Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2017

Open Access 01.12.2017 | Research article

Impaired mixed emotion processing in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: an fMRI study

verfasst von: Ádám György Szabó, Kinga Farkas, Csilla Marosi, Lajos R. Kozák, Gábor Rudas, János Réthelyi, Gábor Csukly

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2017

Abstract

Background

Schizophrenia has a negative effect on the activity of the temporal and prefrontal cortices in the processing of emotional facial expressions. However no previous research focused on the evaluation of mixed emotions in schizophrenia, albeit they are frequently expressed in everyday situations and negative emotions are frequently expressed by mixed facial expressions.

Methods

Altogether 37 subjects, 19 patients with schizophrenia and 18 healthy control subjects were enrolled in the study. The two study groups did not differ in age and education. The stimulus set consisted of 10 fearful (100%), 10 happy (100%), 10 mixed fear (70% fear and 30% happy) and 10 mixed happy facial expressions. During the fMRI acquisition pictures were presented in a randomized order and subjects had to categorize expressions by button press.

Results

A decreased activation was found in the patient group during fear, mixed fear and mixed happy processing in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) and the right anterior insula (RAI) at voxel and cluster level after familywise error correction. No difference was found between study groups in activations to happy facial condition. Patients with schizophrenia did not show a differential activation between mixed happy and happy facial expression similar to controls in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC).

Conclusions

Patients with schizophrenia showed decreased functioning in right prefrontal regions responsible for salience signaling and valence evaluation during emotion recognition. Our results indicate that fear and mixed happy/fear processing are impaired in schizophrenia, while happy facial expression processing is relatively intact.
Abkürzungen
AAL
Automated Anatomical Labeling
AP
Antipsychotic
BA
Brodmann Area
BOLD
Blood Oxygen Level Dependent
BZD
Benzodiazepine
CDT
cluster defining threshold
CNS
Central Nervous System
Cntrl
Control subjects
CPZ
Chlorpromazine Equivalent Dose
DLPFC
Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex
DLPFC
Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex
DMPFC
Dorsomedial Prefrontal Cortex
DODS
Different Offset, Different Slope
DSM-IV
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4thEdition
EEG
Electroencephalogram
EPI
Echo-planar imaging
FEW
Familywise Error Correction
fMRI
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
FOV
Field-of-view
FWHM
Full width at half maximum
GLM
General Linear Model
HRF
Hemodynamical Response Function
IAPS
International Affective Picture Set
MNI
Montreal Neurological Institute
MRI
Magnetic Resonance Imaging
PANSS
Positive and Negative Symptoms Scale
PSP
Personal and Social Performance Scale
QDEC
Query, Design, Estimate, Contrast
RAI
Right Anterior Insula
RMFG
Right Middle Frontal Cortex
RMOG
Right Middle Occipital Gyrus
ROI
Region of Interest
RPO
Right Parietal Operculum
RRO
Right Rolandic Operculum
RSTG
Right Superior Temporal Gyrus
Sch
Patients with Schizophrenia
SD
Standard Deviation
SNPM
Statistical non-Parametric Mapping
T1 W 3D TFE
T1 weighted 3 dimensional spoiled gradient echo
TE
Echo Time (fMRI)
TR
Temporal Resolution (fMRI sampling time)
VC
Vermis of the Cerebellum
VLPFC
Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex
VMPFC
Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex

Background

Schizophrenia is characterized with neurocognitive and social cognitive impairments, both of which contribute to decreased psychosocial functioning [1]. Social cognition consists of three major areas: attributional style, theory of mind and emotion recognition [2]. In the present investigation the focus was on emotion processing and recognition from facial displays.
Brain regions involved in emotion processing are the amygdala, the basal ganglia, the lateral and medial parietal cortex, the medial temporal lobe, the lateral temporal cortex, the dorsal and rostral anterior cingulate cortex, the anterior insular cortex, and the prefrontal cortex [3, 4]. The amygdala is primarily responsible for the cognitive representation of fear [5]. The temporal-parietal structures are activated in the process of face and facial expression detection [6]. The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) are involved in the regulation of cognitive functions linked to emotions such as attention, while the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) signals emotion salience and the need to regulate [7, 8].The right VLPFC is also implicated in the integration of viscerosensory information with affective signals between the bilateral anterior VLPFCs and the bilateral amygdale [9] and activated in situations with decision uncertainty [10]. The anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortex are involved in emotional tasks with cognitive demand [4].
Multiple previous studies have shown that schizophrenia has an effect on the functioning of the temporal [11] and prefrontal cortices [12], including the detection, evaluation and reappraisal of facial emotions. A recent study also revealed that the neural networks of frontolimbic regions are impaired in adolescents at high risk for schizophrenia [13].
While several previous investigations examined the recognition of basic emotions in schizophrenia [14], only a few studied the processing of different emotion intensities, while no previous research focused on the evaluation of mixed emotions, however they are requently expressed in everyday situations. Furthermore in many cases negative emotions such as fear are expressed by mixed facial expression (e.g. a mixture of a smile and a fearful facial display). Therefore using mixed emotions can be more ecological valid. Hence in the present investigation facial displays of mixed positive and negative emotions were also presented to patients with schizophrenia and healthy subjects. We hypothesized that the processing of these more complex emotions are more severely impaired in schizophrenia, and therefore a better differentiation of patients from controls can be achieved based on activation patterns. Specifically decreased activations were expected in regions involved in facial emotion processing in the patient group, especially in prefrontal regions, where previous investigation found activation in task with high cognitive demand linked to emotion processing such the anterior insula [4] and also in regions linked to salience signaling such as the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex [15].

