Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Journal of Nutrition 6/2016

Open Access 18.08.2015 | Original Contribution

Flavonoid-rich orange juice is associated with acute improvements in cognitive function in healthy middle-aged males

verfasst von: Mudi H. Alharbi, Daniel J. Lamport, Georgina F. Dodd, Caroline Saunders, Laura Harkness, Laurie T. Butler, Jeremy P. E. Spencer

Erschienen in: European Journal of Nutrition | Ausgabe 6/2016

Abstract

Purpose

Epidemiological evidence suggests that chronic consumption of fruit-based flavonoids is associated with cognitive benefits; however, the acute effects of flavonoid-rich (FR) drinks on cognitive function in the immediate postprandial period require examination. The objective was to investigate whether consumption of FR orange juice is associated with acute cognitive benefits over 6 h in healthy middle-aged adults.

Methods

Males aged 30–65 consumed a 240-ml FR orange juice (272 mg) and a calorie-matched placebo in a randomized, double-blind, counterbalanced order on 2 days separated by a 2-week washout. Cognitive function and subjective mood were assessed at baseline (prior to drink consumption) and 2 and 6 h post consumption. The cognitive battery included eight individual cognitive tests. A standardized breakfast was consumed prior to the baseline measures, and a standardized lunch was consumed 3 h post-drink consumption.

Results

Change from baseline analysis revealed that performance on tests of executive function and psychomotor speed was significantly better following the FR drink compared to the placebo. The effects of objective cognitive function were supported by significant benefits for subjective alertness following the FR drink relative to the placebo.

Conclusions

These data demonstrate that consumption of FR orange juice can acutely enhance objective and subjective cognition over the course of 6 h in healthy middle-aged adults.
Hinweise
Mudi H. Alharbi and Daniel J. Lamport are joint first authors.

Introduction

Epidemiological data suggest that frequent consumption of citrus fruits is associated with various health benefits such as a reduced risk of cardiovascular disease, cerebral infarction and ischemic stroke [13]. Recently, there has also been interest in the relationship between fruit consumption and cognitive function [4], particularly given that fruits and juices provide a rich, easily available source of flavonoids such as hesperidin and narirutin. Human research of this field is in its infancy; however, recent reviews indicate that increased flavonoid consumption over the lifespan may attenuate age-associated cognitive decline and the onset of neurodegenerative disease [5, 6]. For example, increased consumption of citrus fruits and orange juice was associated with better cognitive outcomes in a cross-sectional analysis of 2031 females aged 70–74 [7].
In support of the epidemiological evidence, a handful of intervention studies in older adults with mild cognitive impairment indicate that daily intake of flavonoid-rich (FR) juices over 12–16 weeks can benefit memory function [810]. Cognitive benefits following chronic fruit juice consumption are not exclusive to adults with mild cognitive impairment; improvements in global cognitive function were observed in healthy older adults (mean age 67) following 8-week daily consumption of flavanone-rich orange juice (305 mg/day) relative to a low-flavanone control (37 mg) [11]. It has been hypothesized that increased cerebral blood flow (CBF) and increased neural activity following consumption of flavonoid-rich drinks could provide an underlying mechanism for the aforementioned cognitive benefits [9, 12]. In support of this, daily dietary supplementation of cocoa flavanols (900 mg/day) over 8 weeks was associated with increased cerebral blood volume in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus [13]. Crucially, concomitant improvements in pattern recognition were also observed, such that response times were 630 ms quicker following high-flavanol relative to low-flavanol supplementation. This provides evidence of possible causation between flavonoid intake, increased CBF and behavioral effects.
It remains to be seen whether acute cognitive benefits occur within the immediate postprandial phase following juice intake. However, acute improvements in memory and executive function have been observed in healthy young adults following consumption of flavonoid-rich cocoa-based drinks and solids [14, 15]. The aim of the present research was to investigate whether consumption of FR orange juice is associated with acute cognitive benefits in healthy middle-aged adults. Orange juice is one of the most commonly consumed sources of flavonoids, and the potential for fruits and fruit-based drinks to enhance cognitive function or attenuate cognitive decline deserves investigation, particularly in healthy populations for whom very limited published data exist. The flavonoid concentration of whole oranges can be higher than the concentration found in orange juice [16], principally because the fibers in orange act as an entrapping matrix [17]; thus, some of the flavonoids are lost during the juicing process. Therefore, in the present study, the intervention drink is composed of whole processed juicing orange which retains the fiber, and thus delivers a high dose of flavonoids.

Subjects and methods

Participants

Twenty-four healthy males were recruited from Reading, UK, and surrounding areas. Two participants withdrew following the completion of arm one due to work commitments (data not included). Inclusion criteria were males aged 30–65 (mean 51, SD 6.6), native English speaking, BMI 25–32 kg/m2 (mean 28.3, SD 3.1), non-smokers. Exclusion criteria were history of stroke/myocardial infarction, alcohol misuse, renal or bowel disease, pancreatitis, diabetes or any other endocrine disorder, dementia or mild cognitive impairment (mini–mental state examination [18] (MMSE) <26), current weight loss regime, vegetarian diet, regular intake of supplements including fish oil, fatty acid, vitamins and minerals, any medication for hypertension, hyperlipidaemia, inflammation, hypercholesterolemia and hypercoagulation, and history or evidence of depression (as indicated by the Brief Symptom Inventory, BSI). Pilot data by our group with a sample size of 24 healthy adults using the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) indicated an effect size (eta squared) of 0.32 following consumption of a fruit-based juice. Therefore, for a crossover design, assuming two-sided test criteria with alpha level 0.05 and a correlation of 0.5 between repeated-measures test scores, for two treatment conditions and three time-point measurements, a sample size of 24 would give power of at least 86 %. Recruitment commenced in June 2012 and terminated in December 2012.

