Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Infectious Diseases 1/2021

Open Access 01.12.2021 | Research article

How aware are swingers about their swing sex partners’ risk behaviours, and sexually transmitted infection status?

verfasst von: Anne-Marie Niekamp, Laura W. L. Spauwen, Nicole H. T. M. Dukers-Muijrers, Christian J. P. A. Hoebe

Erschienen in: BMC Infectious Diseases | Ausgabe 1/2021

Abstract

Background

Swingers are members of a heterosexual couple who, as a couple, have sex with others. They constitute a hidden subpopulation that is at risk for sexually transmitted infections (STIs). This study aimed to determine swingers’ level of awareness about the STI risk (indicators: bisexual behaviour, number of sex partners, and STI status) of their swing sex partners (i.e. alters).

Methods

In this cross-sectional study, data were collected from a convenience sample of swingers who visited our STI clinic. The sample consisted of 70 participants (i.e. egos) and their 299 swing sex partners (i.e. alters) who had undergone an STI test at our clinic. We compared network data (i.e. information that egos provided about alters) and data stored in the electronic patient record (EPR) in our clinic (i.e. information provided by alters themselves). We assessed the agreement (correct estimation, overestimation and underestimation) between the network data and EPR data using chi-squared tests.

Results

Egos underestimated the bisexual behaviours of 37% of their male alters and overestimated the number of sex partners of 54 and 68% of their male and female alters, respectively. Egos correctly estimated the STI statuses of only 22% of the alters who had an STI during the past six months.

Conclusions

The participating swingers underestimated the bisexual behaviours of their male swing sex partners, overestimated their number of sex partners, and underestimated their positive STI status. Underestimating their alters’ STI statuses can cause swingers to underestimate their own STI risk and fail to implement preventive measures. The latter finding has implications for STI prevention. Therefore, more attention should be paid to swingers in general and the promotion of actual partner notification and STI testing among swingers in specific.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12879-021-05813-5.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
EPR
Electronic patient record
HIV
Human immunodeficiency virus
STI
Sexually transmitted infection

Background

The transmission of sexually transmitted infections (STIs) is influenced by the sexual behaviours of individuals and their sex partners as well as their number of sex partners [1]. Furthermore, their perceptions of their sex partners’ risk behaviours (perceived or estimated risk) influence their implementation of preventive measures [24]. For STI prevention to be effective, estimated risk should correspond to actual risk. Therefore, researchers in the field of STI transmission and care are interested in ascertaining the extent to which individuals possess accurate information about the sexual behaviours of their sex partners (i.e. both regular and casual). However, it is challenging to collect data about sexual behaviours because this data is vulnerable to biases (e.g. recall and social desirability bias) [5, 6]. Collecting such data about one’s sex partners is even more challenging.
Several studies have evaluated the accuracy of the information that individuals provide about the sexual behaviours or STI statuses of their sex partners [2, 3, 720]. These studies examined the level of agreement between two types of behaviours. Some studies assessed behaviours and risk factors that were shared by a couple (e.g. frequency of having sex together, condom use) and reported good agreement between partners [3, 9, 10, 1620]. Other studies assessed behaviours and risk factors that a couple did not necessarily share (e.g. concurrency of sex partners, HIV status) [2, 3, 713, 15, 20]. While most of the latter studies examined heterosexual couples in committed relationships, they reported low agreement between partners. To the best of our knowledge, no such study has been conducted among swingers.
Swingers are members of heterosexual couples in committed relationships who, as a couple, have sex with other couples and/or singles. They constitute a hidden sexual subpopulation that is at risk for STIs [2128]. Our study aimed to determine the degree to which swingers are aware of their swing sex partners’ STI risk. We identified indicators of STI risk that are relevant to the swinger population. Couples may not share these risk factors or engage in such risk behaviours together. They were as follows: bisexual behaviour, number of sex partners, and STI status. Although swingers are members of heterosexual couples and identify as heterosexual, many of them engage in same-sex sexual behaviours; therefore, they are bisexual by behaviour [25, 28]. In particular, same-sex sexual behaviours between men are related to a high prevalence of STIs. Further, as implied by the definition, swingers have multiple sexual partners [28]. In a past study, we found that partner notification was the norm among a majority of the participating swingers [28]. Therefore, we hypothesised that most swingers would be aware of each other’s STI statuses.
To address the aim of this study, we examined dyads that consisted of a swinger and his/her casual swing sex partner (i.e. not the partner with whom he/she shared a committed relationship). In this article, we refer to swing sex partners as alters to differentiate them from sex partners in general. The participating swingers are referred to as egos. We examined the level of agreement between ego-estimated and self-reported STI risk among alters. The following risk factors were assessed: bisexual behaviour, number of sex partners, and STI status.

Methods

Study population

The cross-sectional data used in this study were collected as a part of a prospective cohort network study of swingers (i.e. the SWAP study) [21]. A convenience sample of swingers who visited our STI clinic in the south of the Netherlands between 2009 and 2012 constituted the group of egos. Clinic attendees who were self-reported swingers as per the aforementioned definition were eligible for inclusion.
The questions that were used to assess the outcome variables of this study were posed to only a subset of the sample of the cohort study, the first two waves of the cohort study in 2009 and 2010. Therefore, only the 140 egos who had responded to these questions were included in the sample. The sample was further restricted to egos and alters who met the inclusion criteria described in Fig. 1. Egos (n = 70) with at least one alter who met the inclusion criteria for alters were included. The primary reason for the exclusion of egos was the absence of their alters’ electronic patient records (EPRs) in our STI clinic (see Fig. 1).
We compared the subsets of included and excluded egos and alters on their sociodemographic characteristics, swing behaviours, and outcome variables to assess the generalisability of the findings. The results are presented in Additional file 1.

