Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Radiology 4/2017

Open Access 19.07.2016 | Neuro

Functional connectivity and microstructural white matter changes in phenocopy frontotemporal dementia

verfasst von: R. Meijboom, R. M. E. Steketee, I. de Koning, R. J. Osse, L. C. Jiskoot, F. J. de Jong, A. van der Lugt, J. C. van Swieten, M. Smits

Erschienen in: European Radiology | Ausgabe 4/2017

Abstract

Objectives

Phenocopy frontotemporal dementia (phFTD) is a rare and poorly understood clinical syndrome. PhFTD shows core behavioural variant FTD (bvFTD) symptoms without associated cognitive deficits and brain abnormalities on conventional MRI and without progression. In contrast to phFTD, functional connectivity and white matter (WM) microstructural abnormalities have been observed in bvFTD. We hypothesise that phFTD belongs to the same disease spectrum as bvFTD and investigated whether functional connectivity and microstructural WM changes similar to bvFTD are present in phFTD.

Methods

Seven phFTD patients without progression or alternative psychiatric diagnosis, 12 bvFTD patients and 17 controls underwent resting state functional MRI (rs-fMRI) and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Default mode network (DMN) connectivity and WM measures were compared between groups.

Results

PhFTD showed subtly increased DMN connectivity and subtle microstructural changes in frontal WM tracts. BvFTD showed abnormalities in similar regions as phFTD, but had lower increased DMN connectivity and more extensive microstructural WM changes.

Conclusions

Our findings can be interpreted as neuropathological changes in phFTD and are in support of the hypothesis that phFTD and bvFTD may belong to the same disease spectrum. Advanced MRI techniques, objectively identifying brain abnormalities, would therefore be potentially suited to improve the diagnosis of phFTD.

Key points

PhFTD shows brain abnormalities that are similar to bvFTD.
PhFTD shows increased functional connectivity in the parietal default mode network.
PhFTD shows microstructural white matter abnormalities in the frontal lobe.
We hypothesise phFTD and bvFTD may belong to the same disease spectrum.
Begleitmaterial
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1007/​s00330-016-4490-4) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Abkürzungen
phFTD
Phenocopy frontotemporal dementia

Introduction

Phenocopy (or nonprogressive) frontotemporal dementia (phFTD) is a rare and poorly understood syndrome, which was only recently described by Davies et al. 2006 [1] in a subgroup of behavioural variant FTD (bvFTD) patients who had a better prognosis than expected.
PhFTD symptomatology is very similar to that of bvFTD, but some aspects of phFTD are essentially different. In phFTD, core bvFTD symptoms, such as apathy, behavioural disinhibition and loss of insight [2], are generally not accompanied by cognitive and brain abnormalities as is the case in bvFTD. PhFTD patients show a cognitive profile that ranges from normal to suggesting FTD [36] and have a relatively intact performance of daily living activities (ADL) [2, 6]. These clinical features in phFTD appear stable over time, whereas in bvFTD patients rapid progression of cognitive deficits is evident [1, 68]. On conventional (structural) magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), phFTD patients show no or only borderline abnormalities [1, 8] in the frontal and temporal regions, which are typically affected in bvFTD [9]. Positron emission tomography (PET) does not show the frontal hypometabolism that is observed in bvFTD [10]. As bvFTD patients may initially also present without structural MRI abnormalities, early stage distinction between phFTD and bvFTD may be difficult.
A pathophysiological explanation for phFTD symptomatology is currently unavailable. Patients often remain undiagnosed or receive an alternative psychiatric diagnosis. Additionally, they are occasionally found to be C9orf72 mutation carriers [11]. It is therefore of importance to investigate the presence of possible brain abnormalities underlying their symptoms using more advanced MRI techniques such as resting state functional MRI (rs-fMRI) and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). These techniques measure subtle brain changes so far left unexplored by looking at functional connectivity and microstructural white matter (WM). A well-defined network showing functional abnormalities in bvFTD is the default mode network (DMN). In bvFTD, parietal regions of the DMN show increased connectivity [12, 13]. Frontal DMN connectivity changes are more ambiguous, found to be either increased [14] or decreased [12]. Such functional changes are thought to precede grey matter atrophy appearing at the later stages of bvFTD [12, 15]. WM abnormalities in bvFTD are found mainly in frontal and temporal areas such as the uncinate fasciculus (UF), cingulum and genu of the corpus callosum (CC) [16]. Similar regions are found to be already affected in asymptomatic FTD mutation carriers [15].
As phFTD patients present with behavioural symptoms similar to bvFTD, we hypothesise that phFTD and bvFTD may belong to the same disease spectrum. This study investigates whether phFTD patients have underlying brain abnormalities that are similar to those seen in bvFTD patients: functional brain abnormalities expressed as DMN connectivity changes and microstructural WM abnormalities expressed as diffusion changes.