Design

A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover design was used, and good clinical practice and ethical principles were followed according to ICH and the Declaration of Helsinki (1996). There were two drink conditions: placebo and FR orange juice, and measurements were taken at three time points: baseline (prior drink consumption), 2 h post consumption and 6 h post consumption of the drink. All procedures were approved by the University of Reading Research Ethics committee (No. 12/06).

Treatment drinks

The 240-ml FR drink consisted of Tropicana Pure Premium Orange Juice with 5.5 g of added orange pomace fiber (240 ml) which contained a total of 272 mg flavonoids (see Table 1 for drink composition). Analysis of the flavonoid content was performed by PepsiCo (Barrington, Illinois, USA). The orange pomace comprised of the micronized, edible part of the whole orange which is leftover during the production of Tropicana Pure Premium Orange Juice. The pomace is rich in fiber (40:60 ratio of soluble to insoluble) and contains small amounts of micronutrients, and a high proportion of the flavonoids found in whole orange. The control drink was prepared by dissolving a placebo mix containing glucose, fructose, sucrose and 0.67 % citric acid (for flavor purposes) in 240 ml of water. The drinks were closely matched for volume, taste, appearance, energy, glucose, fructose and sucrose (see Table 1). The ingredients for both drinks (except water) were provided by PepsiCo Inc. and were kept frozen (−20 °C) until preparation. The drinks and randomization sequence were prepared at the University of Reading by an independent researcher (AW) who did not come into contact with the participants or administer any of the test-day procedures. Both drinks were stored frozen (−20 °C) as individual portions (240 ml) in aluminum canisters and labeled with a three-digit code known only by AW. Blinded experimenters (MA, GD) performed the test-day procedures and provided participants with the drinks. Participants consumed the drinks from the canisters with black straws. The key for the codes remained sealed until data analysis was complete.
Table 1
Nutritional composition of the high-flavonoid and placebo drinks per 240 ml serving
 
Flavonoid rich
Placebo
Energy (kcal)
92.8
87.7
Glucose (g)
5.1
5.36
Fructose (g)
6.12
6.38
Sucrose (g)
11.99
10.2
Fiber (g)
5.50
Vitamin C (mg)
80.17
Folate (mg)
65.28
Total B-carotenes (mg)
0.26
Hesperidin (mg)
220.46
Narirutin (mg)
34.54
Other flavonoidsa (mg)
17.14
Total flavonoids (mg)
272.14
aIncludes didymin, sinensetin, nobiletin, tetramethylscutellarein and tangeretin

Procedure

Participants initially attended a screening visit during which inclusion and exclusion criteria were checked with the MMSE [18] (mean 28.2, SD 1.5), BSI (mean 1.05, SD 1.89) and a Health and Lifestyle Questionnaire. A practice version of the cognitive battery was completed for familiarization (data not collected). In addition, participants also completed the following tests which provide a proxy for IQ, National Adult Reading Test (NART; mean score 30.5, SD 8.5) and Block Design (from the Wechsler Adults Intelligence Scale-R [19]; mean score 38.2, SD 9.2). Height (mean 1.8 m, SD 7 cm) and weight (92.8 kg, SD 3.1) were also measured. During screening, participants were provided with oral and typed instructions to consume a low-polyphenol diet 24 h prior to each test day which included a description of foods and drinks to avoid (berries, fruits, fruit juices, jams and preserves, red wine, black, green and fruit teas, coffee, cocoa, soy products, caffeinated energy drinks and vegetables except potatoes). The screening visit and each test day were separated by a 2-week washout. The evening prior to each test-day participants consumed (at home) a low-fat standardized chicken and rice meal provided by the research team (350 kcal, 6.9 g fat of which 3 g saturates, 52.1 g carbohydrate of which 9.7 g sugars, 19 g protein, 1.4 g fiber, 0.9 g salt) to avoid second meal cognitive effects [20]. For each test day, participants arrived at 8:30 a.m. following a 12-h fast. During the fast, participants were permitted to consume only water provided at screening. A standardized breakfast of croissants and low-fat cream cheese was consumed upon arrival (24.5 g fat, 10.1 g protein, 38.4 g carbohydrates, 415 kcal). The baseline cognitive battery commenced at 9:10 a.m. followed by the consumption of the FR or control drink at 10:00 a.m. Instructions were to consume the whole drink within 5 min. Repeated testing of the cognitive battery commenced 2 and 6 h post-drink consumption (approximately 12:00 and 16:00 h, respectively). Following completion of the second cognitive battery, a lunch was provided (identical to breakfast at approximately 13:00). The rationale for cognitive testing at 2 and 6 h was based on the predicted bioavailability of hesperidin and narirutin metabolites in the plasma which peak between 4 and 7 h postingestion [21, 22]. All visits took place at the University of Reading Hugh Sinclair Nutrition Unit. Participants remained within the Nutrition Unit for the entire test day during which only water consumption was permitted (notwithstanding the test-day foods and drinks). Participants received a £100 honorarium upon completion of both arms.