Methods

Subjects

Altogether 19 patients with schizophrenia and 18 healthy control subjects were enrolled in the study. All participants underwent a series of psychological tests, EEG (electroencephalographic) examinations and finally a set of functional MRI (magnetic resonance imaging) tasks. The two study groups did not differ in age and education (Table 1). All participants were right-handed with the exception of one left-handed and one mixed handed patient and two left-handed healthy controls. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Selection criteria for all participants were no history of any CNS (central nervous system) disease, mental retardation, epileptic seizure, substance dependence or substance abuse in the past 3 months, no history of head injury with loss of consciousness more than 10 minutes. For healthy controls further exclusion criteria were any psychiatric disorder and a global severity index of >114 on the Symptom Checklist–90–R, [16] according to a Hungarian population sample [17], in order to exclude subjects with high risk for psychiatric disorders.
Patients were recruited from the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary. All patients met the criteria for schizophrenia based on the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4thEdition (DSM-IV)(American Psychiatry Association 1994). The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) were evaluated by a trained psychiatrist [18]. At the time of testing all patients took antipsychotic medication, the mean Chlorpromazine equivalent dose (Gardner et al. 2010) was 716 mg/day (SD = 337), while 10 patients took Benzodiazepines (BZD), and the mean Clonazepam equivalent dose (CPZ) was 1.6 mg/day (SD = 1.1 mg). Psychosocial functioning was measured by the Personal and Social Performance Scale (PSP) [19]. Demographic information for both study groups and the clinical characteristics of the patient group are presented in Table 1 .
Table 1
Demographic information for both study groups and clinical characteristics of the schizophrenia group
 
Schizophrenia Group
(n = 19)
Mean (SD)
Control Group
(n = 18)
Mean (SD)
statistics
p value
Gender (male/female)
12 / 6
11/8
Chi2 = 0.3
n.s.
Age
37.4 (8.4)
37.6 (10.9)
t = 0.1
n.s.
Educationa
2/5/6/6
1/2/7/8
Fisher’s exact test
n.s.
Illness duration (years)
12.2 (6.3)
  
In−/Outpatients
9 / 10
  
CPZ equivalent dose
716 mg (337)
  
PANSS total score
77.7 (18.5)
  
 PANSS positive subscore
17.6 (6.7)
  
 PANSS negative subscore
20.3 (6.8)
  
 PANSS general subscore
39.8 (9.1)
  
PSP
59.5 (16.1)
  
a1 = elementary school; 2 = high school; 3 = vocational school; 4 = college/university
(CPZ = chlorpromazine equivalent dose; PANSS = Positive and Negative Symptoms Scale; PSP = Personal and Social Performance Scale)

Procedures

Stimuli

In order to generate mixed emotional facial expressions morphed faces from the Ekman database [20] were used. These morphed faces were tested in a different group of healthy subjects (n = 20), who had not participated in the MRI study. Subjects had to select the faces which expressed the mixture of the two emotions the best, but dominantly happy or fear respectively. The ones with 30% and 70% mixture of the emotions were selected for the final task as mixed emotions. We refer to these mixed emotions by the 70% component: mixed fear is 70% fear and 30% happy, while mixed happy is 70% happy and 30% fear. The stimulus set consisted of 10 fearful (100%) facial expression, 10 happy (100%) faces and 10 mixed fear and 10 mixed happy faces.
All pictures were presented for 2 s in a randomized order, the inter-stimulus baseline was a distorted (scrambled) picture, which was a Fourier transformation of all the original images (Fig. 1). The inter-stimulus interval was randomized between 2 and 10 s. The subjects had to respond by a button press whether they had seen a happy or a fearful facial expression. The viewing distance was 60 cm. Happy facial expression was selected as counterparts of fearful faces, since the recognition of happiness was found to be mildly affected compared to impaired fear processing in patient with schizophrenia according to previous studies [21], while the processing of neutral facial expressions were found to be severely impaired in schizophrenia [2224]. We decided to exclude neutral facial displays from the paradigm due to the following reasons: using three response buttons may overcomplicate the task and the increasing number of stimuli would have made the task too long, both of which might have raised attentional issues and might have biased the results, especially in the patient group.

Image acquisition

The described MRI procedure took 30 min and were done at the MR Research Center, Semmelweis University on a 3 Tesla Philips Achieva whole body clinical MRI scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands) equipped with an 8-channel SENSE head-coil. The high resolution, whole brain anatomical images were obtained using a T1 weighted 3 dimensional spoiled gradient echo (T1 W 3D TFE) sequence. 180 contiguous slices were acquired from each subject with the following imaging parameters: TR (time resolution) = 9.7 ms; TE (echo time) = 4.6 ms; flip angle = 8°; FOV (Field-of view) of 240 mm × 240 mm; voxel size of 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm. Functional images were acquired using a T2* weighted gradient echo echo-planar imaging sequence with the following parameters: TR = 2.0 ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 70°, FOV of 240 mm × 240 mm; voxel size of 3.0 × 3.0 × 4.0 mm; maximum number of slices = 36. NordicNeuroLab SyncBox and ResponseGrip (NordicNeuroLab, Bergen, Norway) were used to record subject responses during tasks. Stimuli were presented in Psychtoolbox (http://​psychtoolbox.​org/), a Matlab R2012A (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA) toolbox used for visual stimulus presentation. In During the fMRI experiment visual stimuli were projected onto a translucent screen located at the back of the scanner bore using an Panasonic Panasonic PT-D3500E DLP projector (Matsushita Electric Industrial Co., Osaka, Japan) at a refresh rate of 75 Hz. Stimuli were viewed from inside the magnet bore by through a mirror system attached to the head coil, the viewing distance was 58 cm. Head motion was minimized using foam padding.

