippines-educated than Muslims/Saudi Arabia-educated respondents. In familism, which is prevalent in East Asia, [16, 17, 44] the family makes decisions collectively based on the family’s interest [5]. Interestingly, familism orientation may be role-dependent. In our study, family needs and burden were given more weight in the P-perspective compared to the S-perspective. Similarly, a previous study showed that surrogates err more by attributing to the patient a preference for substituted judgment when the patient prefers patient-family shared decision-making or family-only decision-making than the reverse [27].
Second, in the N-perspective, ME respondents put significantly more weight on “What continues the life patient has led” and less weight on “What promotes the life patient has valued” and “Religious or spiritual believes of patient,” suggesting a differential subscription to the authentic- life narrative and endorsed-life narrative models.

Q-methodology-based surrogate’s decision-making models

Q-methodology, a special type of by-person exploratory factor analysis, produces grouping of respondents into thinking models [32, 33]. Comparing ME and EA models, we found substantial overlap but also notable differences. For example, in the N-perspective, relatively surrogate-independent, society’s interests-independent, and emotion-independent models were found in both groups; patient’s life-long narrative-independent, patient’s endorsed-life narrative-independent, family needs-independent, and Golden rule-independent models were found only among ME respondents; and religious/spiritual beliefs-independent and patient’s preference-independent models were found only among EA respondents. Similarly, in the N-perspective, there were relatively patient’s preference-dependent models in both groups; and emotion-dependent, medical facts-dependent, and patient’s religious/spiritual beliefs-depended models only among EA respondents.
Intriguingly, there was no association between Q-methodology models and characteristics of ME or EA respondents in the N-perspective. However, in the ME P-perspective, respondents who loaded on a relatively emotion-dependent model were younger and predominantly students; in the EA P-perspective, the relatively family needs-dependent model had the most “conservative” score while the relatively religious/spiritual beliefs-independent model had the most “liberal” score on social value subscale-3 (deciding good vs bad regarding personal welfare); in the ME S-perspective, the relatively patient’s preference-dependent model had the most “liberal” score and the relatively society’s interests-independent model had the most “conservative” score on social value subscale-1 (planning social change) and subscale-2 (free choice regarding medical ethical questions); and in the EA S-perspective, the relatively society’s interests-independent model had the most “conservative” scores on social value subscale-3.

Study potential implications

The results suggest that guidelines for surrogate decision-making that are based solely on right of self-determination may, in general, poorly fit surrogate decision-making in Middle Eastern and East Asian cultures. However, there is no one-size-fits-all solution as patient’s preference-dependent models were identified in both groups. Further, the discrepancy between how one prefers to make surrogate decisions and how one prefers surrogate decisions be made on their behalf should be considered in clinical practice and in designing and interpreting related research studies. Finally, the results highlight the relative importance of familism orientation in surrogate decision-making, especially among East Asian women. It is suggested that clinicians develop measures for eliciting both patients’ and surrogates’ perspectives to include them into the care and education of patients at risk for requiring surrogate decision-making and their potential surrogates.

Study strengths and limitations

The study was unique in examining an expanded collection of potential factors that may underlie surrogate medical decision-making, in enrolling Middle Eastern and East Asian women, and in analyzing data by both averaging-analysis and Q-methodology. The use of shuffled items and forced-ranking permitted minimizing biases related to order effect and tendency to assign maximum importance to numerous items, respectively. The study design also allowed direct comparison of three perspectives, norm-perception, preference as patient, and preference as surrogate decision-maker. Finally, the three Q-sorts were performed in a random order and separated by completion of respondents’ characteristics and social value scale questionnaire to minimize potential carry-over effect.
The study has several limitations. First, generalizability of the results is limited due to convenience sampling, limiting enrollment to educated individuals, and limiting analysis to women respondents. Second, it is possible that some respondents may have not understood some items as intended (for example, “Life-long story of patient”). Third, it is likely that there are more than the models identified in the study, which is expected because only six factors were extracted and is consistent with the exploratory and none exhaustive nature of Q-methodology. Fourth, given the impressionistic nature of Q-methodology, there was overlap among the models in each perspective.