Cognitive battery and subjective mood

The 45-min cognitive battery consisted of the following tests administered in the respective order: Immediate Word Recall; Simple and Complex Finger Tapping; DSST; Continuous Performance Task (CPT); Serial Sevens; Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS—a measure of subjective mood); Contrast Sensitivity; Delayed Word Recall. Immediate Word Recall involved computerized serial presentation of fifteen words in a random order at a rate of one word every 2 s. Following the presentation, participants were required to orally recall the words in any order. Presentation and recall occurred three times. The dependent variable was the mean number of words recalled per trial (averaged of three trials). False positives were not documented (e.g., saying a new word). Delayed Word Recall (45 min postinitial presentation) involved one attempt to orally recall the words presented during Immediate Word Recall. Simple Finger Tapping involved pressing the “2” key as many times as possible with the index finger over 10 s. The forearm was fixed, and two trials were performed with each hand. The first trial was considered a practice, and data were only collected for the second trial. Complex Finger Tapping followed the same procedure expect the required response was 1–2–3 (consecutively) using the forefinger for 1, the second finger for 2 and the third finger for 3. The dependent variable was the total number of responses, averaged across the dominant and non-dominant hand (errors were not recorded). The DSST [19] is a pen and paper test which contains a key of nine digit-symbol pairs and an accompanying list of digits. Under each listed digit, a space is provided for the participant to enter the corresponding symbol. The dependent variable was the number of symbols correctly entered over 90 s. The CPT involved random presentation of letters at a rate of one every 250 ms for a duration of 6 min. Participants were required to press the space bar when a letter other than “X” was presented. The dependent variable was the number of false positives (i.e., pressing the space bar when an “X” appeared). Serial Sevens required participants to orally count down in sevens from a randomly determined starting number ranging from 700 to 1000. The dependent variable was the number of correct responses over 2 min. Correct answers were determined relative to the previous number given, regardless of whether the previous response was correct or incorrect. The PANAS [23] requires a response to twenty adjective items on a scale ranging from 1 to 5 according to how the respondent is feeling at that moment with “not at all” and “extremely” as the anchor points. Scoring is collated into two factors reflecting Positive Affect and Negative Affect. In addition, analysis of each PANAS mood adjective was also performed. The Contrast Sensitivity test required identification of a series of letters on a computer screen. The letters become increasingly difficult to distinguish as the contrast between the letters and the background is reduced. The dependent variable was the point at which the letters could not be identified (Michelson Contrast).

Statistical analysis

Given the importance of considering baseline cognitive performance prior to drink consumption, change from baseline was calculated at each time point (2 and 6 h) for each cognitive test, and 2 × 2 repeated-measures, Bonferroni-corrected ANOVAs (Drink × Time) were performed using the change from baseline data with planned contrasts (t tests) to compare each drink (FR vs placebo) at 2 and 6 h. To examine equality at baseline, t tests were performed comparing FR and placebo at baseline for each cognitive test. In addition, for consistency, interpretability and agreement with other studies [24], the cognitive tests were combined to create a measure of global cognitive function. To do this, initially for each cognitive test, the change from baseline data was combined across all drink conditions (placebo and FR) and time points (2 and 6 h), thus forming one variable for each cognitive test which was subsequently converted into z-scores [24]. Subsequently, the mean z-score for each drink condition (placebo and FR) and each time point (2 and 6 h) was calculated such that global cognitive function = (Z DSST + Z Serial Sevens + Z Immediate recall + Z Delayed recall + Z CPT + Z simple finger tapping + Z complex finger tapping + Z Contrast Sensitivity)/8. The z-score data for Contrast Sensitivity and the CPT were multiplied by −1 prior to calculating global cognitive function since a higher score on these two tests indicates worse performance. Repeated-measures 2 × 2 ANOVAs (Drink × Time) were then performed with change from baseline z-score data for global cognitive function. Change from baseline was calculated for subjective mood data which were subsequently analyzed with Bonferroni-corrected repeated-measures 2 × 2 (Drink × Time) ANOVAs. Data were analyzed using SPSS Inc. PASW Statistics 18.

Results

Cognitive function

Raw scores for each cognitive test are shown in Table 2. There were no significant differences between the drinks at baseline for any dependent variables. The 2 × 2 ANOVA revealed a main effect of drink for Simple Finger Tapping (F[1, 20] = 8.32, p < 0.01) such that performance was better following the FR drink relative to the placebo. More specifically, the mean change from baseline across time points was significantly higher following the FR drink (mean 1.4, SE 0.8) relative to the placebo (mean −0.6, SE 0.5). As shown in Fig. 1, the planned contrasts revealed that the change from baseline was significantly different between the two drinks at 2 h (t = 3, df = 20, p < 0.01) and 6 h (t = 2.16, df = 20, p < 0.05). Similarly, a Drink × Time interaction was observed for CPT accuracy (F[1,21] = 3.96, p = 0.05) such that change from baseline was significantly different between the two drinks at 6 h (t = 2.02, df = 21, p < 0.05), but no significant difference was observed between the drinks at 2 h. More specifically, following the FR drink, the number of errors was reduced relative to baseline at 6 h (mean −1.6), whereas errors increased following the placebo relative to baseline at 6 h (mean 0.6). Considering all tests combined, the ANOVA model revealed no significant Drink × Time interaction, and no main effect of time. As shown in Fig. 2, the main effect of drink approached significance (F[1,21] = 2.98, p = 0.09), such that there was a nonsignificant trend for better global cognitive function following the FR drink relative to the control; however, the planned contrasts were not performed in light of the nonsignificant ANOVA model.
Table 2
Raw scores per drink condition for each cognitive test and PANAS mood outcomes at all time points (means and SE)
Outcomes measure
Time point
Placebo
Flavonoid-rich orange juice
Digit Symbol Substitution Test (seconds)
Baseline
61.1 (2.3)
62.2 (2.4)
2 h
61.7 (1.3)
62.9 (2.3)
6 h
61.6 (2.2)
62.6 (2.2)
Serial Sevens (number correct)
Baseline
39.7 (4.4)
40.9 (4.1)
2 h
40.1 (4.3)
42 (4.3)
6 h
40.5 (4.5)
43.5 (4.5)
Immediate Verbal Recall (words)
Baseline
11.1 (.5)
11 (.4)
2 h
10.2 (.3)
10.2 (.5)
6 h
10.2 (.4)
10.4 (.5)
Delayed Verbal Recall (words)
Baseline
11 (.7)
11.3 (.6)
2 h
9.6 (.7)
8.8 (.7)
6 h
9.5 (.8)
9.4 (.8)
Continuous Performance Task (errors)a,*
Baseline
8.2 (1.3)
9.2 (1.6)
2 h
6.4 (1.2)
7.9 (1.5)
6 h
8.8 (1.4)
7.6 (1.5)
Simple Finger Tapping* (correct responses)
Baseline
62.1 (1.7)
60.1 (1.8)
2 h
61.1 (1.7)
61.9 (1.7)
6 h
61 (1.6)
60.8 (1.7)
Complex Finger Tapping (correct responses)
Baseline
35 (3)
35.7 (3.5)
2 h
36.1 (2.8)
38 (3.9)
6 h
33.6 (2)
37.6 (3.9)
Contrast Sensitivitya (Michelson Contrast)
Baseline
3 (.2)
3 (.2)
2 h
3 (.1)
3.1 (.2)
6 h
3 (.2)
3 (.2)
Positive Affect (mood) (PANAS scoring)
Baseline
34.1 (1.7)
33.5 (1.7)
2 h
33.3 (1.6)
33.6 (1.6)
6 h
31.7 (1.6)
32.5 (1.9)
Negative Affect (mood) (PANAS scoring)
Baseline
11.2 (.4)
12 (.8)
2 h
10.7 (.3)
10.5 (.3)
6 h
10.7 (.3)
11.1 (.4)
aHigher score indicates worse performance
* A significant main effect of drink was observed for Simple Finger Tapping following a 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with the change from baseline data, such that performance was following the FR drink relative to the placebo (p < 0.05). A Drink × Time interaction was observed for CPT accuracy such that change from baseline was significantly different between the two drinks at 6 h (p < 0.05)