MRI preprocessing and statistical analysis

Image preprocessing was performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK) under Matlab 2012 (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA). Echo-planar imaging (EPI) data of each subject were preprocessed, which included slice timing, realignment and normalization into a standard template (Montreal Neurological Institute, MNI). A motions threshold of 3mms were used (equals to voxel size), any subjects moved beyond this threshold were excluded. Based on these criteria altogether four subjects were excluded from the study. Normalized images were finally smoothed in space with an 8 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) 3D isotropic Gaussian kernel. Images were finally filtered with a high-pass filter of 128 s to remove low frequency drifts. After preprocessing, statistical analyses for each individual subject was performed based on fixed-effects general linear models (GLM). Individual events were modeled as single delta (aka stick) functions at each stimulus onset and convolved with the canonical hemodynamical response function (HRF). In the 1st level analysis the four conditions (happy, fear, mixed happy, mixed fear) were modelled, while the 24-motion parameters were included as nuisance regressors (the motion parameters, their first derivatives, the squared motion parameters, and the squared derivatives). Group level analyses was performed by Statistical Non-Parametric Mapping (SNPM version 13) (http://​warwick.​ac.​uk/​snpm), a permutation based approach [25]. In the second level analysis both voxel-wise and cluster-wise results were calculated with 10.000 permutations (pseudo-T values were calculated), in the latter case a cluster defining threshold (CDT) of p < 0.001 was used in SNPM to determine significant activations. Age and gender were included as covariates in the analysis. The results of the 2nd level analysis are presented in Table 2 including corresponding anatomical regions and Brodmann areas detected using xjView toolbox (http://​www.​alivelearn.​net/​xjview).
Table 2
Between group differences in brain activations
Condition/Anatomical region
Brodmann areas (BA)
Peak MNI coordinates
Voxel level
Cluster level
X
Y
Z
p value (FWE-corr.)
cluster size (voxels/cm3)
p value
(FWE-corr.)
Fear (100% Fear)
 Right Middle Frontal Gyrus (RMFG)
10
42
50
2
0.01
181/6.5
0.006
 Right Anterior Insula (RAI),
 Right Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex (VLPFC)
13, 47
45
20
−6
0.01
166/6.0
0.006
 Right and Left Medial Frontal Gyrus,
 Dorsomedial Prefrontal Cortex (DMPFC)
8, 9
3
35
42
0.03
142/5.1
0.008
Mixed fear (70% Fear / 30% Happy)
 Right Anterior Insula (RAI),
 Right Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex (VLPFC)
13, 47
45
20
−6
0.003
218/7.8
0.002
Mixed happy (70% Happy/30% Fear)
 Right Anterior Insula (RAI),
 Right Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex (VLPFC)
13, 47
33
35
−10
0.03
171/6.2
0.004
 Right Middle Occipital Gyrus (RMOG)
18
39
−85
−6
ns.
54/1.9
0.04
Additionally correlational analyses were performed between clinical variables (PANSS, PSP, AP and BZD dose) and the activations in the clusters/regions where group differences were found. Correlational analyses were performed in SAS 9.3 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Further ROIs selected from the AAL (Automated Anatomical Labeling) brain atlas [26] corresponding to emotion processing [3] were created and extracted by Marsbar [27]. Correlational analyses were performed for these 14 ROIs (Left and Right Amygdala, Fusiform gyrus, Superior/Middle/Inferior Temporal gyrus and Superior/Middle Temporal Pole) with the same clinical variables as listed above.
Cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation were performed with the Freesurfer 5.3 image analysis suite, which is documented and freely available for download online (http://​surfer.​nmr.​mgh.​harvard.​edu/​), and the results contain information about each vertex of the created mesh. The technical details of these procedures are described in prior publications; we made no changes to this pipeline. Segmentations and cortical models were checked and corrected manually on each subject. We used FreeSurfer’s QDEC (Query, Design, Estimate, Contrast) interface to perform group averaging and generate the statistical maps from the cortical morphometric data produced by FreeSurfer stream. General Linear Model (GLM) was computed vertex-by-vertex for analysis of surface area and cortical thickness. The cortical volumes of the schizophrenic patient group was compared with healthy controls, accounting for the effects of age and gender. From the different methods that QDEC provides for automatic design matrix creation, DODS (different offset, different slope) was used. The results were visualised by overlaying the significant areas onto the cortical surfaces, maps were smoothed using a 10 mm full width at half maximum Gaussian kernel. Multiple comparisons were corrected with a Monte Carlo Simulation using a p-value set at <0.05.

Results

Differentiation between study groups based on BOLD activations

Decreased activation was found in the patient group during fear (Fig. 2/A) and mixed fear (Fig. 2/B) processing in clusters corresponding to the right VLPFC (Brodmann Area 47 = BA47), the right anterior insula (RAI/BA13), and the right dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC/BA9) at voxel and cluster level. The results of the analyses are summarized in Table 2.
A significant decrease at voxel and cluster level activation was also found in the right VLPFC and right anterior insula (RAI), and at cluster level in the right middle occipital gyrus (BA18/associative visual cortex) in patients when viewing mixed happy faces (Table 2 ; Fig. 2/B). No difference was found between study groups to happy faces in any regions.
Activations to all four emotion conditions in the right amygdala correlated negatively with antipsychotic dose, which was the strongest for mixed happy condition (mixed happy: r = −0.66, n = 19, p = 0.002; happy: r = −0.55, n = 19, p = 0.02; mixed fear: r = −0.47, n = 19, p = 0.04; fear: r = −0.48, n = 19, p = 0.04) (Fig. 3). Correlations of brain activations with psychotic symptom severity, psychosocial functioning and BZD doses did not reach significance in any regions.