Data

We used two datasets in this study: self-administered questionnaire data and patient data retrieved from the EPR in our clinic. With regard to the first dataset, egos provided individual partner data (i.e. network data). We collected the egocentric network data using a name generator, i.e. a list of all their swing sex partners, in the past six months. For every alter in the name generator the egos completed a partner data questionnaire that assessed each of his or her altars’ characteristics, sexual behaviours, number of sex partners, type of swing relationship, and STI status during the past six months. They also completed a self-administered questionnaire that assessed their personal characteristics and sexual behaviours including their educational level, the number of years for which they had been swingers (i.e. swing duration), the frequency with which they engaged in swinging, swinging venues, their number of sex partners, sexual preferences, and drug or alcohol use during swinging.
The second dataset (i.e. patient data from the EPR), which was collected during STI consultations in our clinic, included information about alters’ self-reported characteristics, their behaviours, and STI test results. The actual STI diagnostic data of the egos were also extracted from their EPR.
To address the aim of this study, we compared the network and EPR data. The network and EPR data served as operationalisations of the ego-estimated and self-reported behaviours of alters, respectively.

Outcome variables

The outcome variables that were examined to assess the level of agreement between the network and EPR data were bisexual behaviour, number of sex partners, and STI status. These variables served as proxies for STI risk.
Heterosexual behaviour was defined as the act of having sex with only partners of the opposite sex. Bisexual behaviour was defined as the act of having sex with both opposite- and same-sex partners. Bisexual behaviour was considered to have been underestimated if heterosexual behaviour was reported in the network data and bisexual behaviour was reported in the EPR data.
With regard to both the network and EPR data, bisexual behaviour was measured by asking the participant whether the alter had had sex with only men, only women, or both men and women. A nurse registered participant responses to this question in the EPR after posing questions that assessed the different types of sex (including oral sex) that they had engaged in with both men and women. Depending on the gender of the alter, the response was coded as ‘heterosexual behaviour’ if the alter had had sex with only members of the opposite gender and as ‘bisexual behaviour’ if the alter had had sex with both women and men.
The number of sex partners was defined as the number of male and female sex partners that one had had during the past six months. A higher number of sex partners was considered to be indicative of higher STI risk. The number of sex partners was considered to have been underestimated if the network data yielded a lower figure than the EPR data did.
The following questionnaire item (i.e. network data) was used to measure egos’ estimations of the number of sex partners that their alters had: ‘Provide an estimation of the number of sex partners that you think [alter name] has had during the past 6 months’. They provided separate responses for their male and female alters. With regard to the EPR data, the following question was posed: ‘How many sex partners have you had during the past 6 months?’. There was no provision to record separate responses for their male and female sex partners.
Accuracy was considered to be less important for higher numbers of sex partners. In other words, STI risk would be higher among those with five sex partners than among those with only one sex partner. However, the difference in STI risk between those with 70 and 60 sex partners is unlikely to be significant. Therefore, we defined the overestimation and underestimation of the number of sex partners based on a range and estimation margin. Higher numbers of sex partners permitted a larger estimation margin. The number of sex partners was classified into the following ranges: < 10, 10–20, 20–50, and > 50. A 20% margin yielded maximum margins of 2, 4, 10, and 20, respectively.
The STI status was defined as positive if at least one STI was diagnosed during the past six months. Using the network data, the STI status of alters was classified as follows: the alter does not have an STI, the alter does have an STI, the ego was not aware of the alter’s STI status. We used STI test results that were retrieved from the EPR data to ascertain alters’ actual STI statuses. STI status was considered to have been underestimated if it was negative in the network data but positive in the EPR data.
The following questionnaire item (i.e. network data) was used to assess alters’ STI status: ‘Did this alter have an STI during the past 6 months?’. The egos could record their responses on a five-point scale that included the following response options: ‘I know for sure that he did not have an STI’, ‘I presume that he did not have an STI’, ‘I don’t know if he had an STI’, ‘I presume that he had an STI’, and ‘I am sure he had an STI’. These five response options were collapsed into the three aforementioned estimations. The first two response options were combined into the estimation that the alter did not have an STI, and the last two response options were combined into the estimation that the alter did have an STI. The remaining response stated that the ego did not know the alter’s STI status.
STI test results across the preceding seven months were extracted from the EPR dataset. They were summed to ascertain the STI status during the past six months. To determine the STI status, we chose to include a one-month window period to account for a delay of two or three weeks between STI testing and the delivery of results. The STI tests included regular screening for Chlamydia trachomatis, Neisseria gonorrhoeae, Treponema pallidum (syphilis), human immunodeficiency virus (HIV), and hepatitis B virus, as well as clinically confirmed diagnoses of condylomata acuminata and herpes genitalis. A STI status was defined positive if at least one of the aforementioned STIs was diagnosed during the past six months.
With regard to the network data, the response, ‘I don’t know’, to any question that assessed the outcome variables was considered to be a lack of response and classified into a separate category. In the tables presented in this article, continuous variables (e.g. age, swinging duration and frequency, number of sex partners) are dichotomised based on their median.