Subjective mood

There were no significant differences between the FR and placebo drinks for any of the subjective mood ratings at baseline. The 2 × 2 ANOVA revealed a main effect of drink for the mood adjective alertness (“how alert do you feel now”) (F[1,21] = 4.1, p = 0.05) indicating that the change from baseline was significantly different between the drinks. As shown in Fig. 3, a 6.4 % decline (SE 1.8) in alertness was observed over the morning relative to baseline following the placebo, whereas a 0.5 % decline (SE 2.7) was observed following the FR drink relative to baseline. This indicates that the FR drink attenuated a decline in alertness which was observed following the placebo. The individual planned contrasts comparing change from baseline alertness ratings between the drinks did not reach significance at either 2 or 6 h post consumption. Raw scores for Positive Affect and Negative Affect are shown in Table 2. The 2 × 2 ANOVA revealed no significant main effects or interactions for Positive Affect or Negative Affect.

Discussion

Consumption of FR orange juice was associated with improvements in cognitive function and subjective alertness relative to an energy-matched placebo in healthy middle-aged adults free of disease or mild cognitive impairment. Specifically, scores for the CPT (a measure of attention and more broadly executive function) were significantly better 6 h post orange juice consumption relative to the placebo, while finger tapping (a measure of psychomotor speed) was significantly better 2 and 6 h post consumption relative to the placebo. These findings were reflected by a nonsignificant trend showing improvement in global performance (all tests combined) throughout the day following the orange juice relative to baseline, in contrast to a subtle decline in global performance at both time points relative to baseline following the placebo. These effects on objective measures of cognitive function were mirrored by subjective alertness ratings whereby orange juice consumption attenuated a decline in alertness at both time points following the placebo. This indicates that FR orange juice can enhance objective and subjective cognitive function acutely overly 6 h.
It is important to acknowledge that orange juice consumption was not associated with a significant improvement on every individual cognitive test; this would not be expected given that the effects of nutritional interventions on cognitive performance are small and difficult to detect in healthy adults [25]. However, consistently higher means were observed following orange juice relative to the placebo in the vast majority of outcomes (see Table 2). This is consistent with the findings from an 8-week chronic orange juice intervention in healthy older adults which showed improved global cognitive function relative to a low-flavonoid control [11]. This emphasizes the importance of considering cognitive function as a whole rather than focusing exclusively on individual cognitive tests and domains. Nevertheless, it is interesting to note that the strongest benefits of orange juice were observed for the CPT and Simple Finger Tapping, tasks which required strong elements of executive function and psychomotor speed, respectively. Kean et al. [11] also reported subtle improvements for executive function following dietary supplementation of flavanone-rich orange juice; however, other chronic studies have focused almost exclusively on memory. For example, 12–16 weeks of grape or blueberry juice consumption has been shown to benefit memory in adults with mild cognitive impairment, but executive function was not assessed [810].
A key finding from the present research was the observation that cognitive benefits following a FR drink can occur within the immediate 6-h postprandial time period. Hendrickson and Mattes [26] failed to find any acute effects when cognitive performance in young adults was assessed in the afternoon following consumption of a grape juice drink during lunch. The younger sample recruited by Hendrickson and Mattes [26] (mean age 26) could account for the null effects relative to the present study (mean age 51); it is conceivable that healthy young adults typically perform at peak cognitive capability, and therefore, nutritional interventions may have little scope for impact. Having said that, benefits for executive function [15], spatial working memory and reaction time [14] have been observed in healthy young adults 2 h post consumption of non-fruit-based flavanol-rich cocoa drinks. Furthermore, general increases in mental fatigue over the morning were attenuated by a 520-mg cocoa flavanol drink [15], which reflects the attenuation of a decline in subjective alertness following the FR drink in the present study. Collectively, these data indicate that flavonoid-based acute nutritional interventions can improve performance on cognitive tests and enhance subjective mood and alertness in healthy adults. This highlights the possibility that cognitive effects are mediated by subjective mood, and proposed mechanisms of action for flavonoids on the brain should consider this.