Interactions between study group and condition

A study group X condition (Fear vs Happy) interaction was found significant (FWE corrected p = 0.03) in a 62 voxel cluster in the right parietal operculum (RPO) and right superior temporal gyrus (RSTG/BA22) and also in the right middle frontal gyrus (RMFG/BA10) (FWE corrected p = 0.04) in a 50 voxel cluster (Table 3 ; Fig. 2/C). Further analyzing average activations in these clusters (Fig. 4), increased activations to fear compared to happy facial expressions were found in controls in RPO and RSTG (p = 0.004), and in RMFG (p = 0.04), while there were increased activations to happy compared to fearful faces in patients in RPO and RSTG (p = 0.006), and in RMFG (p < 0.0001).
Table 3
Interaction between study group and stimulus condition in bran activations
Condition/Anatomical region
Brodmann areas (BA)
Peak MNI coordinates
Voxel level
Cluster level
X
Y
Z
p value (FWE-corr.)
cluster size (voxels/cm3)
p value
(FWE-corr.)
(Fear vs Happy) X (Control vs Patient)
 Right Superior Temporal Gyrus (RSTG), Right Rolandic Operculum (RRO)
22
60
−4
6
ns.
62/2.2
0.03
 Right Middle Frontal Gyrus (RMFG)
10
30
47
6
ns.
50/1.8
0.04
(Mixed Happy vs Happy) X (Control vs Patient)
 Right and Left Cerebellum, Vermis
NA
3
−76
−38
< 0.05
94/3.4
0.01
 Right Middle and Inferior Frontal Gyrus, Right Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex (DLPFC)
9, 46
51
23
22
0.06
80/2.9
0.02
Also the study group X condition (Mixed Happy vs Happy) interaction was found significant (FWE corrected p = 0.01 (cluster level) p < 0.05 (peak voxel)) in a 94 voxel cluster in the vermis of the Cerebellum (VC) and in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC/BA46) (FWE corrected p = 0.02) in a 80 voxel cluster (Table 3 ; Fig. 2/C). Further analyzing average activations in these clusters (Fig. 4), increased activations to mixed happy compared to happy facial expressions were found in controls both in VC and (p < 0.0001), and in DLPFC (p < 0.0001), while there were no significant between condition differences in the patient groups in any of these regions.

Behavioral results

Recognition performance

The study groups did not differ significantly in the recognition of happy, mixed happy, and fearful facial expressions (happy: Control subjects (Cntrl) = 92% (SD = 17%), Patients with Schizophrenia (Sch) = 98% (SD = 4%), t = 1.2, p = 0.2; mixed happy: Cntrl = 86% (SD = 17%), Sch = 90% (SD = 15%), t = 0.9, p = 0.4; fear: Cntrl = 99% (SD = 3%), Sch = 99% (SD = 3%), t = 0.2, p = 0.9), while patients recognized mixed fear emotions as fear at a significantly lower rate compared to controls (mixed fear: Cntrl = 90% (SD = 9%), Sch = 84% (SD = 11%), t = 2.4, p = 0.02, Effect Size = 0.6 in terms of Cohen’s d).

Reaction times

The two groups did not differ in reaction times (overall: Cntrl = 1.13 s (SD = 0.17 s), Sch = 1.21 s (SD = 0.23 s), t = −1.2, p = 0.24; happy: Cntrl = 1.07 s (SD = 0.19 s), Sch = 1.24 s (SD = 0.32 s), t = −1.9, p = 0.06; mixed happy: Cntrl = 1.26 s (SD = 0.23 s), Sch = 1.39 s (SD = 0.33 s), t = −1.4, p = 0.18; fear: Cntrl = 1.18 s (SD = 0.24 s), Sch = 1.47 s (SD = 0.36 s), t = −2.1, p = 0.054; mixed fear: Cntrl = 1.60s (SD = 0.33 s), Sch = 1.54 s (SD = 0.30s), t = 0.5, p = 0.61).

Volumetric comparison of patients and controls

The primary purpose of this analysis was to check if functional activation differences between the groups can be explained by volumetric differences. After Familywise Error Correction (FEW) no significant structural differences were found between the study groups.