Methods

Participants

All patients were recruited in the Alzheimer Centre Southwest Netherlands. PhFTD patients (aged 40-75 years) with prominent behavioural changes interfering with social functioning, consisting of disinhibition and/or apathy and/or stereotypy; no reported progression 1 year after initial routine diagnostic workup, and bvFTD patients (aged 45-70 years) with a diagnosis of bvFTD [17]; a clinical dementia rating scale score of ≤1; and a Mini-Mental State Examination [18] (MMSE) score of ≥ 20 were included in the study.
Patients with other neurological disorders, past or current substance abuse or other psychiatric diagnosis were excluded. PhFTD patients with a diagnosis of dementia and missing heteroanamnesis and bvFTD patients with a different cause of dementia were also excluded.
Healthy controls (aged 60-70 years), without neurological or psychiatric history, were recruited through advertisement. They were matched for gender with phFTD patients and for age with all patients.
The study was approved by the local medical ethics committee. All participants gave written informed consent.

Psychiatric, neuropsychological and genetic mutation assessment

PhFTD patients underwent full psychiatric assessment as part of this study to exclude alternative diagnoses. Additionally, their DNA was tested for the C9orf72 mutation.
All participants underwent a full neuropsychological assessment and an MMSE [18]. Six phFTD patients had a cognitive profile suggestive of FTD, but showed no progression relative to previous neuropsychological testing consistent with the phFTD criteria. One phFTD patient had a normal cognitive profile. Mean interval between current and first neuropsychological testing was 36 months (range: 8-71 months). The cognitive profile was consistent with bvFTD for bvFTD patients and normal for controls.

Image acquisition

Scanning was performed on two 3T GE Discovery MR750 systems (GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, WI, US) with identical protocols. PhFTD patients and 12 controls were scanned on one and bvFTD patients and 8 controls on the other scanner. All participants underwent a high-resolution three-dimensional (3D) inversion recovery (IR) fast spoiled gradient echo (FSPGR) T1-weighted image for anatomical reference and a functional gradient echo echo planar imaging (EPI) and DTI spin echo EPI with full coverage of the supratentorial brain (Table 1). During the functional scan participants were instructed to focus on a fixation cross, to think of nothing in particular and to stay awake.
Table 1
Acquisition parameters
 
T1w
fMRI
DTI
FOV (mm)
240
240
240
TE (ms)
3.06
30
84.2*
TR (ms)
7.90
3000
7925
ASSET factor
2
2
2
Flip angle
120°
90°
90°
Acquisition matrix
240 × 240
96 × 96
128 × 128
Slice thickness (mm)
1
3
2.5
Volumes (slices per volume)
1 (176)
200 (44)
28 (59)
Duration (min)
4.41
10.00
3.50
Diffusion-weighted directions
n/a
n/a
25
Maximum b-value (s/mm2)
n/a
n/a
1000
TI (ms)
450
n/a
n/a
T1w = T1-weighted, fMRI = functional magnetic resonance imaging, DTI = diffusion tensor imaging, FOV = field of view, TE = echo time, TR = repetition time, ASSET = array spatial sensitivity encoding technique, TI = inversion time
*TE for DTI was set to the minimum. This number represents the average TE. The range of TE was 81.9-90.8 ms

Structural MRI analysis

Whole-brain grey matter (GM) volume was calculated for each participant according to the methods described in Bron et al. 2014 [19]. For each participant, whole-brain GM volume was divided by their individual intracranial volume (ICV) to correct for head size (expressed as % ICV) [2022] and referred to as normalised GM (nGM). These values (%ICV) were then averaged per group, resulting in a mean nGM volume expressed as %ICV. These group means were then compared between groups using a one-way ANOVA and post-hoc Bonferroni tests (SPSS21, USA).

