Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine 1/2021

Open Access 01.12.2021 | Research article

Increasing hepatitis virus screening uptake at worksites in Japan using nudge theory and full subsidies

verfasst von: Jun Fukuyoshi, Masaaki Korenaga, Yui Yoshii, Lek Hong, Soichiro Kashihara, Byron Sigel, Toru Takebayashi

Erschienen in: Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine | Ausgabe 1/2021

Abstract

Background

Despite the importance of hepatitis screening for decreasing liver cancer mortality, screening rates remain low in Japan. Previous studies show that full subsidies increase screening uptake, but full subsidies are costly and difficult to implement in low-resource settings. Alternatively, applying nudge theory to the message design could increase screening at lower costs. This study examined the effects of both methods in increasing hepatitis virus screening rates at worksites.

Methods

1496 employees from a Japanese transportation company received client reminders for an optional hepatitis virus screening before their general health checkups. Groups A and B received a client reminder designed based on the principles of “Easy” and “Attractive,” while the control group received a client reminder not developed using nudge theory. Additionally, hepatitis virus screening was offered to the control group and group A for a co-payment of JPY 612, but was fully subsidized for group B. The hepatitis virus screening rates among the groups were compared using a Chi-square test with Bonferroni correction, and the risk ratios of group A and group B to the control group were also calculated. To adjust for unobservable heterogeneity per cluster, the regression analysis was performed using generalized linear mixed models.

Results

The screening rate was 21.2%, 37.1%, and 86.3% for the control group, group A, and group B, respectively. And the risk ratio for group A was 1.75 (95% confidence interval [CI] 1.45–2.12) and that of group B was 4.08 (95% CI 3.44–4.83). The parameters of group A and group B also were significant when estimated using generalized linear mixed models. However, the cost-effectiveness (incremental cost-effectiveness ratio (ICER)) of the nudge-based reminder with the full subsidies was lower than that of only the nudge-based reminder.

Conclusions

While fully subsidized screening led to the highest hepatitis screening rates, modifying client reminders using nudge theory significantly increased hepatitis screening uptake at lower costs per person.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
BIT
Behavioural Insights Team
CRC
Colorectal cancer
DAA
Direct-acting antivirals
ICER
Incremental cost-effectiveness ratio
HBV
Hepatitis B virus
HCV
Hepatitis C virus
JHIA
Japan Health Insurance Association
JPY
Japanese Yen
USD
US dollar

Introduction

Liver cancer is one of the leading causes of cancer deaths worldwide [1]. In Japan, an estimated 29,000 people died of liver cancer in 2017 [2]. Furthermore, approximately 70% of liver cancer cases in Japan develop from an infection caused by the hepatitis B virus (HBV) or hepatitis C virus (HCV) [3]. These patients, especially those who are infected with HCV, can be cured with minimal adverse effects using direct-acting antivirals (DAAs) [4]. Thus, the early detection of the hepatitis virus infection by screening is a major public health goal.
To promote hepatitis screening, the Japanese government enacted the Basic Act on Hepatitis Countermeasures in 2009, encouraging municipalities to provide hepatitis virus screening for citizens over 40 years of age [5]. As a result, hepatitis virus screening rates at the municipal level increased. However, hepatitis virus screening rates at worksites remain low in Japan. A previous study surveying 2420 employees showed that only 13.8% had undergone hepatitis screening [6].
Previous studies suggest that providing cancer screenings free of charge could increase screening rates [7]. For example, in Japan, eliminating cost significantly increased screening rates for cervical cancer screening [8]. However, even if free screening is effective, this can add substantial costs to providers as well as employers. Alternatively, applications of nudge theory tend to be low-cost solutions [9], and substantial evidence demonstrates its effectiveness in the realm of cancer screenings [1012]. Nudge theory, as defined by Thaler and Sunstein, “is any aspect of the choice architecture that alters people’s behavior in a predictable way without forbidding any options or significantly changing their economic incentives” [13]. To illustrate, in Hachioji city, Japan, the city office developed client reminders based on nudge theory and sent to non-takers of colorectal cancer (CRC) screening. These reminders focused on (i) readability and (ii) the low co-payment for screening due to subsidies [14]. This nudge-theory-based reminder led to a CRC uptake of 26.8%, exceeding initial expectations of 19%. Nudge theory could change behavior by modifying the perception of the screening without offering significant financial incentives. Therefore, it is important to compare not only the effectiveness but also the cost-effectiveness of these interventions in promoting hepatitis screening.
Currently, few studies aim to promote the hepatitis virus screening rates at worksites. For many small- to medium-sized companies in Japan, the Japan Health Insurance Association (JHIA) covers the healthcare costs for workers. As of 2015, 17 million workers over the age of 40 were enrolled [15]. This study examines the effects of (i) making screenings free of charge and (ii) applying nudge theory to the design of client reminders on hepatitis virus screening among members of JHIA. Given the large number of premium holders under JHIA, these results could significantly contribute to a reduction in patients with liver cancer in Japan.