Examining the potential underlying mechanisms was not the aim of this research; however, the greatest cognitive benefits were observed at 6 h which is consistent with the anticipated peak in plasma flavanone metabolites [21, 22]. In support, pilot bioavailability data (n = 6) in healthy adults following consumption of the same FR drink showed peak concentrations of 0.25 µM hesperetin and 0.12 µM naringenin in the plasma at 6 h. Therefore, it is reasonable to propose that mechanisms relating to the flavanones hesperidin and narirutin may underlie the cognitive benefits at 6 h. This hypothesis requires investigation as plasma flavanone concentration was not measured concomitantly with the behavioral outcomes. Effects at 2 h are more difficult to explain via this mechanism given that the concentration of metabolites would not be expected to be peak at this point according to the timescale for these flavanones to be absorbed into the small intestine, converted into glycosides, hydrolyzed by microflora in the large intestine, and appear as glucuronidated and sulfated metabolites in the plasma [22, 27, 28]. However, aforementioned research with cocoa shows that behavioral effects can be achieved within the 2-h postprandial period following flavonoid consumption [14, 15]. It is possible that the flavanone metabolites may be exerting effects in advance of reaching peak concentration in the plasma.
Once ingested and metabolized, the specific mechanisms by which flavonoids may affect cognitive function have been described in detail elsewhere [29, 30]. Flavanone metabolites are known to cross the blood-brain barrier following oral ingestion [31], and research on rodents indicates that flavanones may have specific neuroprotective effects such as increasing the expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) [32, 33]. However, it is unlikely mechanisms relating to BDNF and synthesis of proteins and enzymes in the brain such as cAMP-response element-binding protein (CREB), which affect neuronal signal transduction [34], could be effective within a 6-h timescale. One plausible hypothesis is that flavonoid consumption may lead to acute cognitive benefits via increased CBF as a result of enhanced endothelial function and increased bioavailability of nitric oxide [35, 36]. Human studies show significantly increased CBF several hours following cocoa flavanol consumption [12, 36]. These are supported by chronic studies which show increased activation in the right middle prefrontal cortex and the right superior parietal cortex following anthocyanin and flavanol-rich grape juice consumption [9]. Moreover, increased steady-state-evoked potentials in posterior parietal and central–frontal regions [37] and increased CBF in the hippocampus during a spatial memory task [13] have been observed following several weeks daily consumption of cocoa flavanols. As yet, there are no published data examining peripheral or CBF in humans following flavanone consumption; therefore, these potential mechanisms are speculative at this time.
It is possible that the observed cognitive effects in this study could be in part accounted for by vitamin C and/or folate which were not matched across the FR and placebo drinks. Cognitive benefits associated with these micronutrients are only likely to occur following chronic consumption over several weeks and months, or in cases where there are deficiencies [4]. Therefore, it is unlikely that vitamins or folate could account for the cognitive benefits observed 2–6 h post consumption in this healthy sample. A strength of the present study is that the beneficial effects occurred within the everyday context of consuming a standardized breakfast and lunch, indicating that positive effects are not dependent upon a period of fasting and relative energy deprivation. To extrapolate, FR orange juice-based interventions can offer acute cognitive benefits over the course of the day when consumed in conjunction with normal dietary intake. Having said that, participants were instructed to consume a low-flavonoid diet prior to each test day; therefore, it is unclear whether a habitually high-flavonoid diet would mask the effects of similar interventions. With regard to the generalizability of these findings, the orange juice drink was a commercial available product (Tropicana Pure Premium Orange Juice) with the addition of FR pomace, which is an edible part of the whole orange usually leftover during the production of the juice (see method). This FR drink is not currently available to the public although it is important to point out that it consists entirely of edible oranges.
To conclude, consumption of an orange juice containing 272 mg flavonoids was associated with acute improvements in cognitive function and subjective alertness up to 6 h post consumption relative to an energy-matched control drink containing no flavonoids in healthy middle-aged adults. Fruit juices and their respective fruits are an easily available commonly consumed source of flavonoids and polyphenols. These data demonstrate that fruit juice-based flavonoids can acutely enhance cognition in healthy adults. This is consistent with the accumulating evidence from chronic interventions and epidemiological research that increased consumption of fruits, fruit juices and other flavonoid-rich foods over the lifespan is associated with cognitive benefits such as a reduced risk of neuropsychological disease, attenuation of aging-induced cognitive decline and maintenance of optimal cognitive facilities [4].

Acknowledgments

This research was conducted as part of a PhD and was funded by PepsiCo Inc. CS and LH are employees of PepsiCo Inc. The authors have no other conflicts of interest to declare. MA, GFD, CS, LTB and JPES designed the research. DJL and MA analyzed the data and prepared the manuscript. CS, LH, LTB and JPES reviewed and edited the manuscript. MA, GFD and DJL conducted the research. The views expressed in this manuscript are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the position or policy of PepsiCo Inc. The ClinicalTrials.gov identifier is NCT01963416.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