Conclusions

We found that in surrogate medical decision-making: 1) Patient’s health was more important than patient’s preference to both Middle Eastern and East Asian women from all three perspectives, norm-perception, preference as patient, and preference as surrogate decision-maker. This is more in line with the substituted interests than the substituted judgment concept. Society interest was least important. 2) Respondents put more weight on family and surrogate’s needs and burdens and less weight on fear of loss as patients than as surrogate decision-makers. 3) Family needs were more important to East Asian than Middle Eastern women; the opposite was true for medical facts and surrogate’s wishes for patient. 4) Middle Eastern and East Asian women could be assigned to heterogeneous but overlapping Q-methodology models that were associated with some respondents’ characteristics and included, relatively patient’s preference-, religious/spiritual beliefs-, emotion-, family needs-, and financial needs- dependent or independent models; patient’s life-long narrative-, surrogate-, Golden Rule-, and society’s interests-independent models; and medical facts-dependent model.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12904-020-00643-9.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.
Not applicable.
The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki after approval of the Research Ethics Committee of the King Faisal Specialist Hospital and Research Center. All respondents provided written informed consent.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Emanuel EJ, Emanuel LL. Proxy decision making for incompetent patients: an ethical and empirical analysis. JAMA. 1992;267:2067–71.CrossRef Emanuel EJ, Emanuel LL. Proxy decision making for incompetent patients: an ethical and empirical analysis. JAMA. 1992;267:2067–71.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Combs MP, Rasinski KA, Yoon JD, Curlin FA. Substituted judgment in principle and practice: a national physician survey. Mayo Clin Proc. 2013;88(7):666–73.CrossRef Combs MP, Rasinski KA, Yoon JD, Curlin FA. Substituted judgment in principle and practice: a national physician survey. Mayo Clin Proc. 2013;88(7):666–73.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Schenker Y, Crowley-Matoka M, Dohan D, Tiver GA, Arnold RM, White DB. I don’t want to be the one saying ‘we should just let him die’: intrapersonal tensions experienced by surrogate decision makers in the ICU. J Gen Intern Med. 2012;27(12):1657–65.CrossRef Schenker Y, Crowley-Matoka M, Dohan D, Tiver GA, Arnold RM, White DB. I don’t want to be the one saying ‘we should just let him die’: intrapersonal tensions experienced by surrogate decision makers in the ICU. J Gen Intern Med. 2012;27(12):1657–65.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Pruchno RA, Lemay EP Jr, Feild L, Levinsky NG. Spouse as health care proxy for dialysis patients: whose preferences matter? Gerontologist. 2005;45(6):812–9.CrossRef Pruchno RA, Lemay EP Jr, Feild L, Levinsky NG. Spouse as health care proxy for dialysis patients: whose preferences matter? Gerontologist. 2005;45(6):812–9.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Fritch J, Petronio S, Helft PR, Torke A. Making decisions for hospitalized older adults: ethical factors considered by family surrogates. J Clin Ethics. 2013;24(2):125–34.PubMedCentral Fritch J, Petronio S, Helft PR, Torke A. Making decisions for hospitalized older adults: ethical factors considered by family surrogates. J Clin Ethics. 2013;24(2):125–34.PubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Devnani R, Slaven JE Jr, Bosslet GT, Montz K, Inger L, Burke ES, Torke AM. How surrogates decide: a secondary data analysis of decision-making principles used by the surrogates of hospitalized adults. J Gen Intern Med. 2017;32(12):1285–93.CrossRef Devnani R, Slaven JE Jr, Bosslet GT, Montz K, Inger L, Burke ES, Torke AM. How surrogates decide: a secondary data analysis of decision-making principles used by the surrogates of hospitalized adults. J Gen Intern Med. 2017;32(12):1285–93.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Tsinorema S. The principle of autonomy and the ethics of advance directives. Synthesis Philosoph. 2015;59:73–88. Tsinorema S. The principle of autonomy and the ethics of advance directives. Synthesis Philosoph. 2015;59:73–88.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Davis JK. Precedent autonomy and subsequent consent. Ethical Theory Moral Pract. 2004;7:267–91.CrossRef Davis JK. Precedent autonomy and subsequent consent. Ethical Theory Moral Pract. 2004;7:267–91.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Torke AM, Alexander GC, Lantos J. Substituted judgment: the limitations of autonomy in surrogate decision making. J Gen Inten Med. 2008;23(9):1514–7.CrossRef Torke AM, Alexander GC, Lantos J. Substituted judgment: the limitations of autonomy in surrogate decision making. J Gen Inten Med. 2008;23(9):1514–7.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Sussman B. A narrative approach to the ethical dilemmas of surrogate decision making. Prog Palliat Care. 2015;3:137–41.CrossRef Sussman B. A narrative approach to the ethical dilemmas of surrogate decision making. Prog Palliat Care. 2015;3:137–41.