Discussion

In this study we examined emotion processing in a group of patients with schizophrenia and in healthy controls. The study groups were matched by age, gender, and education. Patients showed a decreased activation in the right anterior insula (RAI), in the right VLPFC, in the right DMPFC during fear, mixed fear, and mixed happy facial processing and showed poorer emotion recognition performance in fear detection relative to controls. In patients on higher doses of APs decreased activations to all emotional displays were detected in the right anterior amygdala, which correlation was the strongest for mixed happy faces.
Subjects were presented with facial displays of happiness, fear, and the mixture of these emotions as described in the methods section. From previous research we know that VLPFC activation is related to emotion intensity and reappraisal [7, 28], and signals the salience of the stimulus [8]. Moreover, emotional tasks with cognitive demand involves primarily the anterior cingulate and insula [4]. Decreased activation was detected in the right anterior insula (RAI), and in the VLPFC region to fear, mixed fear, and mixed happy conditions in patients relative to controls. Previous investigations showed that VLPFC plays a key role in emotion regulation and reappraisal through cortical-subcortical pathways especially to aversive stimuli [29]. It is also found that VLPFC activity correlated with reduced negative emotional experience during cognitive reappraisal of aversive images [30]. Taken these results together with our findings raised the possibility that salience signaling and perhaps also emotion regulation linked to negative emotion processing are impaired in schizophrenia, which is in line with previous studies examining emotion processing in schizophrenia that found decreased activation in the VLPFC relative to controls [12, 15, 31].
Patients showed decreased activation relative to controls in the right middle occipital gyrus (RMOG/BA18) to facial conditions of mixed happy facial stimuli. The RMOG/BA18 is part of the ventral stream of visual processing for which preferential processing of affective stimuli has previously been demonstrated [32]. Previous studies found increased activation in the RMOG/BA18 to fearful faces compared to happy stimuli during facial emotion processing [32], furthermore a recent study found that this region is under control by the anterior cingulate cortex during facial emotion processing [33]. Our finding is in line with previous results showing an early visual processing impairment in emotional facial processing in schizophrenia [3436]. Furthermore patients with schizophrenia showed decreased activations in the right middle frontal cortex (RMFG/BA10) and in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) to fearful facial stimuli. According to previous investigations decreased DMPFC activity in response to emotion facial cues may reflect less cognitive control involved in decoding and/or regulating negative emotions [37], which was also observed in chronically violent men [38], and in postpartum depression [39]. A study examining the theory of mind impairments in subjects with autistic spectrum disorder found decreased activity in the DMPFC during inferring other person’s social emotions in a false belief task [40].
Patients on higher doses of APs showed decreased activation in the right amygdala to all emotional faces with the strongest correlation for mixed happy condition. Little is known so far on the effects of AP treatment on emotion processing in schizophrenia. In a previous study Sachs et al. [41] reported no difference in amygdala activation between patients treated with atypical APs and healthy subjects. In another study 12 previously drug-free/naive patients with schizophrenia were treated with olanzapine for 8 weeks and underwent two fMRI scans after 4 and 8 weeks of treatment during implicit and explicit emotional processing. Activity in left amygdala was greater in patients than in controls at the first scan during both explicit and implicit processing, while it was lower in patients at the second relative to the first scan [42]. However none of the previous studies reported any dose related effect of APs on the activity of brain regions involved in emotion processing. Contrary to previous investigations no between group differences were found in the amygdala activation to fearful faces. A possible explanation to that is that fearful faces in the present study were not as strong fear evoking stimuli as the scenarios depicted in the International Affective Picture Set (IAPS) [43].
Previous studies found that the superior temporal gyrus and the insular cortex have been involved in the perception of emotions in facial stimuli [5, 44], while the BA10 region is mainly activated during multi-tasking and theory of mind tasks [45]. In the present study an increased activation to fearful faces relative to happy faces were detected in the right superior temporal gyrus and in the right middle frontal gyrus (BA10) in healthy subjects, while the opposite difference (happy > fear) was found in patients. In a previous investigation, similarly to our results, an increased activation to fearful stimuli relative to safe stimuli was detected in the insular cortex in healthy subjects [44].
The activation of the DLPFC is related to the regulation of cognitive processes linked to emotion processing such as attention [7, 28]. A decreased activation difference between mixed happy faces and happy faces in patients relative to controls were found in the cerebellum (vermis) and in the right DLPFC. Controls showed an increased activation to mixed happy faces relative to happy faces in these regions, while similar between emotion differences were not found in patients. These findings are in line with previous investigations showing that during emotion processing [46] and social cognition tasks [47] patients with schizophrenia showed decreased activations in the DLPFC and in the cerebellum during face processing [48].
Structural impairments in patients may explain the functional differences, since some previous investigations found structural differences in patients with schizophrenia relative to healthy controls in temporal [49] and frontal regions. However no similar volumetric differences were found in the present study.
A major limitation of the present study was that no neutral facial expressions were used. The reason was that we intended keep the paradigm as short as possible to avoid attention related confounds. A further limitation was that patients were at antipsychotic medication, which may bias the activations, however there were no correlations between antipsychotic doses and BOLD activations in the regions where the between group differences were found. Study groups were matched by age, gender, and education, however this approach has the drawback that it does not take into consideration that patients with schizophrenia experience a significant reduction in educational attainment relative to what we be predicted had they not received such a diagnosis. Due to a technical malfunction of the input device several responses were lost, which made it impossible to include responses into the 1st level model. This may lead to confounds related to the motor activation needed to produce a button press, however no between study group differences were detected in motor areas or supplementary motor regions, which makes it unlikely that this was a major confound.

Conclusions

Patients with schizophrenia showed decreased activation in the right anterior insula (RAI) and in the right VLPFC during facial emotion processing. Whereas the former region is activated in emotional tasks with cognitive demand, the latter is responsible for salience signaling [8]. These impaired process were detected in response to facial expressions containing more or less fear with or without happiness, while no similar impairment was detected in the processing of happy facial expressions without fear. Therefore we can conclude that fear and mixed happy/fear processing are impaired in schizophrenia, while happy facial expression processing is relatively intact. Furthermore patients with schizophrenia did not show a differential activation between mixed happy and happy facial expressions similar to controls in the right DLPFC, a region related to the regulation of cognitive processes linked to emotions such as attention, and valence evaluation [8]. In sum patients with schizophrenia showed decreased functioning in right prefrontal regions responsible for salience signaling and valence evaluation during emotion recognition.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the staff of the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy for their assistance helping this study to be completed.