Rs-fMRI analysis

Rs-fMRI data were analysed using FMRIB Software library (FSL4.1.9, UK) (see supplement section 1 for more detail). The brain was extracted using the Brain Extraction Tool (BET) [23]. Then, MELODIC independent component analysis was used for pre-processing of the functional data and establishing the DMN component. Due to the small sample size, only one network of interest could be investigated. The DMN was chosen as it is a well-defined functional network showing functional abnormalities in bvFTD, allowing for meaningful comparison between phFTD and bvFTD. Hereafter, the participant-specific DMN was identified using dual regression [24]. Subsequently, Randomize [25] was used to assess between-group DMN connectivity differences using a one-way ANCOVA with three groups (phFTD≠bvFTD≠controls) and GM volume as covariate. Main effects were investigated and six t-contrasts (phFTD>HC, HC>phFTD, bvFTD>HC, HC>bvFTD, phFTD>bvFTD, bvFTD>phFTD) were constructed to assess post-hoc between-group differences.
Cluster was used to extract cluster information. Results [p corrected < 0.05, cluster size (k) ≥ 1; puncorrected <0.05, k ≥ 20] were visualised using FSLview. Structural atlases implemented in FSLView were used to anatomically identify the DMN regions.

DTI analysis

Data were corrected for motion, eddy currents and EPI distortions using ExploreDTI [26]. Further analyses were performed with FSL (5.0.2.2, UK) (see supplement section 1 for more detail). BET [23] was used to create skull-stripped binary masks, after which DTIFIT [27] was used to reconstruct diffusion tensors and to create subject images for all WM measures, i.e. fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD), axial diffusivity (AxD) and radial diffusivity (RD). Then, registration was performed using Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS) [28], resulting in subject-specific WM skeletons for each WM measure. Subsequently, Randomize [25] was used to assess between-group WM measure differences using a one-way ANOVA with the same groups and post-hoc t-tests as in the rs-fMRI analysis.
Cluster was used to extract cluster information. Results (p corrected < 0.05, k ≥ 20) were visualised using FSLView. The WM atlases implemented in FSLView were used to anatomically identify the WM regions.

Results

Participant and disease characteristics

Data of 7 phFTD patients, 12 bvFTD patients (9 for rs-fMRI analysis) and 17 controls were used for the current analyses (Table 2). In total, 9 phFTD patients (all male), 12 bvFTD patients (7 male) and 20 healthy controls (all male) were originally included in the study. Two phFTD patients and three controls were excluded from the analysis (see supplement section 2). Three bvFTD patients had missing rs-fMRI data. One phFTD patient’s follow-up was just under 1 year (11 months), for logistical reasons. The clinical diagnosis phFTD was established based on the clinical profile and lack of disease progression.
Table 2
Demographic characteristics
Group
N
Mean age (years)
Mean MMSE
PhFTD
7 (all male)
63.4 (4.8)
26.6 (1.4)
BvFTD
12 (7 male)
60.2 (7.6)
26.6 (2.8)
Controls
17 (all male)
64.1 (3.3)
28.2 (1.5)
PhFTD = phenocopy FTD, BvFTD = behavioural FTD, N = sample size, values given as mean (SD), MMSE = Mini-mental State Examination
Age [H (2) = 2,23 p > 0.05, Table 2] and the MMSE score did not differ between groups [H (2) = 5,93, p > 0.05, Table 2].
None of the phFTD patients received an alternative psychiatric diagnosis that could explain their behavioural symptoms. Additionally, none carried the C9orf72 mutation.

Structural MRI

The three groups showed a difference in nGM [H (2) = 16.38, p < 0.05], with a mean of 0.32 %ICV (SD 0.02) for phFTD, 0.29 %ICV (SD 0.04) for bvFTD and 0.35 %ICV (SD 0.02) for controls. Compared with controls, both phFTD patients (p = 0.013) and bvFTD patients (p < 0.001) had lower nGM volume. nGM volume was not different between phFTD and bvFTD patients (p = 0.359).

Functional connectivity

PhFTD and bvFTD patients (Fig. 1, supplement Table 1A) showed connectivity in all regions of the DMN. Controls showed connectivity in all DMN regions except the right lateral temporal cortex (LTC) (Fig. 1, supplement Table 1A).
PhFTD patients compared with controls showed increased DMN connectivity in the bilateral medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), LTC and inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and in the left posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)/precuneus (Fig. 2, supplement Table 1B).
BvFTD patients compared with controls showed increased DMN connectivity in the bilateral mPFC and right LTC and IPL (Fig. 2, supplement Table 1B) and decreased DMN connectivity in the more posterior right IPL.
PhFTD patients compared with bvFTD patients showed increased DMN connectivity in the bilateral mPFC and LTC and right IPL (Fig. 2, supplement Table 1B).