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Blustein J. Choosing for others as continuing a life story: the problem of personal identity revisited. J Law Med Ethics. 1999;27:20–31.CrossRef Blustein J. Choosing for others as continuing a life story: the problem of personal identity revisited. J Law Med Ethics. 1999;27:20–31.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuczewski MG. Commentary: narrative views of personal identity and substituted judgment in surrogate decision making. J Law Med Ethics. 1999;27:32–6.CrossRef Kuczewski MG. Commentary: narrative views of personal identity and substituted judgment in surrogate decision making. J Law Med Ethics. 1999;27:32–6.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Sulmasy DP, Snyder L. Substituted interests and best judgments. An integrated model of surrogate decision making. JAMA. 2010;304(17):1946–9.CrossRef Sulmasy DP, Snyder L. Substituted interests and best judgments. An integrated model of surrogate decision making. JAMA. 2010;304(17):1946–9.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Phillips J, Wendler D. Clarifying substituted judgment: the endorsed life approach. J Med Ethics. 2015;41:723–30.CrossRef Phillips J, Wendler D. Clarifying substituted judgment: the endorsed life approach. J Med Ethics. 2015;41:723–30.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Brudney D. Choosing for another: beyond autonomy and best interests. Hast Cent Rep. 2009;39(2):31–7.CrossRef Brudney D. Choosing for another: beyond autonomy and best interests. Hast Cent Rep. 2009;39(2):31–7.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Chan HM. Sharing death and dying: advance directives, autonomy and the family. Bioethics. 2004;18(2):87–103.CrossRef Chan HM. Sharing death and dying: advance directives, autonomy and the family. Bioethics. 2004;18(2):87–103.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Nagai H. Blood ties and trust: a comparative history of policy on family consent in Japan and the United States. Monash Bioeth Rev. 2017;34:226–38.CrossRef Nagai H. Blood ties and trust: a comparative history of policy on family consent in Japan and the United States. Monash Bioeth Rev. 2017;34:226–38.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Meeker MA. Family surrogate decision making at the end of life: seeing them through with care and respect. Qual Health Res. 2004;14(2):204–25.CrossRef Meeker MA. Family surrogate decision making at the end of life: seeing them through with care and respect. Qual Health Res. 2004;14(2):204–25.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Stonestreet EL. Love as a regulative ideal in surrogate decision making. J Med Philos. 2014;39(5):523–42.CrossRef Stonestreet EL. Love as a regulative ideal in surrogate decision making. J Med Philos. 2014;39(5):523–42.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Shalowitz DI, Garrett-Mayer E, Wendler D. How should treatment decisions be made for incapacitated patients, and why? PLoS Med. 2007;4(3):0423–8.CrossRef Shalowitz DI, Garrett-Mayer E, Wendler D. How should treatment decisions be made for incapacitated patients, and why? PLoS Med. 2007;4(3):0423–8.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Hammami MM, Al Gaai E, Hammami S, Attala S. Exploring end of life priorities in Saudi males: usefulness of Q-methodology. BMC Palliat Care. 2015;14:66.CrossRef Hammami MM, Al Gaai E, Hammami S, Attala S. Exploring end of life priorities in Saudi males: usefulness of Q-methodology. BMC Palliat Care. 2015;14:66.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Hammami MM, Hammami S, Amer H, Khodr NA. Typology of end-of-life priorities in Saudi females: averaging analysis and Q-methodology. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2016;10:781–94.CrossRef Hammami MM, Hammami S, Amer H, Khodr NA. Typology of end-of-life priorities in Saudi females: averaging analysis and Q-methodology. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2016;10:781–94.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen CJ, Chen Y, Orbach H, Freier-Dror Y, Auslander G, Breuer GS. Social values as an independent factor affecting end of life medical decision making. Med Health Care Philos. 2015;18(1):71–80.CrossRef Cohen CJ, Chen Y, Orbach H, Freier-Dror Y, Auslander G, Breuer GS. Social values as an independent factor affecting end of life medical decision making. Med Health Care Philos. 2015;18(1):71–80.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Meeker MA, Jezewski MA. Family decision making at end of life. Palliat Support Care. 2005;3(2):131–42.CrossRef Meeker MA, Jezewski MA. Family decision making at end of life. Palliat Support Care. 2005;3(2):131–42.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Shalowitz DI, Garrett-Mayer E, Wendler D. The accuracy of surrogate decision makers. A systematic review. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166:493–7.CrossRef Shalowitz DI, Garrett-Mayer E, Wendler D. The accuracy of surrogate decision makers. A systematic review. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166:493–7.