Statistical analysis

We compared the network and EPR data to ascertain the level of agreement with regard to the three outcome variables at the ego-alter dyad level. Chi-squared test was used to assess the level of agreement between the ego-estimated and self-reported behaviours of alters. The data were stratified by the gender of the egos and alters because there were significant differences in the bisexual behaviours of male and female alters (see Tables 1 and 2). All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS version 24 for Mac (IBM Inc., Somers, NY, USA).
Table 1
Outcome variables derived from the network data and EPR data stratified by gender
 
Estimation given by egos
EPR alter
 
Male egos (n = 135)
Female egos (n = 164)
Male egos
Female egos
 
Male alters (n = 38)
Female alters (n = 97)
 
Male alters (n = 85)
Female alters (n = 79)
 
Male alters (n = 38)
Female alters (n = 97)
 
Male alters (n = 85)
Female alters (n = 79)
 
 
n
% (95%CI)
n
% (95%CI)
p#
n
% (95%CI)
n
% (95%CI)
p#
n
% (95%CI)
n
% (95%CI)
p#
n
% (95%CI)
n
% (95%CI)
p#
Bisexuality
    
**
    
**
    
**
    
**
 Heterosexual
20
52.6 (36.7–68.5)
5
5.2 (0.8–9.6)
< 0.001
55
64.7 (54.5–74.9)
6
7.6 (1.8–13.4)
< 0.001
11
28.9 (14.5–43.3)
0
0 (0.0–0.0)
< 0.001
31
36.5 (26.3–46.7)
0
0,0 (0.0–0,0)
< 0.001
 Bisexual
18
47.4 (31.5–63.3)
92
94.8 (90.3–99.2)
 
25
29.4 (19.7–39.1)
73
92.4 (86.6–98.2)
 
27
71.1 (56.7–85.5)
97
100,0 (100.0–100.0)
 
54
63.5 (53.3–73.7)
79
100,0 (100.0–100.0)
 
 Don’t know
0
0,0 (0.0–0.0)
0
0,0 (0.0–0.0)
 
5
5.9 (0.9–10.9)
0
0.0 (0.0–0,0)
           
Sex partners in past 6 months
  < 14 partners
13
34.2 (19.1–49.2)
23
23.7 (15.2–32.2)
0.462
35
41.2 (30.7–51.7)
19
24.1 (14.7–33.5)
0.054
26
68.4 (53.6–83.1)
69
71.1 (62.1–80.1)
0.827
63
74.1 (64.8–83.4)
58
73.4 (63.7–83.1)
0.937
  ≥ 14 partners
9
23.7 (10.2–37.2)
26
26.8 (18.0–35.6)
 
22
25.9 (16.6–35.2)
30
38.0 (27.3–48.7)
 
11
28.9 (14.5–43.3)
24
24.7 (16.1–33.3)
 
18
21.2 (12.5–29.9)
18
22.8 (13.5–32.1)
 
 Don’t know
16
42.1 (26.4–57.8)
48
49.5 (39.6–59.5)
 
28
32.9 (22.9–42.9)
30
38.0 (27.3–48.7)
 
1
2.6 (−2.5–7.7)
4
4.1 (0.2–8.0)
 
4
4.7 (0.2–9.2)
3
3.8 (−0.4–8.0)
 
STI status
         
**
          
 No
12
31.6 (16.8–46.4)
46
47.4 (37.5–57.3)
0.114
62
72.9 (63.5–82.3)
33
41.8 (27.3–48.7)
< 0.001
27
71.1 (56.7–85.5)
77
79.4 (71.4–87.4)
0.301
69
81.2 (72.9–89.6)
66
83.5 (75.3–91.7)
0.691
 Yes
4
10.5 (0.8–20.2)
14
14.4 (7.4–21.4)
 
5
5.9 (0.9–10.9)
4
5.1 (0.2–10,0)
 
11
28.9 (14.5–43.3)
20
20.6 (12.5–28.6)
 
16
18.8 (10.5–27.1)
13
16.5 (8.3–24.7)
 
 Don’t know
22
57.9 (42.2–73.6)
37
38.1 (28.4–47.8)
 
18
21.2 (12.5–29.9)
42
53.2 (42.2–64.2)
           
# p values (χ2-test)
** = p < 0.01, indicates significant differences between males and females
Table 2
Agreement measures for the outcome variables at the dyadic level, stratified by gender
 
Male egos (n = 135)
Female egos (n = 164)
Total participants (n = 299)
 
Male alters (n = 38)
Female alters (n = 97)
 
Male alters (n = 85)
Female alters (n = 79)
 
Male alters (n = 123)
Female alters (n = 176)
 
 
n
% (95%CI)
n
% (95%CI)
p#
n
% (95%CI)
n
% (95%CI)
p#
n
% (95%CI)
n
% (95%CI)
p#
Bisexuality
    
**
    
**
    
**
 Underestimation (bisexuality estimated as heterosexuality)
11
28.9 (14.5–43.3)
5
5.2  (0.8–9.6)
< 0.001
34
40.0 (29.6–50.4)
6
7.6 (1.8–13.4)
< 0.001
45
36.6 (28.1–45.1)
11
6.3  (2.7–9.9)
< 0.001
 Correctly estimated
25
65.8 (50.7–80.9
90
92.8 (87.7–97.9)
 
40
47.1 (36.5–57.7)
71
89.9 (83.3–96.5)
 
65
52.8 (44.0–61.6)
161
91.5 (83.4–95.6)
 
 Overestimation (heterosexuality estimated as bisexuality)
2
5.3 (−1.8–12.4)
2
2.1 (−0,8–5.0)
 
6
7.1 (1.6–12.6)
2
2.5 (−0.9–5.9)
 