Funding

This work was partially funded by the grants from the Hungarian Research Fund (OTKA PD 115837). Furthermore Lajos R Kozak was supported by the Bolyai Research Fellowship Program of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences. The funders had no role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets during and/or analysed during the current study available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
All procedures were carried out only after written informed consent was obtained from the participants. All potential participants who declined to participate or otherwise did not participate were not disadvantaged in any way by not participating in the study.
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. The research was approved by the “Scientific and Research Committee, Medical Research Council, Hungary”.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Addington J, Saeedi H, Addington D. Influence of social perception and social knowledge on cognitive and social functioning in early psychosis. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:373–8.CrossRefPubMed Addington J, Saeedi H, Addington D. Influence of social perception and social knowledge on cognitive and social functioning in early psychosis. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:373–8.CrossRefPubMed
2.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Lieberman MD. Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:259–89.CrossRefPubMed Lieberman MD. Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:259–89.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage. 2002;16(2):331–48.CrossRefPubMed Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage. 2002;16(2):331–48.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Phelps EA, O'Connor KJ, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Grillon C, Davis M. Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4(4):437–41.CrossRefPubMed Phelps EA, O'Connor KJ, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Grillon C, Davis M. Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4(4):437–41.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Fusar-Poli P, Placentino A, Carletti F, Landi P, Allen P, Surguladze S, Benedetti F, Abbamonte M, Gasparotti R, Barale F, et al. Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009;34(6):418–32.PubMedPubMedCentral Fusar-Poli P, Placentino A, Carletti F, Landi P, Allen P, Surguladze S, Benedetti F, Abbamonte M, Gasparotti R, Barale F, et al. Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009;34(6):418–32.PubMedPubMedCentral
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Grimm S, Schmidt CF, Bermpohl F, Heinzel A, Dahlem Y, Wyss M, Hell D, Boesiger P, Boeker H, Northoff G. Segregated neural representation of distinct emotion dimensions in the prefrontal cortex-an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2006;30(1):325–40.CrossRefPubMed Grimm S, Schmidt CF, Bermpohl F, Heinzel A, Dahlem Y, Wyss M, Hell D, Boesiger P, Boeker H, Northoff G. Segregated neural representation of distinct emotion dimensions in the prefrontal cortex-an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2006;30(1):325–40.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Kohn N, Eickhoff SB, Scheller M, Laird AR, Fox PT, Habel U. Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation--an ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis. NeuroImage. 2014;87:345–55.CrossRefPubMed Kohn N, Eickhoff SB, Scheller M, Laird AR, Fox PT, Habel U. Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation--an ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis. NeuroImage. 2014;87:345–55.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Kohno S, Noriuchi M, Iguchi Y, Kikuchi Y, Hoshi Y. Emotional discrimination during viewing unpleasant pictures: timing in human anterior ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9:51.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kohno S, Noriuchi M, Iguchi Y, Kikuchi Y, Hoshi Y. Emotional discrimination during viewing unpleasant pictures: timing in human anterior ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9:51.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Levy BJ, Wagner AD. Cognitive control and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: reflexive reorienting, motor inhibition, and action updating. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011;1224:40–62.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Levy BJ, Wagner AD. Cognitive control and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: reflexive reorienting, motor inhibition, and action updating. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011;1224:40–62.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Habel U, Chechko N, Pauly K, Koch K, Backes V, Seiferth N, Shah NJ, Stocker T, Schneider F, Kellermann T. Neural correlates of emotion recognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2010;122(1-3):113–23.CrossRefPubMed Habel U, Chechko N, Pauly K, Koch K, Backes V, Seiferth N, Shah NJ, Stocker T, Schneider F, Kellermann T. Neural correlates of emotion recognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2010;122(1-3):113–23.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Leitman DI, Wolf DH, Loughead J, Valdez JN, Kohler CG, Brensinger C, Elliott MA, Turetsky BI, Gur RE, Gur RC. Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the effects of task demand context on facial affect appraisal in schizophrenia. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2011;6(1):66–73.CrossRefPubMed Leitman DI, Wolf DH, Loughead J, Valdez JN, Kohler CG, Brensinger C, Elliott MA, Turetsky BI, Gur RE, Gur RC. Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the effects of task demand context on facial affect appraisal in schizophrenia. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2011;6(1):66–73.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Diwadkar VA, Wadehra S, Pruitt P, Keshavan MS, Rajan U, Zajac-Benitez C, Eickhoff SB. Disordered corticolimbic interactions during affective processing in children and adolescents at risk for schizophrenia revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging and dynamic causal modeling. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(3):231–42.CrossRefPubMed Diwadkar VA, Wadehra S, Pruitt P, Keshavan MS, Rajan U, Zajac-Benitez C, Eickhoff SB. Disordered corticolimbic interactions during affective processing in children and adolescents at risk for schizophrenia revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging and dynamic causal modeling. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(3):231–42.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Marwick K, Hall J. Social cognition in schizophrenia: a review of face processing. Br Med Bull. 2008;88(1):43–58.CrossRefPubMed Marwick K, Hall J. Social cognition in schizophrenia: a review of face processing. Br Med Bull. 2008;88(1):43–58.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat van der Meer L, Swart M, van d V, Pijnenborg G, Wiersma D, Bruggeman R, Aleman A. Neural correlates of emotion regulation in patients with schizophrenia and non-affected siblings. PLoS One. 2014;9(6):e99667.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral van der Meer L, Swart M, van d V, Pijnenborg G, Wiersma D, Bruggeman R, Aleman A. Neural correlates of emotion regulation in patients with schizophrenia and non-affected siblings. PLoS One. 2014;9(6):e99667.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Derogatis LR, Melisaratos N. The brief symptom inventory: an introductory report. Psychol Med. 1983;13(3):595–605.CrossRefPubMed Derogatis LR, Melisaratos N. The brief symptom inventory: an introductory report. Psychol Med. 1983;13(3):595–605.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Unoka Z, Rózsa S, Ko N, Kállai J, Fábián Á, Simon L. Validity and reliability of the SCL-90 in a Hungarian population sample. Psychiatria Hungarica. 2004;19:235–43. Unoka Z, Rózsa S, Ko N, Kállai J, Fábián Á, Simon L. Validity and reliability of the SCL-90 in a Hungarian population sample. Psychiatria Hungarica. 2004;19:235–43.