Neuer Inhalt

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Dauchet L, Ferrières J, Arveiler D, Yarnell JW, Gey F, Ducimetière P, Ruidavets J-B, Haas A, Bingham A, Amouyel P et al (2004) Frequency of fruit and vegetable consumption and coronary heart disease in France and Northern Ireland: the PRIME study. Br J Nutr 92:963–972CrossRef Dauchet L, Ferrières J, Arveiler D, Yarnell JW, Gey F, Ducimetière P, Ruidavets J-B, Haas A, Bingham A, Amouyel P et al (2004) Frequency of fruit and vegetable consumption and coronary heart disease in France and Northern Ireland: the PRIME study. Br J Nutr 92:963–972CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Yamada T, Hayasaka S, Shibata Y, Ojima T, Saegusa T, Gotoh T, Ishikawa S, Nakamura Y, Kayaba K (2011) Frequency of citrus fruit intake is associated with the incidence of cardiovascular disease: the Jichi Medical School cohort study. J Epidemiol 21:169–175CrossRef Yamada T, Hayasaka S, Shibata Y, Ojima T, Saegusa T, Gotoh T, Ishikawa S, Nakamura Y, Kayaba K (2011) Frequency of citrus fruit intake is associated with the incidence of cardiovascular disease: the Jichi Medical School cohort study. J Epidemiol 21:169–175CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Cassidy A, Rimm EB, O’Reilly ÉJ, Logroscino G, Kay C, Chiuve SE, Rexrode KM (2012) Dietary flavonoids and risk of stroke in women. Stroke 43:946–951CrossRef Cassidy A, Rimm EB, O’Reilly ÉJ, Logroscino G, Kay C, Chiuve SE, Rexrode KM (2012) Dietary flavonoids and risk of stroke in women. Stroke 43:946–951CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Lamport DJ, Saunders C, Butler LT, Spencer JPE (2014) Fruits, vegetables, 100% juices, and cognitive function. Nutr Rev 72:774–789CrossRef Lamport DJ, Saunders C, Butler LT, Spencer JPE (2014) Fruits, vegetables, 100% juices, and cognitive function. Nutr Rev 72:774–789CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Macready AL, Kennedy OB, Ellis JA, Williams CM, Spencer JPE, Butler LT (2009) Flavonoids and cognitive function: a review of human randomized controlled trial studies and recommendations for future studies. Genes Nutr 4:227–242CrossRef Macready AL, Kennedy OB, Ellis JA, Williams CM, Spencer JPE, Butler LT (2009) Flavonoids and cognitive function: a review of human randomized controlled trial studies and recommendations for future studies. Genes Nutr 4:227–242CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Lamport DJ, Dye L, Wightman JD, Lawton CL (2012) The effects of flavonoid and other polyphenol consumption on cognitive performance: a systematic research review of human experimental and epidemiological studies. Nutr Aging 1:5–25 Lamport DJ, Dye L, Wightman JD, Lawton CL (2012) The effects of flavonoid and other polyphenol consumption on cognitive performance: a systematic research review of human experimental and epidemiological studies. Nutr Aging 1:5–25
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Nurk E, Refsum H, Drevon CA, Tell GS, Hygaard HA, Engedal K, Smith AD (2010) Cognitive performance among the elderly in relation to the intake of plant foods. The Hordaland Health Study. Br J Nutr 104:1190–1201CrossRef Nurk E, Refsum H, Drevon CA, Tell GS, Hygaard HA, Engedal K, Smith AD (2010) Cognitive performance among the elderly in relation to the intake of plant foods. The Hordaland Health Study. Br J Nutr 104:1190–1201CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Krikorian R, Nash TA, Shidler MD, Shukitt-Hale B, Joseph JA (2010) Concord grape juice supplementation improves memory function in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Br J Nutr 103:730–734CrossRef Krikorian R, Nash TA, Shidler MD, Shukitt-Hale B, Joseph JA (2010) Concord grape juice supplementation improves memory function in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Br J Nutr 103:730–734CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Krikorian R, Boespflug EL, Fleck DE, Stein AL, Wightman JD, Shidler MD, Sadat-Hossieny S (2012) Concord grape juice supplementation and neurocognitive function in human aging. J Agric Food Chem 60:5736–5742CrossRef Krikorian R, Boespflug EL, Fleck DE, Stein AL, Wightman JD, Shidler MD, Sadat-Hossieny S (2012) Concord grape juice supplementation and neurocognitive function in human aging. J Agric Food Chem 60:5736–5742CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Krikorian R, Shidler MD, Nash TA, Kalt W, Vinqvist-Tymchuk MR, Shukitt-Hale Joseph JA (2010) Blueberry supplementation improves memory in older adults. J Agric Food Chem 58:3996–4000CrossRef Krikorian R, Shidler MD, Nash TA, Kalt W, Vinqvist-Tymchuk MR, Shukitt-Hale Joseph JA (2010) Blueberry supplementation improves memory in older adults. J Agric Food Chem 58:3996–4000CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Kean RJ, Lamport DJ, Dodd GF, Freeman JE, Williams CM, Ellis JA, Butler LT, Spencer JPE (2015) Chronic consumption of flavanone-rich orange juice is associated with cognitive benefits: an 8-wk, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial in health adults. Am J Clin Nutr 101:506–514CrossRef Kean RJ, Lamport DJ, Dodd GF, Freeman JE, Williams CM, Ellis JA, Butler LT, Spencer JPE (2015) Chronic consumption of flavanone-rich orange juice is associated with cognitive benefits: an 8-wk, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial in health adults. Am J Clin Nutr 101:506–514CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Francis S, Head K, Morris PG, Macdonald IA (2006) The effect of flavanol-rich cocoa on the fMRI response to a cognitive task in healthy young people. J Cardiovasc Pharmacol 47:S215–S220CrossRef Francis S, Head K, Morris PG, Macdonald IA (2006) The effect of flavanol-rich cocoa on the fMRI response to a cognitive task in healthy young people. J Cardiovasc Pharmacol 47:S215–S220CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Brickman AM, Khan UA, Provenzano FA, Yeung L-K, Suzuki W, Schroeter H, Wall M, Sloan RP, Small SA (2014) Enhancing dentate gyrus function with dietary flavanols improves cognition in older adults. Nat Neurosci 