8
6.5 (2.1–10.9)
4
2.3  (0.8–4.5)
 
 No estimation
0
0,0  (0.0–0.0)
0
0,0  (0,0-0,0)
 
5
5.9 (0.9–10.9)
0
0,0 ( 0.0–0.0)
 
5
4.1  (0.6–7.6)
0
0.0 ( 0.0–0.0)
 
Number of sex partners
 Underestimation (number of sex partners estimated too low)
7
18.4 (6.1–30.7)
10
10.3 (4.3–16.3)
0.205
11
13.3 (6.1–20.5)
6
7.6 (1.8–13.4)
0.599
18
14.9 (8.6–2.1)
16
9.1 (4.9–13.3)
0.121
 Correctly estimated
5
13.2 (2.4–23.9)
5
5.2  (0.8–9.6)
 
11
13.3 (6.1–20.5)
8
10.3 (3.6–17.0)
 
16
13.2 (7.2–19.2)
13
7.5 (3.6–11.4)
 
 Overestimation (number of sex partners estimated too high)
10
26.3 (12.3–40.3)
33
34.4 (24.9–43.9)
 
33
39.8 (29.4–50.2)
34
43.6 (32.7–54.5)
 
43
35.5 (27.0–44.0)
67
38.5 (31.3–45.7)
 
 No estimation
16
42.1 (26.4–57.8)
48
50.0 (40.0–60.0)
 
28
33.7 (23.7–43.7)
30
38.5 (27.8–49.2)
 
44
36.4 (27.9–44.9)
78
44.8 (37.5–52.1)
 
STI status
         
**
    
*
 Underestimation (STI positive, estimated STI negative)
1
2.6 (−2.5–7.7)
10
10.3 (4.3–16.3)
0.258
7
8.2 (2.4–14.0)
6
7.6 (1.8–13.4)
< 0.001
8
6.5 (2.1–10.9)
16
9.1 (4.9–13.3)
0.046
 Correctly estimated STI positive
2
5.3 (−1.8–12.4)
6
6.2 (1.4–11.0)
 
3
3.5 (−0.4–7.4)
2
2.5 (−0.9–5.9)
 
5
4.1  (0.6–7.6)
8
4.5  (1.4–7.6)
 
 Correctly estimated STI negative
11
28.9 (14.5–43.3)
36
37.1 (27.5–46.7)
 
55
64.7 (54.5–74-9)
27
34.2 (23.7–44.7)
 
66
53.7 (44.9–62.5)
63
35.8 (28.7–42.9)
 
 Overestimation (STI negative, estimated STI positive)
2
5.3 (−1.8–12.4)
8
8.2 (2.7–13.7)
 
2
2.4 (−0.9–5.7)
2
2.5 (− 0.9–5.9)
 
4
3.3  (0.1–6.5)
10
5.7  (2.3–9.1)
 
 No estimation
22
57.9 (42.2–73.6)
37
38.1 (28.4–47.8)
 
18
21.2 (12.5–29.9)
42
53.2 (42.2–64.2)
 
40
32.5 (24.2–40.8)
79
44.9 (37.6–52.2)
 
# p values (χ2-test)
* = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, indicates significant differences between males and females

Ethics

Medical ethical approval to conduct the SWAP study was granted by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Maastricht University Medical Centre. As required by this committee, written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to data collection. Medical ethics approval to use the coded EPR data was granted by the same committee. As approved by the committee, all attendees of our clinic gave permission by opt-out consent. A medical professional in the clinic assigned codes to the swingers and linked them to the datasets before anonymising them. We performed the data analysis in a fully anonymised and de-identified manner.

Results

Egos

Of the 70 egos, 54 and 46% of them were women and men, respectively. Their median age was 43 years (range = 27–60). Their swinging frequency over the past six months ranged from 1 to 50 (median = 10), and they had had a median of 14 sex partners (range = 1–83) during the past six months. Table A in Additional file 1 summarises the demographic characteristics, swinging behaviours, and STI statuses of the egos. Nine egos (13%) were diagnosed with an STI in the past six months: 7 (10%) with Chlamydia trachomatis and 2 (2.9%) with Neisseria gonorrhoeae. All egos were tested negative for HIV, Treponema pallidum (syphilis) and Hepatitis B. Condylomata acuminata and Herpes genitalis were not diagnosed.

Alters

Of the 299 alters, 59 and 41% were women and men, respectively. Their median age was 44 years (range = 21–60). Table B in Additional file 1 summarises their characteristics and swinging behaviours as perceived by the egos. Table 1 presents the three outcome variables (i.e. bisexual behaviour, number of sex partners, and STI status) derived from the network and EPR data. Sixty (20%) of the alters were diagnosed with an STI in the past six months: 23 (7.7%) with Chlamydia Trachomatis, 29 (10%) with Neisseria gonorrhoeae, 11 (3.7%) with Condylomata acuminata and 1 (0.3%) with Herpes genitalis. All alters were tested negative for HIV, Treponema pallidum (syphilis) and Hepatitis B. Table 2 presents the level of agreement at the dyadic level.

Alters’ bisexual behaviour

Both male and female egos were significantly better at estimating the bisexual behaviours of female alters than male alters (p < 0.01). Egos underestimated the bisexual behaviours of 37% of their male alters. Male egos underestimated the bisexual behaviours of 29% of their male alters.