Microstructural WM

PhFTD and bvFTD patients compared with controls showed decreased FA and increased RD and MD mainly in the frontal and temporoparietal WM (Fig. 3, supplement Tables 2A-C), such as the cingulum (both cingulate and hippocampus portion), inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), corpus callosum and uncinate fasciculus. In bvFTD patients in comparison with controls increased AxD was observed in these regions as well (Fig. 3, supplement Table 2D).
BvFTD patients compared with phFTD patients showed decreased FA and increased AxD in frontal WM and increased MD and RD in frontotemporal WM, mainly in the cingulum (cingulate portion), IFOF, SLF, corpus callosum and uncinate fasciculus (Fig. 4, supplement Tables 2A-D).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study demonstrating functional connectivity changes and microstructural WM abnormalities in phFTD. There was increased DMN connectivity in nearly all regions of the DMN and abnormal microstructural WM in the frontal and temporoparietal lobes. These changes were similar to the changes observed in bvFTD, supporting our hypothesis that phFTD may belong to the same disease spectrum as bvFTD. Specifically, bvFTD also showed higher connectivity in DMN regions, but to a lesser extent than in phFTD, and microstructural WM abnormalities in the frontal and temporoparietal lobes, but more pronounced than in phFTD.
DMN connectivity was increased, albeit to a moderate extent, in both phFTD and bvFTD. As there are overt behavioural symptoms in phFTD, as well as frequent neuropsychological abnormalities, it is not surprising to observe functional brain abnormalities. Increased functional connectivity reflects changes in neuronal activity becoming more congruent between regions. This may point to a brain mechanism compensating for early diminished neuronal functioning [29, 30]. The degree of increased connectivity may reflect the brain’s remaining ability of compensation, and in case of advanced neuronal dysfunctioning, this ability may no longer present, resulting in decreased connectivity. This theory may explain why we observe higher connectivity in phFTD than in bvFTD. As pronounced cortical atrophy is evident in bvFTD but not in phFTD [1, 8, 9, 31], neuronal dysfunctioning is likely much more prominent in bvFTD. This means that more relatively preserved neurons in phFTD may be able to provide a better functioning compensational mechanism than in bvFTD. The observation that the inferior parietal lobule showed both increased and decreased (depending on its subregion) functional connectivity in bvFTD in comparison with controls is in line with this view. It is plausible that the various subregions of the inferior parietal lobule are not affected to the same extent in bvFTD, resulting in decreased functional connectivity in the more affected subregion and in increased functional connectivity in the less affected subregion. Future studies consisting of a larger patient sample could shed further light on whether other resting-state networks show similar functional connectivity changes to the DMN. A network of particular interest would be the salience network, which is a frontal functional network known to be affected in bvFTD [13]. This would allow for further meaningful comparisons between phFTD and bvFTD.
Frontotemporal and parietal microstructural WM abnormalities were observed in both phFTD and bvFTD. In phFTD, FA was decreased (i.e. there was less directional diffusion) and RD and MD were increased (i.e. there was more diffusion, particularly perpendicular to the tract’s axis) in multiple WM tracts, including the cingulum, UF, IFOF, genu of the CC and SLF. Damage to these WM tracts has been linked to the various cognitive functions typically affected in bvFTD. Loss of behavioural control (e.g. disinhibition) has been related to diffusion changes in the UF, forceps minor and cingulum [32, 33]. Additionally, abnormalities in executive functioning, visuo-spatial attention, working memory and apathy have also been linked with diffusivity changes in the cingulum [34, 35]. Interestingly, RD changes in these tracts were less pronounced than FA changes, which may be explained by FA being a composite measure of both RD and AxD (i.e. diffusion along the tract’s main axis), and therefore more sensitive to subtle myelin and/or axonal changes, reflected by changes in RD [36] and AxD [37] respectively. In phFTD, AxD abnormalities were not observed, which may be explained by myelin damage only, without axonal injury. In bvFTD, WM changes were more pronounced and widespread, with lower FA and higher MD, RD and AxD than in phFTD. Hence, here we show an association in phFTD, similar to bvFTD, between symptomatology and damage to the frontotemporal and parietal WM tracts. Additionally, we show differences possibly reflecting neuropathological changes between phFTD and bvFTD, with phFTD suggesting myelin damage only and bvFTD showing more pronounced myelin and axonal damage.