17:798–1803CrossRef Brickman AM, Khan UA, Provenzano FA, Yeung L-K, Suzuki W, Schroeter H, Wall M, Sloan RP, Small SA (2014) Enhancing dentate gyrus function with dietary flavanols improves cognition in older adults. Nat Neurosci 17:798–1803CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Scholey AB, French SJ, Morris PJ, Kennedy DO, Milne AL, Haskell CF (2010) Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in acute improvements in mood and cognitive performance during sustained mental effort. J Psychopharmacol 24:1505–1514CrossRef Scholey AB, French SJ, Morris PJ, Kennedy DO, Milne AL, Haskell CF (2010) Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in acute improvements in mood and cognitive performance during sustained mental effort. J Psychopharmacol 24:1505–1514CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Field DT, Williams CM, Butler LT (2011) Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in acute improvement in visual and cognitive function. Physiol Behav 304:255–260CrossRef Field DT, Williams CM, Butler LT (2011) Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in acute improvement in visual and cognitive function. Physiol Behav 304:255–260CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Manach C, Morand C, Gil-Izquierdo A, Bouteloup-Demange C, Remesy C (2003) Bioavailability in humans of the flavanones hesperidin and narirutin after the ingestion of two doses of orange juice. Eur J Clin Nutr 57:235–242CrossRef Manach C, Morand C, Gil-Izquierdo A, Bouteloup-Demange C, Remesy C (2003) Bioavailability in humans of the flavanones hesperidin and narirutin after the ingestion of two doses of orange juice. Eur J Clin Nutr 57:235–242CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Palafox-Carlos H, Ayala-Zavala JF, Gonzalez-Aguilar GA (2011) The role of dietary fiber in the bioaccessibility and bioavailability of fruit and vegetable antioxidants. J Food Sci 76:R6–R15CrossRef Palafox-Carlos H, Ayala-Zavala JF, Gonzalez-Aguilar GA (2011) The role of dietary fiber in the bioaccessibility and bioavailability of fruit and vegetable antioxidants. J Food Sci 76:R6–R15CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Folstein M, Folstein S, McHugh PR (1975) ‘Mini-Mental State’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189–198CrossRef Folstein M, Folstein S, McHugh PR (1975) ‘Mini-Mental State’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189–198CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D (1981) WAIS-R manual: Wechsler adult intelligence scale—revised. The Psychological Corporation, New York Wechsler D (1981) WAIS-R manual: Wechsler adult intelligence scale—revised. The Psychological Corporation, New York
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Lamport DJ, Hoyle E, Lawton CL, Mansfield MW, Dye L (2011) Evidence for a second meal cognitive effect: glycaemic responses to high and low glycaemic index meals are associated with cognitive the following morning. Nutr Neurosci 14:66–72CrossRef Lamport DJ, Hoyle E, Lawton CL, Mansfield MW, Dye L (2011) Evidence for a second meal cognitive effect: glycaemic responses to high and low glycaemic index meals are associated with cognitive the following morning. Nutr Neurosci 14:66–72CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Manach C, Scalbert A, Morand C, Remesy C, Jimenez L (2004) Polyphenols: food sources and bioavailability. Am J Clin Nutr 79:727–747 Manach C, Scalbert A, Morand C, Remesy C, Jimenez L (2004) Polyphenols: food sources and bioavailability. Am J Clin Nutr 79:727–747
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Mullen W, Archeveque MA, Edwards CA, Matsumoto H, Crozier A (2008) Bioavailability and metabolism of orange juice flavanones in humans: impact of a full-fat yogurt. J Agr Food Chem 56:11157–11164CrossRef Mullen W, Archeveque MA, Edwards CA, Matsumoto H, Crozier A (2008) Bioavailability and metabolism of orange juice flavanones in humans: impact of a full-fat yogurt. J Agr Food Chem 56:11157–11164CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect; the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 54:1063–1070CrossRef Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect; the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 54:1063–1070CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Dangour AD, Allen E, Elbourne D, Fasey N, Fletcher AE, Hardy P, Holder GE, Knight R, Letley L, Richards M et al (2010) Effect of 2-y long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation on cognitive function in older people: a randomised double-blind, controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 91:1725–1732CrossRef Dangour AD, Allen E, Elbourne D, Fasey N, Fletcher AE, Hardy P, Holder GE, Knight R, Letley L, Richards M et al (2010) Effect of 2-y long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation on cognitive function in older people: a randomised double-blind, controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 91:1725–1732CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat de Jager CA, Dye L, de Bruin EA, Butler L, Fletcher J, Lamport DJ, Latulippe ME, Spencer JPE, Wesnes K (2014) Cognitive function: criteria for validation and selection of cognitive tests for investigating the effects of foods and nutrients. Nutr Rev 72:162–179CrossRef de Jager CA, Dye L, de Bruin EA, Butler L, Fletcher J, Lamport DJ, Latulippe ME, Spencer JPE, Wesnes K (2014) Cognitive function: criteria for validation and selection of cognitive tests for investigating the effects of foods and nutrients. Nutr Rev 72:162–179CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Nielsen ILF, Chee WSS, Poulsen L, Offord-Cavin E, Rasmussen SE, Frederiksen H, Enslen M, Barron D, Horcajada M-N, Williamson G (2006) Bioavailability is improved by enzymatic modification of the citrus flavonoid hesperidin in humans: a randomized, double-blind, crossover trial. J Nutr 136:404–408 Nielsen ILF, Chee WSS, Poulsen L, Offord-Cavin E, Rasmussen SE, Frederiksen H, Enslen M, Barron D, Horcajada M-N, Williamson G (2006) Bioavailability is improved by enzymatic modification of the citrus flavonoid hesperidin in humans: a randomized, double-blind, crossover trial. J Nutr 136:404–408