Alters’ number of sex partners

Agreement with regard to the number of sex partners did not differ significantly between male and female alters. Analysis of the network data revealed that the alters had had a median of 13 sex partners (range = 0–230) during the past six months. In contrast, analysis of the EPR data revealed that they had had a median of 8 sex partners (range = 2–270) during the past six months. The median discrepancy in the number of sex partners was 7 (range = 0–215). Overall, when egos did provide an estimation, they overestimated the number of sex partners of 54 and 68% of male and female alters, respectively.

Alters’ STI status

Agreement with regard to alters’ STI status differed significantly between male and female alters. When an estimation was provided, the STI status of female alters was more frequently underestimated (17%) than that of male alters (9.7%). The negative STI status of male alters (80%) was more frequently correctly estimated than that of female alters (65%). Egos underestimated the STI status of 40% of the 60 alters who had been diagnosed with an STI during the past six months, correctly estimated the STI status of 22% of these alters, and provided no estimation for 38% of these alters. Overall, when they did provide an estimation, they underestimated the positive STI status of 62% of their male alters and 67% of their female alters.

Discussion

In this study, we adopted a network approach to examine how accurately swingers could estimate the sexual behaviour, number of sex partners, and STI status (i.e. risk factors for STIs) of their swing sex partners. The main findings of this study are that egos underestimated the bisexual behaviour of male alters, overestimated the number of alters’ sex partners, and underestimated their positive STI status.
Although a majority of egos correctly estimated the bisexual behaviour of their alters, a sizeable proportion of them underestimated the bisexual behaviour of male alters. When compared to past findings, lower percentages of the underestimation of the bisexual behaviour of male alters emerged in this study. Ellen et al. [7] found that 86% of women underestimated their male sex partner having sex with men. In comparison, only 47% of the female egos in this study underestimated the bisexual behaviour of their male alters. Surprisingly, male egos estimated that almost one-third of their male alters had sex only with women. We expected egos to estimate that 100% of their same-sex alters would engage in bisexual behaviour because these alters were their sex partners. A possible explanation for this discrepancy is that swingers often do not regard oral sex as sex.
In this study, rates of agreement regarding the number of sex partners that alters had was low. However, in contradistinction to past findings, [2] egos overestimated the number of sex partners that their alters had: Specifically, they overestimated the number of partners of 54 and 68% of their male and female alters, respectively. This overestimation of the number of sex partners may be attributable to underreporting in the EPR. Collecting data about sexual behaviours (e.g. our EPR data) is vulnerable to biases and can result in the underreporting of the number of sex partners that one has [5, 6]. Thus, if too low numbers of sex partners were recorded in the EPR data, egos’ estimations may have been accurate estimations instead of overestimations.
A majority of the egos estimated their alters’ STI statuses correctly (as most alters tested negative) but underestimated their positive STI status. They correctly estimated the STI statuses of only 22% of STI-positive alters. Our findings mirror past observations [12]. However, in this study, swingers were slightly more accurate at estimating the STI statuses of their STI-positive alters.
Past studies [2, 3, 713, 15] that have examined the level of agreement between sex partners’ sexual behaviours have reported low rates of agreement for behaviours that a couple does not engage in together. Only a few studies [2] have examined the same risk behaviours, as we did among swingers. In contradistinction to these findings, swingers estimated alters’ bisexual behaviours and number of sex partners rather accurately. A possible explanation is that swingers generally swing at clubs and house parties, [21] where they can openly observe the sexual behaviours of their swing sex partners. However, the stigma surrounding STIs and reluctance to notify one’s partners can contribute to the underestimation of a positive STI status.
The conclusion that swingers underestimate the positive STI status of their alters has implications for STI prevention. In a past study [28] that was conducted within the same population of swingers, STI testing and partner notification emerged as the norm among a majority of the participants. Therefore, relatively accurate estimations of alters’ positive STI statuses were expected. Most egos should have been notified by their alters, as half of them had gone on more than one date with their alters and had shared a swing relationship that exceeded the period of partner notification for most STIs (see Table B in Additional file 1). Thus, swingers’ intentions to notify their partners about an STI diagnosis [28] does not seem to necessarily lead to an actual notification. This suggests that infected swingers find it difficult to notify their sex partners about an STI diagnosis.
Swingers’ tendency to underestimate their alters’ positive STI statuses can cause them to underestimate their own STI risk. Several studies [2, 8, 12, 29, 30] have reported a similar tendency to underestimate STI risk and found that it negatively influences STI testing behaviours and the adoption of preventive measures [2, 29]. Therefore, our findings have important implications for STI prevention. Further attention should be paid to proper partner notification and the promotion of STI testing among swingers.
Our study is unique because it focused on a hidden population: swingers. Further, we compared the behaviours of casual swing sex partners rather than couples in committed relationships. These swing sex partnerships characterise the swinger population. However, our study also has several limitations. First, the findings may not be generalisable to all swingers because the study used a convenience sample of swingers who visited our clinic [5]. Evidently, swingers who visit an STI clinic to be tested will be more aware of their STI risk than the general swinger population. Moreover, we included only a third of the alters and half of the egos in our sample (see Fig. 1). It is possible that some differences in the outcomes were present but not statistically significant in our study due to the current sample size of our convenience sample. The main reason for the exclusion of egos and alters was that many alters were not or could not be identified as clients of our clinic. A comparison of the included and excluded alters (see Additional file 1) revealed that the egos and included alters had gone on more dates together and had been in swing relationships for longer durations. These findings lead one to expect that egos would be more aware of the STI risk of their included alters. However, logistic regression analysis (data not presented) revealed that there was no significant relation between these variables and the outcome variables.
Second, the outcome variables were estimated within a six-month temporal framework. Thus, as noted earlier, recall biases may adversely influence the information contained in both datasets. This limitation may be attributable to the fact that many egos did not provide an estimation of alters’ number of sex partners and STI status. Furthermore, if an alter had contracted an STI and received treatment prior to his/her first swing date with an ego, then the underestimation of a positive STI status may have actually been correct. Because a majority of the egos and alters had gone on more than one date and had been in a swing relationship for more than one year, these underestimations are unlikely to be erroneous.
Finally, the data used in this study were collected between 2009 and 2010. Nevertheless, there is no reason to expect that swingers’ levels of awareness about their alters’ behaviours would have significantly changed since the time of data collection. There is no STI prevention program for swingers in the Netherlands. Therefore, the findings of this study are unlikely to have been adversely influenced by the aforementioned limitation.