Previous literature has shown a relationship between microstructural WM changes and functional connectivity changes [3840] and proposes that microstructural WM predicts, or is reflected by, functional connectivity [39, 40]. For example, the medial prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex, core regions of the DMN, are connected through the cingulum [39]. Cingulum abnormalities such as observed in this study may have— to a certain extent—disconnected these regions, reducing the functional connectivity between the anterior and posterior DMN regions. In support of this idea, there were more pronounced anterior WM abnormalities in bvFTD that extended more posteriorly than in phFTD, and both frontal and parietal functional connectivity were seen to be correspondingly lower. A recent study by Weiler et al. (2014) [41] observed that higher RD in the cingulum and parahippocampal bundle (both connecting DMN regions) predicted reduced performance on measures related to DMN cognitive functions. We therefore hypothesise that more advanced abnormalities of WM tracts of the DMN may eventually lead to a functional decrease in the associated DMN areas and result in reduced cognitive functioning. As we did not aim to directly explore such a mechanism, our study design does not allow for any firm conclusions concerning this mechanism. A longitudinal design employing correlational analyses is needed to verify this hypothesis.
Overall, phFTD showed functional connectivity and subtle WM changes, whereas bvFTD showed fewer functional connectivity and more extensive WM changes. Lower overall cortical GM volume in phFTD patients was observed in this study using a quantitative method, but was not observed in the regular clinical diagnostic work-up. This suggests that GM volume loss is also present in phFTD patients, but at such a limited degree that it is not clinically detected. These findings are indicative of incipient degeneration in phFTD. In order to investigate phFTD without the interference of an alternative diagnosis we ruled out alternative psychiatric disorders, neuropsychological progression and presence of the C9orf72 mutation. The observed incipient brain changes in this well-defined population are in favour of the controversial notion that phFTD and bvFTD may belong to the same disease spectrum. PhFTD presents with behavioural, neuropsychological and, as shown here, also neurodegenerative changes that are all similar to those observed in bvFTD.
This study has some limitations. First, we were only able to investigate a small number of phFTD and bvFTD patients, limiting the statistical power. As a result, rs-fMRI effects, already expected to be subtle, were only detectable using a relatively lenient statistical threshold and could only be investigated in one functional network. The sample size is inherent to the rarity of the phFTD syndrome, together with the application of strict inclusion and exclusion criteria to avoid inadvertent inclusion of patients with bvFTD or alternative psychiatric disorders. Second, the bvFTD group was not fully matched for gender with the phFTD and control group. There is no conclusive evidence on gender differences in functional connectivity [42] or microstructural WM. Both higher and lower FA was measured in the cingulum and in the WM underlying the frontal cortex in men and in women compared with the opposite gender [43]. Given these findings it is not likely that the FA decreases observed in this study were driven by gender differences. Third, the bvFTD group and part of the control group were scanned on a different scanner, although of identical type and field strength, and with identical protocols. While a scanner effect cannot be excluded, both rs-fMRI and DTI have been shown to be highly reproducible in terms of DMN functional connectivity and TBSS respectively, even across different scanner platforms and vendors [44, 45]. Moreover, the fact that our findings in bvFTD are consistent with the previous literature suggests that the scanner effect is likely to be minimal.
In conclusion, our findings are in support of the hypothesis that phFTD and bvFTD may belong to the same disease spectrum. In phFTD, there are changes in functional connectivity and microstructural WM that are similar to those found in bvFTD. Advanced MRI techniques, such as rs-fMRI and DTI, are therefore potentially suited to improve the diagnosis of phFTD by identifying such incipient changes. Naturally, the hypothesis that phFTD and bvFTD may belong to the same disease spectrum would require confirmation with other diagnostic tools, such as histopathology. Also, further assessment in a longitudinal study to assess changes over time would be required, at which our future efforts are aimed. Our findings could provide a direction for further development of MR—or other—diagnostic tools.