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13(2):261–76.CrossRefPubMed Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13(2):261–76.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Burns T, Patrick D. Social functioning as an outcome measure in schizophrenia studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2007;116(6):403–18.CrossRefPubMed Burns T, Patrick D. Social functioning as an outcome measure in schizophrenia studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2007;116(6):403–18.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Ekman P, WV F. Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1976. Ekman P, WV F. Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1976.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Mandal MK, Pandey R, Prasad AB. Facial expressions of emotions and schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Bull. 1998;24(3):399–412.CrossRefPubMed Mandal MK, Pandey R, Prasad AB. Facial expressions of emotions and schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Bull. 1998;24(3):399–412.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Pinkham AE, Brensinger C, Kohler C, Gur RE, Gur RC. Actively paranoid patients with schizophrenia over attribute anger to neutral faces. Schizophr Res. 2011;125(2-3):174–8.CrossRefPubMed Pinkham AE, Brensinger C, Kohler C, Gur RE, Gur RC. Actively paranoid patients with schizophrenia over attribute anger to neutral faces. Schizophr Res. 2011;125(2-3):174–8.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Hall J, Whalley HC, McKirdy JW, Romaniuk L, McGonigle D, McIntosh AM, Baig BJ, Gountouna VE, Job DE, Donaldson DI, et al. Overactivation of fear systems to neutral faces in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(1):70–3.CrossRefPubMed Hall J, Whalley HC, McKirdy JW, Romaniuk L, McGonigle D, McIntosh AM, Baig BJ, Gountouna VE, Job DE, Donaldson DI, et al. Overactivation of fear systems to neutral faces in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(1):70–3.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Surguladze S, Russell T, Kucharska-Pietura K, Travis MJ, Giampietro V, David AS, Phillips ML. A reversal of the normal pattern of parahippocampal response to neutral and fearful faces is associated with reality distortion in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(5):423–31.CrossRefPubMed Surguladze S, Russell T, Kucharska-Pietura K, Travis MJ, Giampietro V, David AS, Phillips ML. A reversal of the normal pattern of parahippocampal response to neutral and fearful faces is associated with reality distortion in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(5):423–31.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;15(1):1–25.CrossRefPubMed Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;15(1):1–25.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, Mazoyer B, Joliot M. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage. 2002;15(1):273–89.CrossRefPubMed Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, Mazoyer B, Joliot M. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage. 2002;15(1):273–89.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Brett M, Anton JL, Valabregue R, Poline JB. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. In: 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain: 2002; Sendai. NeuroImage. 2002;13(2):210–17. Brett M, Anton JL, Valabregue R, Poline JB. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. In: 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain: 2002; Sendai. NeuroImage. 2002;13(2):210–17.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Silvers JA, Weber J, Wager TD, Ochsner KN. Bad and worse: neural systems underlying reappraisal of high- and low-intensity negative emotions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10(2):172–9.CrossRefPubMed Silvers JA, Weber J, Wager TD, Ochsner KN. Bad and worse: neural systems underlying reappraisal of high- and low-intensity negative emotions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10(2):172–9.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Torrisi SJ, Lieberman MD, Bookheimer SY, Altshuler LL. Advancing understanding of affect labeling with dynamic causal modeling. NeuroImage. 2013;82:481–8.CrossRefPubMed Torrisi SJ, Lieberman MD, Bookheimer SY, Altshuler LL. Advancing understanding of affect labeling with dynamic causal modeling. NeuroImage. 2013;82:481–8.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Wager TD, Davidson ML, Hughes BL, Lindquist MA, Ochsner KN. Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron. 2008;59(6):1037–50.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wager TD, Davidson ML, Hughes BL, Lindquist MA, Ochsner KN. Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron. 2008;59(6):1037–50.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris RW, Sparks A, Mitchell PB, Weickert CS, Green MJ. Lack of cortico-limbic coupling in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia during emotion regulation. Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2:e90.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Morris RW, Sparks A, Mitchell PB, Weickert CS, Green MJ. Lack of cortico-limbic coupling in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia during emotion regulation. Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2:e90.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Pessoa L, McKenna M, Gutierrez E, Ungerleider LG. Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99(17):11458–63.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pessoa L, McKenna M, Gutierrez E, Ungerleider LG. Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99(17):11458–63.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Steinhauser M, Flaisch T, Meinzer M, Schupp HT. Cognitive control modulates preferential sensory processing of affective stimuli. Neuropsychologia. 2016;91:435–43.CrossRefPubMed Steinhauser M, Flaisch T, Meinzer M, Schupp HT. Cognitive control modulates preferential sensory processing of affective stimuli. Neuropsychologia. 2016;91:435–43.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Csukly G, Stefanics G, Komlosi S, Czigler I, Czobor P. Emotion-related visual mismatch responses in schizophrenia: impairments and correlations with emotion recognition. PLoS One. 2013;8(10):e75444.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Csukly G, Stefanics G, Komlosi S, Czigler I, Czobor P. Emotion-related visual mismatch responses in schizophrenia: impairments and correlations with emotion recognition. PLoS One. 2013;8(10):e75444.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
35.