Conclusion

Swingers underestimated the bisexual behaviours of their male swing sex partners, overestimated the number of alters’ sex partners, and underestimated their positive STI status. By underestimating the positive STI statuses of their partners, swingers may underestimate their own STI risk and fail to implement preventive measures. The latter finding has important implications for STI prevention. Tailored interventions for swingers that include the promotion of STI testing and actual partner notification are needed for this hidden sexual subpopulation.

Acknowledgements

The authors like to thank the staff of the Centre for Sexual Health of the South Limburg Public Health Service for recruiting study participants and the participants for their willingness to provide information on all the private aspects of their swinging lifestyle and sexual behaviour. We thank Marita Werner and Henriette ter Waarbeek for their helpful comments on earlier versions of this paper.
Medical ethical approval to conduct the SWAP study was granted by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Maastricht University Medical Centre (Reference: NL23703.068.08). As required by this committee, written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to data collection. Medical ethics approval to use the coded EPR data was granted by the same committee (Reference 11-4-108). As approved by the committee, all attendees of our clinic gave permission by opt-out consent. A medical professional in the clinic assigned codes to the swingers and linked them to the datasets before anonymising them. We performed the data analysis in a fully anonymised and de-identified manner.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

Dr. Nicole HTM Dukers-Muijrers is a member of the editorial board of BMC Infectious Diseases. She declares to have no further competing interests. All other authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris M, Kretzschmar M. Concurrent partnerships and the spread of HIV. AIDS. 1997;11:641–8.CrossRef Morris M, Kretzschmar M. Concurrent partnerships and the spread of HIV. AIDS. 1997;11:641–8.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Stoner BP, Whittington WLH, Aral SO, Hughes JP, Handsfield HH, Holmes KK. Avoiding risky sex partners: perception of partners“ risks v partners” self reported risks. Sex Transm Infect. 2003;79:197–201.CrossRef Stoner BP, Whittington WLH, Aral SO, Hughes JP, Handsfield HH, Holmes KK. Avoiding risky sex partners: perception of partners“ risks v partners” self reported risks. Sex Transm Infect. 2003;79:197–201.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Drumright LN, Gorbach PM, Holmes KK. Do people really know their sex partners? Concurrency, knowledge of partner behavior, and sexually transmitted infections within partnerships. Sex Transm Dis. 2004;31:437–42.CrossRef Drumright LN, Gorbach PM, Holmes KK. Do people really know their sex partners? Concurrency, knowledge of partner behavior, and sexually transmitted infections within partnerships. Sex Transm Dis. 2004;31:437–42.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Riehman KS, Wechsberg WM, Francis SA, Moore M, Morgan-Lopez A. Discordance in monogamy beliefs, sexual concurrency, and condom use among young adult substance-involved couples: implications for risk of sexually transmitted infections. Sex Transm Dis. 2006;33:677–82.CrossRef Riehman KS, Wechsberg WM, Francis SA, Moore M, Morgan-Lopez A. Discordance in monogamy beliefs, sexual concurrency, and condom use among young adult substance-involved couples: implications for risk of sexually transmitted infections. Sex Transm Dis. 2006;33:677–82.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat McCallum EB, Peterson ZD. Investigating the impact of inquiry mode on self-reported sexual behavior: theoretical considerations and review of the literature. J Sex Res. 2012;49:212–26.CrossRef McCallum EB, Peterson ZD. Investigating the impact of inquiry mode on self-reported sexual behavior: theoretical considerations and review of the literature. J Sex Res. 2012;49:212–26.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Schroder KEE, Carey MP, Vanable PA. Methodological challenges in research on sexual risk behavior: II. Accuracy of self-reports. Ann Behav Med. 2003;26:104–23.CrossRef Schroder KEE, Carey MP, Vanable PA. Methodological challenges in research on sexual risk behavior: II. Accuracy of self-reports. Ann Behav Med. 2003;26:104–23.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Ellen JM, Vittinghoff E, Bolan G, Boyer CB, Padian NS. Individuals“ perceptions about their sex partners” risk behaviors. J Sex Res. 1998;35:328–32.CrossRef Ellen JM, Vittinghoff E, Bolan G, Boyer CB, Padian NS. Individuals“ perceptions about their sex partners” risk behaviors. J Sex Res. 1998;35:328–32.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Niccolai LM, Farley TA, Ayoub MA, Magnus M, Kissinger PJ. HIV-infected persons“ knowledge of their sexual partners” HIV status. AIDS Educ Prev. 2002;14:183–9.CrossRef Niccolai LM, Farley TA, Ayoub MA, Magnus M, Kissinger PJ. HIV-infected persons“ knowledge of their sexual partners” HIV status. AIDS Educ Prev. 2002;14:183–9.