Acknowledgements

We want to thank Elise Dopper for sharing her resting-state fMRI expertise, Rick Zuur and Sanne Detiger for conducting helpful work on resting-state fMRI during their internship, and Lieke Meeter and Tsz Wong for assisting with the genetic component of this study. The scientific guarantor of this publication is Dr M. Smits. The authors of this manuscript declare no relationships with any companies, whose products or services may be related to the subject matter of the article. This study has received funding by a personal fellowship granted in 2008 by the Erasmus University Rotterdam. One of the authors has significant statistical expertise. Institutional Review Board approval was obtained. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects (patients) in this study. Methodology: prospective, case-control study, performed at one institution.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Radiologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Radiologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Radiologie, den Premium-Inhalten der radiologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Radiologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Anhänge

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Davies RR, Kipps CM, Mitchell J et al (2006) Progression in frontotemporal dementia: identifying a benign behavioral variant by magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Neurol 63:1627–1631CrossRefPubMed Davies RR, Kipps CM, Mitchell J et al (2006) Progression in frontotemporal dementia: identifying a benign behavioral variant by magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Neurol 63:1627–1631CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Hornberger M, Shelley BP, Kipps CM et al (2009) Can progressive and non-progressive behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia be distinguished at presentation? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 80:591–593CrossRefPubMed Hornberger M, Shelley BP, Kipps CM et al (2009) Can progressive and non-progressive behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia be distinguished at presentation? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 80:591–593CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Bertoux M, de Souza LC, Corlier F, et al. Two Distinct Amnesic Profiles in Behavioral Variant Frontotemporal Dementia. Biol Psychiatry Published Online First: 2013. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.017 Bertoux M, de Souza LC, Corlier F, et al. Two Distinct Amnesic Profiles in Behavioral Variant Frontotemporal Dementia. Biol Psychiatry Published Online First: 2013. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.017
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Hornberger M, Piguet O, Kipps C et al (2008) Executive function in progressive and nonprogressive behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 71:1481–1488CrossRefPubMed Hornberger M, Piguet O, Kipps C et al (2008) Executive function in progressive and nonprogressive behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 71:1481–1488CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Irish M, Graham A, Graham KS et al (2012) Differential impairment of source memory in progressive versus non-progressive behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 27:338–347CrossRefPubMed Irish M, Graham A, Graham KS et al (2012) Differential impairment of source memory in progressive versus non-progressive behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 27:338–347CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Mioshi E, Hodges JR (2009) Rate of change of functional abilities in frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 28:419–426CrossRefPubMed Mioshi E, Hodges JR (2009) Rate of change of functional abilities in frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 28:419–426CrossRefPubMed
7.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Kipps CM, Davies RR, Mitchell J et al (2007) Clinical significance of lobar atrophy in frontotemporal dementia: application of an MRI visual rating scale. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 23:334–342CrossRefPubMed Kipps CM, Davies RR, Mitchell J et al (2007) Clinical significance of lobar atrophy in frontotemporal dementia: application of an MRI visual rating scale. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 23:334–342CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Kipps CM, Hodges JR, Fryer TD et al (2009) Combined magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography brain imaging in behavioural variant frontotemporal degeneration: refining the clinical phenotype. Brain 132:2566–2578CrossRefPubMed Kipps CM, Hodges JR, Fryer TD et al (2009) Combined magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography brain imaging in behavioural variant frontotemporal degeneration: refining the clinical phenotype. Brain 132:2566–2578CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Khan BK, Yokoyama JS, Takada LT et al (2012) Atypical, slowly progressive behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia associated with C9ORF72 hexanucleotide expansion. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 83:358–364CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Khan BK, Yokoyama JS, Takada LT et al (2012) Atypical, slowly progressive behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia associated with C9ORF72 hexanucleotide expansion. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 83:358–364CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Whitwell JL, Josephs KA, Avula R et al (2011) Altered functional connectivity in asymptomatic MAPT subjects: a comparison to bvFTD. Neurology 77:866–874CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Whitwell JL, Josephs KA, Avula R et al (2011) Altered functional connectivity in asymptomatic MAPT subjects: a comparison to bvFTD. Neurology 77:866–874CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhou J, Greicius MD, Gennatas ED et al (2010) Divergent network connectivity changes in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 133:1352–1367CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Zhou J, Greicius MD, Gennatas ED et al (2010) Divergent network connectivity changes in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 133:1352–1367CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Rytty R, Nikkinen J, Paavola L et al (2013) GroupICA dual regression analysis of resting state networks in a behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Front Hum Neurosci 7:461CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rytty R, Nikkinen J, Paavola L et al (2013) GroupICA dual regression analysis of resting state networks in a behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Front Hum Neurosci 7:461CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Dopper EGP, Rombouts SARB, Jiskoot LC et al (2014) Structural and functional brain connectivity in presymptomatic familial frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 83:e19–e26CrossRefPubMed Dopper EGP, Rombouts SARB, Jiskoot LC et al (2014) Structural and functional brain connectivity in presymptomatic familial frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 83:e19–e26CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Mahoney CJ, Ridgway GR, Malone IB et al (2014) Profiles of white matter tract pathology in frontotemporal dementia. Hum Brain Mapp 35:4163–4179CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Mahoney CJ, Ridgway GR, Malone IB et al (2014) Profiles of white matter tract pathology in frontotemporal dementia. Hum Brain Mapp 35:4163–4179CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman D et al (2011) Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 134:2456–2477CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman D et al (2011) Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 134:2456–2477CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Bron EE, Steketee RME, Houston GC et al (2014) Diagnostic classification of arterial spin labeling and structural MRI in presenile early stage dementia. Hum Brain Mapp 35:4916–4931CrossRefPubMed Bron EE, Steketee RME, Houston GC et al (2014) Diagnostic classification of arterial spin labeling and structural MRI in presenile early stage dementia. Hum Brain Mapp 35:4916–4931CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Arvanitakis Z, Fleischman DA, Arfanakis K, et al. Association of white matter hyperintensities and gray matter volume with cognition in older individuals without cognitive impairment. Brain Struct Funct 2015; 135–46. doi:10.1007/s00429-015-1034-7 Arvanitakis Z, Fleischman DA, Arfanakis K, et al. Association of white matter hyperintensities and gray matter volume with cognition in older individuals without cognitive impairment. Brain Struct Funct 2015; 135–46. doi:10.1007/s00429-015-1034-7
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Elderen SSGC, Zhang Q, Sigurdsson S et al (2015) Brain Volume as an integrated marker for the risk of death in a community-based sample: Age Gene/Environment Susceptibility-Reykjavik Study. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci 71:131–137CrossRef Van Elderen SSGC, Zhang Q, Sigurdsson S et al (2015) Brain Volume as an integrated marker for the risk of death in a community-based sample: Age Gene/Environment Susceptibility-Reykjavik Study. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci 71:131–137CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Dalton CM, Chard DT, Davies GR et al (2004) Early development of multiple sclerosis is associated with progressive grey matter atrophy in patients presenting with clinically isolated syndromes. Brain 127:1101–1107CrossRefPubMed Dalton CM, Chard DT, Davies GR et al (2004) Early development of multiple sclerosis is associated with progressive grey matter atrophy in patients presenting with clinically isolated syndromes. Brain 127:1101–1107CrossRefPubMed
23.
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Filippini N, MacIntosh BJ, Hough MG et al (2009) Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-epsilon4 allele. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:7209–7214CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Filippini N, MacIntosh BJ, Hough MG et al (2009) Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-epsilon4 allele. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:7209–7214CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Leemans A, Jeurissen B, Sijbers J et al (2009) ExploreDTI: a graphical toolbox for processing, analyzing, and visualizing diffusion MR data. In: Proceedings 17th Scientific Meeting, International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine Leemans A, Jeurissen B, Sijbers J et al (2009) ExploreDTI: a graphical toolbox for processing, analyzing, and visualizing diffusion MR data. In: Proceedings 17th Scientific Meeting, International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Jenkinson M et al (2003) Characterization and propagation of uncertainty in diffusion-weighted MR imaging. Magn Reson Med 50:1077–1088CrossRefPubMed Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Jenkinson M et al (2003) Characterization and propagation of uncertainty in diffusion-weighted MR imaging. Magn Reson Med 50:1077–1088CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H et al (2006) Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage 31:1487–1505CrossRefPubMed Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H et al (2006) Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage 31:1487–1505CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Borroni B, Alberici A, Cercignani M et al (2012) Granulin mutation drives brain damage and reorganization from preclinical to symptomatic FTLD. Neurobiol Aging 33:2506–2520CrossRefPubMed Borroni B, Alberici A, Cercignani M et al (2012) Granulin mutation drives brain damage and reorganization from preclinical to symptomatic FTLD. Neurobiol Aging 33:2506–2520CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Bookheimer SY, Strojwas MH, Cohen MS et al (2000) Patterns of brain activation in people at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 343:450–456CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bookheimer SY, Strojwas MH, Cohen MS et al (2000) Patterns of brain activation in people at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 343:450–456CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Seeley WW, Crawford R, Rascovsky K et al (2008) Frontal paralimbic