Methods

Setting

As a member of JHIA, the transportation company provided general health checkups to 12,450 employees as mandated by the Industrial Safety and Health Act [16]. Hepatitis virus screening was offered as an optional test for JPY 612 (US$6) during these general checkups. From 2015 to 2016, a trial program was conducted and designed by the transportation company to increase the uptake of hepatitis virus screening among its employees, who were mostly truck drivers.

Procedure

Participants were included if they (i) applied and attended the general health checkups conducted in July 2015 and January 2016 and (ii) were 35–74 years old. 1496 employees met the inclusion criteria.
The company designed and executed a cluster-randomized trial, in which the thirteen tracking stations located in urban cities were randomly assigned to control group, group A, or group B (Fig. 1). The employees received different types of client reminders depending on the tracking stations they worked at. Client reminders were sent a few weeks before the general health checkups. Information on the homogeneity among the groups is provided in the results.

Intervention

The control group (n = 534) received the client reminder created without applying nudge theory (Fig. 2a). While this reminder included detailed information, including the risks and benefits of undertaking hepatitis virus screening, the reminder was wordy (342 words) and difficult to read.
Group A (n = 655) received the client reminder designed using nudge-theory (Fig. 2b), specifically the “EAST” framework [17] created by the UK’s Behavioural Insights Team (BIT). According to BIT, the four principles for effective behavior change are as follows: easy, attractive, social, and timely. Specifically, our study focused on the principles of easy and attractive. To simplify the message, as recommended by the BIT executive report, information on the risks of hepatitis and the benefits of hepatitis virus screening was removed, reducing the word count to 152 words [17]. Also, the discounted cost was emphasized by striking out the original cost of screening (JPY 2040) and using capital letters for the new cost (JPY 612).

Hepatitis virus screening free of charge

Although the hepatitis virus screening cost of the control group and group A was JPY 612, group B (n = 307) was offered the hepatitis virus screening free of charge (shown in Fig. 3).
For group B, the reminder was written in the same format as that of group A, except that the hepatitis virus screening was indicated as free of charge (Fig. 3).

Data collection and measures

Primary outcome

The primary outcome was hepatitis virus screening rate. The healthcare provider checked the number of employees who had undergone the hepatitis virus screening using a standard record-keeping process.

Statistical analysis

We described the sex and age distributions of the participants, and we performed a chi-squared test to measure any differences among intervention groups. We quantified the difference in hepatitis virus screening among groups using a chi-squared test with a Bonferroni correction. These analyses were performed using Python 3.6 (The Python Software Foundation, Delaware, USA). Also, we calculated risk ratios. In addition, a regression analysis was performed using generalized linear mixed models to see if significant differences exist even after adjusting for unobservable heterogeneity per cluster. Calculation of risk ratios and the regression analysis were performed using R 4.0.2 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).

Economic analysis

Cost-effectiveness was calculated using the incremental cost-effectiveness ratio (ICER), expressed in terms of JPY per one additional person screened.
$$ \mathrm{ICER}=\frac{\mathrm{Difference}\ \mathrm{in}\ \mathrm{costs}\ \mathrm{between}\ \mathrm{the}\ \mathrm{control}\ \mathrm{group}\ \mathrm{and}\ \mathrm{the}\ \mathrm{in}\mathrm{tervention}\ \mathrm{group}\mathrm{s}\ \left(\mathrm{group}\ \mathrm{A}\ \mathrm{or}\ \mathrm{group}\ \mathrm{B}\right)}{\mathrm{Difference}\ \mathrm{in}\ \mathrm{number}\ \mathrm{of}\ \mathrm{screening}\ \mathrm{takers}\ \mathrm{between}\ \mathrm{the}\ \mathrm{control}\ \mathrm{group}\ \mathrm{and}\ \mathrm{the}\ \mathrm{in}\mathrm{tervention}\ \mathrm{group}\mathrm{s}\ \left(\mathrm{group}\ \mathrm{A}\ \mathrm{or}\ \mathrm{B}\right)} $$
Costs include the design and printing fees for the client reminder, and, in the case of group B, the cost of making screening free of charge.