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Lenoir CD, Adler NE, Borzekowski DLG, Tschann JM, Ellen JM. What you don’t know can hurt you: Perceptions of sex-partner concurrency and partner-reported behavior. Journal of Adolescent Health 2006;38:179–85. Lenoir CD, Adler NE, Borzekowski DLG, Tschann JM, Ellen JM. What you don’t know can hurt you: Perceptions of sex-partner concurrency and partner-reported behavior. Journal of Adolescent Health 2006;38:179–85.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Koniak-Griffin D, Huang R, Lesser J, González-Figueroa E, Takayanagi S, Cumberland WG. Young Parents’ Relationship Characteristics, Shared Sexual Behaviors, Perception of Partner Risks, and Dyadic Influences. J Sex Res. 2009;46:483–93.CrossRef Koniak-Griffin D, Huang R, Lesser J, González-Figueroa E, Takayanagi S, Cumberland WG. Young Parents’ Relationship Characteristics, Shared Sexual Behaviors, Perception of Partner Risks, and Dyadic Influences. J Sex Res. 2009;46:483–93.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Anglewicz PA, Bignami-Van Assche S, Clark S, Mkandawire J. HIV risk among currently married couples in rural Malawi: what do spouses know about each other? AIDS Behav. 2010;14:103–12.CrossRef Anglewicz PA, Bignami-Van Assche S, Clark S, Mkandawire J. HIV risk among currently married couples in rural Malawi: what do spouses know about each other? AIDS Behav. 2010;14:103–12.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Witte SS, El-Bassel N, Gilbert L, Wu E, Chang M. Lack of awareness of partner STD risk among heterosexual couples. Perspect Sex Reprod Health. 2010;42:49–55.CrossRef Witte SS, El-Bassel N, Gilbert L, Wu E, Chang M. Lack of awareness of partner STD risk among heterosexual couples. Perspect Sex Reprod Health. 2010;42:49–55.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Swartzendruber A, Niccolai LM, Jennings JM, Zenilman JM, Divney AA, Magriples U, et al. Perceptions about sexual concurrency and factors related to inaccurate perceptions among pregnant adolescents and their partners. Sex Transm Dis. 2012;39:577–82.CrossRef Swartzendruber A, Niccolai LM, Jennings JM, Zenilman JM, Divney AA, Magriples U, et al. Perceptions about sexual concurrency and factors related to inaccurate perceptions among pregnant adolescents and their partners. Sex Transm Dis. 2012;39:577–82.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Grey JA, Rothenberg R, Sullivan PS, Rosenberg ES. Racial Differences in the Accuracy of Perceived Partner HIV Status among Men who have Sex with Men (MSM) in Atlanta, Georgia. J Int Assoc Providers AIDS Care. 2015;14:26–32.CrossRef Grey JA, Rothenberg R, Sullivan PS, Rosenberg ES. Racial Differences in the Accuracy of Perceived Partner HIV Status among Men who have Sex with Men (MSM) in Atlanta, Georgia. J Int Assoc Providers AIDS Care. 2015;14:26–32.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Kenyon CR, Kirungi W, Kaharuza F, Buyze J, Bunnell R. Who knows their Partner's HIV status? Results from a nationally representative survey in Uganda. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2015;69:92–7.CrossRef Kenyon CR, Kirungi W, Kaharuza F, Buyze J, Bunnell R. Who knows their Partner's HIV status? Results from a nationally representative survey in Uganda. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2015;69:92–7.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanchez DM, Schoenbach VJ, Harvey SM, Warren JT, Poole C, Leone PA, et al. Can young adults accurately report sexual partnership dates? Factors associated with Interpartner and dyad agreement. Sex Transm Dis. 2016;43:324–31.CrossRef Sanchez DM, Schoenbach VJ, Harvey SM, Warren JT, Poole C, Leone PA, et al. Can young adults accurately report sexual partnership dates? Factors associated with Interpartner and dyad agreement. Sex Transm Dis. 2016;43:324–31.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Ellish NJ, Weisman CS, Celentano D, Zenilman JM. Reliability of partner reports of sexual history in a heterosexual population at a sexually transmitted diseases clinic. Sex Transm Dis. 1996;23:446–52.CrossRef Ellish NJ, Weisman CS, Celentano D, Zenilman JM. Reliability of partner reports of sexual history in a heterosexual population at a sexually transmitted diseases clinic. Sex Transm Dis. 1996;23:446–52.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Padian NS, Aral S, Vranizan K, Bolan G. Reliability of sexual histories in heterosexual couples. Sex Transm Dis. 1995;22:169–72.CrossRef Padian NS, Aral S, Vranizan K, Bolan G. Reliability of sexual histories in heterosexual couples. Sex Transm Dis. 1995;22:169–72.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Upchurch DM, Weisman CS, Shepherd M, Brookmeyer R, Fox R, Celentano DD, et al. Interpartner reliability of reporting of recent sexual behaviors. Am J Epidemiol. 1991;134:1159–66.CrossRef Upchurch DM, Weisman CS, Shepherd M, Brookmeyer R, Fox R, Celentano DD, et al. Interpartner reliability of reporting of recent sexual behaviors. Am J Epidemiol. 1991;134:1159–66.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Seal DW. Interpartner concordance of self-reported sexual behavior among college dating couples. J Sex Res. 1997;34:39–55.CrossRef Seal DW. Interpartner concordance of self-reported sexual behavior among college dating couples. J Sex Res. 1997;34:39–55.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Niekamp A-M, Mercken LAG, Hoebe CJPA, Dukers-Muijrers NHTM. A sexual affiliation network of swingers, heterosexuals practicing risk behaviours that potentiate the spread of sexually transmitted infections: a two-mode approach. Soc Networks. 