network atrophy in very mild behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Arch Neurol 65:249–255CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Seeley WW, Crawford R, Rascovsky K et al (2008) Frontal paralimbic network atrophy in very mild behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Arch Neurol 65:249–255CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Hornberger M, Geng J, Hodges JR (2011) Convergent grey and white matter evidence of orbitofrontal cortex changes related to disinhibition in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain 134:2502–2512CrossRefPubMed Hornberger M, Geng J, Hodges JR (2011) Convergent grey and white matter evidence of orbitofrontal cortex changes related to disinhibition in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain 134:2502–2512CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Whitwell JL, Avula R, Senjem ML et al (2010) Gray and white matter water diffusion in the syndromic variants of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 74:1279–1287CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Whitwell JL, Avula R, Senjem ML et al (2010) Gray and white matter water diffusion in the syndromic variants of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 74:1279–1287CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Lu PH, Lee GJ, Shapira J et al (2014) Regional differences in white matter breakdown between frontotemporal dementia and early-onset Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 39:261–269PubMedPubMedCentral Lu PH, Lee GJ, Shapira J et al (2014) Regional differences in white matter breakdown between frontotemporal dementia and early-onset Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 39:261–269PubMedPubMedCentral
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Tartaglia MC, Zhang Y, Racine C et al (2012) Executive dysfunction in frontotemporal dementia is related to abnormalities in frontal white matter tracts. J Neurol 259:1071–1080CrossRefPubMed Tartaglia MC, Zhang Y, Racine C et al (2012) Executive dysfunction in frontotemporal dementia is related to abnormalities in frontal white matter tracts. J Neurol 259:1071–1080CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Song S-K, Sun S-W, Ramsbottom MJ et al (2002) Dysmyelination revealed through MRI as increased radial (but unchanged axial) diffusion of water. Neuroimage 17:1429–1436CrossRefPubMed Song S-K, Sun S-W, Ramsbottom MJ et al (2002) Dysmyelination revealed through MRI as increased radial (but unchanged axial) diffusion of water. Neuroimage 17:1429–1436CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Song S-K, Sun S-W, Ju W-K et al (2003) Diffusion tensor imaging detects and differentiates axon and myelin degeneration in mouse optic nerve after retinal ischemia. Neuroimage 20:1714–1722CrossRefPubMed Song S-K, Sun S-W, Ju W-K et al (2003) Diffusion tensor imaging detects and differentiates axon and myelin degeneration in mouse optic nerve after retinal ischemia. Neuroimage 20:1714–1722CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Damoiseaux JS, Greicius MD (2009) Greater than the sum of its parts: a review of studies combining structural connectivity and resting-state functional connectivity. Brain Struct Funct 213:525–533CrossRefPubMed Damoiseaux JS, Greicius MD (2009) Greater than the sum of its parts: a review of studies combining structural connectivity and resting-state functional connectivity. Brain Struct Funct 213:525–533CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Greicius MD, Supekar K, Menon V et al (2009) Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cereb Cortex 19:72–78CrossRefPubMed Greicius MD, Supekar K, Menon V et al (2009) Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cereb Cortex 19:72–78CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Honey CJ, Sporns O, Cammoun L, et al. Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity. 2009;106:1–6. Honey CJ, Sporns O, Cammoun L, et al. Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity. 2009;106:1–6.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Weiler M, de Campos BM, Nogueira MH et al (2014) Structural connectivity of the default mode network and cognition in Alzheimer׳s disease. Psychiatry Res 223:15–22CrossRefPubMed Weiler M, de Campos BM, Nogueira MH et al (2014) Structural connectivity of the default mode network and cognition in Alzheimer׳s disease. Psychiatry Res 223:15–22CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Weissman-Fogel I, Moayedi M, Taylor KS et al (2010) Cognitive and default-mode resting state networks: do male and female brains ‘rest’ differently? Hum Brain Mapp 31:1713–1726PubMed Weissman-Fogel I, Moayedi M, Taylor KS et al (2010) Cognitive and default-mode resting state networks: do male and female brains ‘rest’ differently? Hum Brain Mapp 31:1713–1726PubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Gong G, He Y, Evans AC (2011) Brain connectivity: gender makes a difference. Neuroscientist 17:575–591CrossRefPubMed Gong G, He Y, Evans AC (2011) Brain connectivity: gender makes a difference. Neuroscientist 17:575–591CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Jovicich J, Marizzoni M, Bosch B et al (2014) Multisite longitudinal reliability of tract-based spatial statistics in diffusion tensor imaging of healthy elderly subjects. Neuroimage 101:390–403CrossRefPubMed Jovicich J, Marizzoni M, Bosch B et al (2014) Multisite longitudinal reliability of tract-based spatial statistics in diffusion tensor imaging of healthy elderly subjects. Neuroimage 101:390–403CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Functional connectivity and microstructural white matter changes in phenocopy frontotemporal dementia
verfasst von
R. Meijboom
R. M. E. Steketee
I. de Koning
R. J. Osse
L. C. Jiskoot
F. J. de Jong
A. van der Lugt
J. C. van Swieten
M. Smits
Publikationsdatum
19.07.2016
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Radiology / Ausgabe 4/2017
Print ISSN: 0938-7994
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-1084
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00330-016-4490-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2017

European Radiology 4/2017 Zur Ausgabe

Update Radiologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.