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Brett GM, Hollands W, Needs PW, Teucher B, Dainty RJ, Davis BD, Brodbelt S, Kroon PA (2009) Absorption, metabolism and excretion of flavanones from single portions of orange fruit and juice and effects of anthropometric variables and contraceptive pill use on flavanone excretion. Br J Nutr 101:664–675CrossRef Brett GM, Hollands W, Needs PW, Teucher B, Dainty RJ, Davis BD, Brodbelt S, Kroon PA (2009) Absorption, metabolism and excretion of flavanones from single portions of orange fruit and juice and effects of anthropometric variables and contraceptive pill use on flavanone excretion. Br J Nutr 101:664–675CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Spencer JPE (2010) Beyond antioxidants: the cellular and molecular interactions of flavonoids and how these underpin their actions on the brain. Proc Nutr Soc 60:244–260CrossRef Spencer JPE (2010) Beyond antioxidants: the cellular and molecular interactions of flavonoids and how these underpin their actions on the brain. Proc Nutr Soc 60:244–260CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Spencer JPE, Vafeiadou K, Williams RJ, Vauzour D (2012) Neuroinflammation: modulation by flavonoids and mechanisms of action. Mol Aspects Med 33:83–97CrossRef Spencer JPE, Vafeiadou K, Williams RJ, Vauzour D (2012) Neuroinflammation: modulation by flavonoids and mechanisms of action. Mol Aspects Med 33:83–97CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Youdim KA, Dobbie MS, Kuhnle G, Proteggente AR, Abbott NJ, Rice-Evans CJ (2003) Interaction between flavonoids and the blood–brain barrier: in vitro studies. J Neurochem 85:180–192CrossRef Youdim KA, Dobbie MS, Kuhnle G, Proteggente AR, Abbott NJ, Rice-Evans CJ (2003) Interaction between flavonoids and the blood–brain barrier: in vitro studies. J Neurochem 85:180–192CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Kumar P, Kumar A (2010) Protective effect of hesperidin and naringin against 3-nitropropionic acid induced Huntington’s like symptoms in rats: possible role of nitric oxide. Behav Brain Res 206:38–46CrossRef Kumar P, Kumar A (2010) Protective effect of hesperidin and naringin against 3-nitropropionic acid induced Huntington’s like symptoms in rats: possible role of nitric oxide. Behav Brain Res 206:38–46CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Rendeiro C, Vauzour D, Rattray M, Waffo-Teguo P, Merillon JM, Butler L, Williams CM, Spencer JPE (2013) Dietary levels of pure flavonoids improve spatial memory performance and increase hippocampal brain derived neurotrophic factor. PLoS One 28:e63535CrossRef Rendeiro C, Vauzour D, Rattray M, Waffo-Teguo P, Merillon JM, Butler L, Williams CM, Spencer JPE (2013) Dietary levels of pure flavonoids improve spatial memory performance and increase hippocampal brain derived neurotrophic factor. PLoS One 28:e63535CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Shieh PB, Hu S-C, Bobb K, Timmusk T, Ghosh A (1998) Identification of a signalling pathway involved in calcium regulation of BDNF expression. Neuron 20:727–740CrossRef Shieh PB, Hu S-C, Bobb K, Timmusk T, Ghosh A (1998) Identification of a signalling pathway involved in calcium regulation of BDNF expression. Neuron 20:727–740CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Fisher ND, Hughes M, Gerhard-Herman M, Hollenberg NK (2003) Flavanol-rich cocoa induces nitric-oxide-dependent vasodilation in healthy humans. J Hypertens 21:2281–2286CrossRef Fisher ND, Hughes M, Gerhard-Herman M, Hollenberg NK (2003) Flavanol-rich cocoa induces nitric-oxide-dependent vasodilation in healthy humans. J Hypertens 21:2281–2286CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Schroeter H, Heiss C, Balzer J, Kleinbongard P, Keen CL, Hollenberg NK, Sies H, Kwik-Uribe C, Schmitz HH, Kelm M (2006) (-)-Epicatechin mediates beneficial effects of flavanol-rich cocoa on vascular function in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:1024–1029CrossRef Schroeter H, Heiss C, Balzer J, Kleinbongard P, Keen CL, Hollenberg NK, Sies H, Kwik-Uribe C, Schmitz HH, Kelm M (2006) (-)-Epicatechin mediates beneficial effects of flavanol-rich cocoa on vascular function in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:1024–1029CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Camfield DA, Scholey A, Pipingas A, Silberstein R, Kras M, Nolidin K, Wesnes K, Pase M, Stough C (2012) Steady state visually evoked potential (SSVEP) topography changes associated with cocoa flavanol consumption. Physiol Behav 105:948–957CrossRef Camfield DA, Scholey A, Pipingas A, Silberstein R, Kras M, Nolidin K, Wesnes K, Pase M, Stough C (2012) Steady state visually evoked potential (SSVEP) topography changes associated with cocoa flavanol consumption. Physiol Behav 105:948–957CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Flavonoid-rich orange juice is associated with acute improvements in cognitive function in healthy middle-aged males
verfasst von
Mudi H. Alharbi
Daniel J. Lamport
Georgina F. Dodd
Caroline Saunders
Laura Harkness
Laurie T. Butler
Jeremy P. E. Spencer
Publikationsdatum
18.08.2015
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Journal of Nutrition / Ausgabe 6/2016
Print ISSN: 1436-6207
Elektronische ISSN: 1436-6215
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00394-015-1016-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 6/2016

European Journal of Nutrition 6/2016 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Innere Medizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Neu im Fachgebiet Innere Medizin

22.04.2024 | DGIM 2024 | Kongressbericht | Nachrichten

Krebspatienten impfen: Was? Wen? Und wann nicht?

22.04.2024 | DGIM 2024 | Kongressbericht | Nachrichten

Nierenultraschall: Tipps vom Profi

22.04.2024 | DGIM 2024 | Kongressbericht | Nachrichten

„KI sieht, was wir nicht sehen“

Update Innere Medizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.