2013;35:223–36.CrossRef Niekamp A-M, Mercken LAG, Hoebe CJPA, Dukers-Muijrers NHTM. A sexual affiliation network of swingers, heterosexuals practicing risk behaviours that potentiate the spread of sexually transmitted infections: a two-mode approach. Soc Networks. 2013;35:223–36.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Dukers-Muijrers NHTM, Niekamp A-M, Brouwers EEHG, Hoebe CJPA. Older and swinging; need to identify hidden and emerging risk groups at STI clinics. Sex Transm Infect. 2010;86:315–7.CrossRef Dukers-Muijrers NHTM, Niekamp A-M, Brouwers EEHG, Hoebe CJPA. Older and swinging; need to identify hidden and emerging risk groups at STI clinics. Sex Transm Infect. 2010;86:315–7.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Spauwen LWL, Niekamp A-M, Hoebe CJPA, Dukers-Muijrers NHTM. Drug use, sexual risk behaviour and sexually transmitted infections among swingers: a cross-sectional study in the Netherlands. Sex Transm Infect. 2015;91:31–6.CrossRef Spauwen LWL, Niekamp A-M, Hoebe CJPA, Dukers-Muijrers NHTM. Drug use, sexual risk behaviour and sexually transmitted infections among swingers: a cross-sectional study in the Netherlands. Sex Transm Infect. 2015;91:31–6.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat O'Byrne P, Watts JA. Exploring sexual networks: a pilot study of swingers' sexual behaviour and health-care-seeking practices. Can J Nurs Res. 2011;43:80–97.PubMed O'Byrne P, Watts JA. Exploring sexual networks: a pilot study of swingers' sexual behaviour and health-care-seeking practices. Can J Nurs Res. 2011;43:80–97.PubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Platteau T, van Lankveld J, Ooms L, Florence E. Sexual behavior and sexually transmitted infections among swingers: results from an online survey in Belgium. J Sex Marital Ther. 2016;36:1–11. Platteau T, van Lankveld J, Ooms L, Florence E. Sexual behavior and sexually transmitted infections among swingers: results from an online survey in Belgium. J Sex Marital Ther. 2016;36:1–11.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Dukers-Muijrers NHTM, van Rooijen MS, Hogewoning A, van Liere GAFS, Steenbakkers M, Hoebe CJPA. Incidence of repeat testing and diagnoses of chlamydia trachomatis and Neisseria gonorrhoea in swingers, homosexual and heterosexual men and women at two large Dutch STI clinics, 2006-2013. Sex Transm Infect. 2017;93:383–9.CrossRef Dukers-Muijrers NHTM, van Rooijen MS, Hogewoning A, van Liere GAFS, Steenbakkers M, Hoebe CJPA. Incidence of repeat testing and diagnoses of chlamydia trachomatis and Neisseria gonorrhoea in swingers, homosexual and heterosexual men and women at two large Dutch STI clinics, 2006-2013. Sex Transm Infect. 2017;93:383–9.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Mercer CH. Swinging: if you do not ask you may not find, but you need to. Sex Transm Infect. 2017;93:381–2.CrossRef Mercer CH. Swinging: if you do not ask you may not find, but you need to. Sex Transm Infect. 2017;93:381–2.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Spauwen LWL, Niekamp A-M, Hoebe CJPA, Dukers-Muijrers NHTM. Do swingers self-identify as swingers when attending STI services for testing? A cross-sectional study. Sex Transm Infect. 2018;94:559–61.CrossRef Spauwen LWL, Niekamp A-M, Hoebe CJPA, Dukers-Muijrers NHTM. Do swingers self-identify as swingers when attending STI services for testing? A cross-sectional study. Sex Transm Infect. 2018;94:559–61.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat ten Hoor GA, Ruiter RAC, van Bergen JEAM, Hoebe CJPA, Dukers-Muijrers NHTM, Kok G. Predictors of chlamydia trachomatis testing: perceived norms, susceptibility, changes in partner status, and underestimation of own risk. BMC Public Health. 2016;16:1–9. ten Hoor GA, Ruiter RAC, van Bergen JEAM, Hoebe CJPA, Dukers-Muijrers NHTM, Kok G. Predictors of chlamydia trachomatis testing: perceived norms, susceptibility, changes in partner status, and underestimation of own risk. BMC Public Health. 2016;16:1–9.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Ethier KA, Kershaw T, Niccolai L, Lewis JB, Ickovics JR. Adolescent women underestimate their susceptibility to sexually transmitted infections. Sex Transm Infect. 2003;79:408–11.CrossRef Ethier KA, Kershaw T, Niccolai L, Lewis JB, Ickovics JR. Adolescent women underestimate their susceptibility to sexually transmitted infections. Sex Transm Infect. 2003;79:408–11.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
How aware are swingers about their swing sex partners’ risk behaviours, and sexually transmitted infection status?
verfasst von
Anne-Marie Niekamp
Laura W. L. Spauwen
Nicole H. T. M. Dukers-Muijrers
Christian J. P. A. Hoebe
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2021
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Infectious Diseases / Ausgabe 1/2021
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2334
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12879-021-05813-5

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

BMC Infectious Diseases 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Innere Medizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Innere Medizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.