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Song MK, Ward SE, Lin FC. End-of-life decision-making confidence in surrogates of African-American dialysis patient is overly optimistic. J Palliat Med. 2012;15(4):412–7.CrossRef Song MK, Ward SE, Lin FC. End-of-life decision-making confidence in surrogates of African-American dialysis patient is overly optimistic. J Palliat Med. 2012;15(4):412–7.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Sharma RK, Hughes MT, Nolan MT, Tudor C, Kub J, Terry PB, Sulmasy DP. Family understanding of seriously-ill patient preferences for family involvement in healthcare decision making. J Gen Intern Med. 2011;26(8):881–6.CrossRef Sharma RK, Hughes MT, Nolan MT, Tudor C, Kub J, Terry PB, Sulmasy DP. Family understanding of seriously-ill patient preferences for family involvement in healthcare decision making. J Gen Intern Med. 2011;26(8):881–6.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Mayer PA, Esplin B, Burant CJ, Wilson BM, Krall ML, Daly BJ, Gatliff J. Characteristics of completed comprehensive advance directives at a veterans affairs medical center. A J Hosp Palliat Care. 2017;34(2):160–5.CrossRef Mayer PA, Esplin B, Burant CJ, Wilson BM, Krall ML, Daly BJ, Gatliff J. Characteristics of completed comprehensive advance directives at a veterans affairs medical center. A J Hosp Palliat Care. 2017;34(2):160–5.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Hammami S, Farah FA, Hammami MM. Surrogate end-of-life decisions: projection or simulation. Gen Med. 2016;4(3):1000247.CrossRef Hammami S, Farah FA, Hammami MM. Surrogate end-of-life decisions: projection or simulation. Gen Med. 2016;4(3):1000247.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Downey L, Engelberg RA, Curtis JR, Lafferty WE, Patrick DL. Shared priorities for the end-of-life period. J Pain Symptom Manag. 2009;37:175–88.CrossRef Downey L, Engelberg RA, Curtis JR, Lafferty WE, Patrick DL. Shared priorities for the end-of-life period. J Pain Symptom Manag. 2009;37:175–88.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Thomas DM, Watson RT. Q-sorting and MIS research: a primer. Comm Assoc Info Syst. 2002;8:141–56. Thomas DM, Watson RT. Q-sorting and MIS research: a primer. Comm Assoc Info Syst. 2002;8:141–56.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Hammami MM, Hammami S, Aboushaar R, Aljomah AS. Lay People's ethical attitudes to placebo treatment: a Q-methodology study. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2019;13:1599–617.CrossRef Hammami MM, Hammami S, Aboushaar R, Aljomah AS. Lay People's ethical attitudes to placebo treatment: a Q-methodology study. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2019;13:1599–617.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Hammami MM, Hammami MB, Aboushaar R. Modeling lay People’s ethical attitudes to organ donation: a Q-methodology study. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2020;14:173–89.CrossRef Hammami MM, Hammami MB, Aboushaar R. Modeling lay People’s ethical attitudes to organ donation: a Q-methodology study. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2020;14:173–89.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Graham J, Haidt J, Nosek B. Liberals and conservatives rely on different moral foundations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009;96(5):1029–46.CrossRef Graham J, Haidt J, Nosek B. Liberals and conservatives rely on different moral foundations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009;96(5):1029–46.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Libbus MK, Russell C. Congruence of decisions between patients and their potential surrogates about life-sustaining therapies. Image J Nurs Sch. 1995;27(2):135–40.CrossRef Libbus MK, Russell C. Congruence of decisions between patients and their potential surrogates about life-sustaining therapies. Image J Nurs Sch. 1995;27(2):135–40.CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Fried TR, Bradley EH, Towle VR. Valuing the outcomes of treatment. Do patients and their caregivers agree? Arch Intern Med. 2003;163:2073–8.CrossRef Fried TR, Bradley EH, Towle VR. Valuing the outcomes of treatment. Do patients and their caregivers agree? Arch Intern Med. 2003;163:2073–8.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Perkins HS, Cortez MA, Hazuda HP. Substituted judgment. J Gen Intern Med. 2008;24(1):144.CrossRef Perkins HS, Cortez MA, Hazuda HP. Substituted judgment. J Gen Intern Med. 2008;24(1):144.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Factors underlying surrogate medical decision-making in middle eastern and east Asian women: a Q-methodology study
verfasst von
Muhammad M. Hammami
Areej Al Balkhi
Sophia S. De Padua
Kafa Abuhdeeb
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2020
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Palliative Care / Ausgabe 1/2020
Elektronische ISSN: 1472-684X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12904-020-00643-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2020

BMC Palliative Care 1/2020 Zur Ausgabe

Update AINS

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.