Zurück zum Zitat De SP, Foxe JJ, Czobor P, Wylie GR, Kamiel SM, Huening J, Nair-Collins M, Krakowski MI. Early sensory-perceptual processing deficits for affectively valenced inputs are more pronounced in schizophrenia patients with a history of violence than in their non-violent peers. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013;8(6):678–87. De SP, Foxe JJ, Czobor P, Wylie GR, Kamiel SM, Huening J, Nair-Collins M, Krakowski MI. Early sensory-perceptual processing deficits for affectively valenced inputs are more pronounced in schizophrenia patients with a history of violence than in their non-violent peers. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013;8(6):678–87.
36.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Pardini DA, Phillips M. Neural responses to emotional and neutral facial expressions in chronically violent men. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2010;35(6):390–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pardini DA, Phillips M. Neural responses to emotional and neutral facial expressions in chronically violent men. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2010;35(6):390–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Moses-Kolko EL, Perlman SB, Wisner KL, James J, Saul AT, Phillips ML. Abnormally reduced dorsomedial prefrontal cortical activity and effective connectivity with amygdala in response to negative emotional faces in postpartum depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(11):1373–80.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Moses-Kolko EL, Perlman SB, Wisner KL, James J, Saul AT, Phillips ML. Abnormally reduced dorsomedial prefrontal cortical activity and effective connectivity with amygdala in response to negative emotional faces in postpartum depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(11):1373–80.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Aoki Y, Yahata N, Watanabe T, Takano Y, Kawakubo Y, Kuwabara H, Iwashiro N, Natsubori T, Inoue H, Suga M, et al. Oxytocin improves behavioural and neural deficits in inferring others' social emotions in autism. Brain. 2014;137(Pt 11):3073–86.CrossRefPubMed Aoki Y, Yahata N, Watanabe T, Takano Y, Kawakubo Y, Kuwabara H, Iwashiro N, Natsubori T, Inoue H, Suga M, et al. Oxytocin improves behavioural and neural deficits in inferring others' social emotions in autism. Brain. 2014;137(Pt 11):3073–86.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Sachs G, Moser E, Katschnig H, Gur RC, Gur RE, Erfurth A. No amygdala attenuation in schizophrenic patients treated with atypical antipsychotics. Psychiatry Res. 2012;202(2):168–71.CrossRefPubMed Sachs G, Moser E, Katschnig H, Gur RC, Gur RE, Erfurth A. No amygdala attenuation in schizophrenic patients treated with atypical antipsychotics. Psychiatry Res. 2012;202(2):168–71.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Blasi G, Popolizio T, Taurisano P, Caforio G, Romano R, Di GA, Sambataro F, Rubino V, Latorre V, Lo BL, et al. Changes in prefrontal and amygdala activity during olanzapine treatment in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2009;173(1):31–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Blasi G, Popolizio T, Taurisano P, Caforio G, Romano R, Di GA, Sambataro F, Rubino V, Latorre V, Lo BL, et al. Changes in prefrontal and amygdala activity during olanzapine treatment in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2009;173(1):31–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Lang P, Bradley M, Cuthbert BN: International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual.; 2008. Lang P, Bradley M, Cuthbert BN: International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual.; 2008.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Radua J, Phillips ML, Russell T, Lawrence N, Marshall N, Kalidindi S, El-Hage W, McDonald C, Giampietro V, Brammer MJ, et al. Neural response to specific components of fearful faces in healthy and schizophrenic adults. NeuroImage. 2010;49(1):939–46.CrossRefPubMed Radua J, Phillips ML, Russell T, Lawrence N, Marshall N, Kalidindi S, El-Hage W, McDonald C, Giampietro V, Brammer MJ, et al. Neural response to specific components of fearful faces in healthy and schizophrenic adults. NeuroImage. 2010;49(1):939–46.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Gilbert SJ, Spengler S, Simons JS, Steele JD, Lawrie SM, Frith CD, Burgess PW. Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): a meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18(6):932–48.CrossRefPubMed Gilbert SJ, Spengler S, Simons JS, Steele JD, Lawrie SM, Frith CD, Burgess PW. Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): a meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18(6):932–48.CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Ursu S, Kring AM, Gard MG, Minzenberg MJ, Yoon JH, Ragland JD, Solomon M, Carter CS. Prefrontal cortical deficits and impaired cognition-emotion interactions in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(3):276–85.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ursu S, Kring AM, Gard MG, Minzenberg MJ, Yoon JH, Ragland JD, Solomon M, Carter CS. Prefrontal cortical deficits and impaired cognition-emotion interactions in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(3):276–85.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Shin JE, Choi SH, Lee H, Shin YS, Jang DP, Kim JJ. Involvement of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and superior temporal sulcus in impaired social perception in schizophrenia. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2015;58:81–8.CrossRef Shin JE, Choi SH, Lee H, Shin YS, Jang DP, Kim JJ. Involvement of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and superior temporal sulcus in impaired social perception in schizophrenia. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2015;58:81–8.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Mothersill O, Morris DW, Kelly S, Rose EJ, Bokde A, Reilly R, Gill M, Corvin AP, Donohoe G. Altered medial prefrontal activity during dynamic face processing in schizophrenia spectrum patients. Schizophr Res. 2014;157(1-3):225–30.CrossRefPubMed Mothersill O, Morris DW, Kelly S, Rose EJ, Bokde A, Reilly R, Gill M, Corvin AP, Donohoe G. Altered medial prefrontal activity during dynamic face processing in schizophrenia spectrum patients. Schizophr Res. 2014;157(1-3):225–30.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Goghari VM, Macdonald AW III, Sponheim SR. Temporal lobe structures and facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia patients and nonpsychotic relatives. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(6):1281–94.CrossRefPubMed Goghari VM, Macdonald AW III, Sponheim SR. Temporal lobe structures and facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia patients and nonpsychotic relatives. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(6):1281–94.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Impaired mixed emotion processing in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: an fMRI study
verfasst von
Ádám György Szabó
Kinga Farkas
Csilla Marosi
Lajos R. Kozák
Gábor Rudas
János Réthelyi
Gábor Csukly
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2017
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2017
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1558-x

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2017

BMC Psychiatry 1/2017 Zur Ausgabe