Results

Baseline characteristics of general health checkup takers

Table 1 presents the baseline demographic characteristics of the three study groups. No significant differences among the groups were observed by age and gender.
Table 1
The baseline characteristics of the participants in the general health checkup
 
Control group
Group A
Group B
p value*
 
n
(%)
n
(%)
n
(%)
Sex
 Male
506
94.8%
619
94.5%
291
94.8%
0.97
 Female
28
5.2%
36
5.5%
16
5.2%
Age
 30s
73
13.7%
93
14.2%
47
15.3%
0.24
 40s
271
50.7%
308
47.0%
130
42.3%
 50s
144
27.0%
189
28.9%
97
31.6%
 60s
40
7.5%
51
7.8%
31
10.1%
 70s
6
1.1%
14
2.1%
2
0.7%
*There was no significant difference among the three groups in sex and age proportion by chi-squared test

Effectiveness of interventions on the hepatitis virus screening rates

Figure 4 shows the hepatitis virus screening rates by intervention. While the hepatitis virus screening rate among the control group was 21.2%, group A (who received the nudge-based client reminder) had a 15.9% higher screening rate at 37.1%. The largest difference was observed between group A and group B, where the hepatitis virus screening rate for group B (who were offered the hepatitis virus screening free of charge) was 49.2% higher at 86.3%. The effect of the intervention, expressed as the risk ratio to the control group, was 1.75 (95% CI 1.45–2.12) for group A and 4.08 (95% CI 3.44–4.83) for group B.
Also, to examine whether significant differences exist even after adjusting for unobservable heterogeneity per cluster, a regression analysis was performed using generalized linear mixed models. Table 2 shows the estimation results of the generalized linear mixed models. The variables “Group A” and “Group B” represent the effect of each intervention. The parameters were significantly positive for both group A and group B (both p < 0.001), even after adjusting for heterogeneity due to clusters.
Table 2
The result of regression using the generalized linear mixed models
 
β
SE
95% CI
p value
Intercept
− 2.241
0.437
(− 3.121, − 1.391)
< 0.00001
Group A
1.109
0.312
(0.480, 1.812)
0.0004
Group B
3.344
0.372
(2.595, 4.170)
< 0.00001
Female
1.211
0.265
(0.696, 1.737)
< 0.00001
Age
0.015
0.008
(0.000, 0.030)
0.0489
n = 1496. SE is standard error, and CI is confidence interval

Cost-effectiveness of each intervention on hepatitis virus screening rates using the ICER calculation

Table 3 shows the cost-effectiveness of each intervention, where a lower ICER value represents greater cost-effectiveness. While the ICER for group B is JPY 1168.7, the ICER of group A is JPY 172.5.
Table 3
The cost-effectiveness of the interventions
 
Control
A
B
Cost for design of reminder
(1) Design fee (JPY)
0
20,000
20,000
Cost for all employees
(2) Number of employees in each group
534
655
307
(3) Printing per person (JPY)
20
20
20
(4) Subtotal cost ((1) + (2) × (3)) (JPY)
10,680
33,100
26,140
Cost for only screening takers
(5) Number of screening takers
113
243
265
(6) Subsidization cost for making one screening free of charge (applied only to screening takers) (JPY)
0
0
612
(7) Subtotal cost ((5) × (6)) (JPY)
0
0
162,180
Total cost
(8) Total cost ((4) + (7)) (JPY)
10,680
33,100
188,320
Effectiveness
(9) Additional cost (JPY)
reference
22,420
177,640
(10) Number of additional screening takers
reference
130
152
(11) ICER ((9)/10) (JPY/1 additional screening taker)
reference
172.5
1,168.7

HBV and HCV screening results

No employee from any of the three groups tested positive for the hepatitis B virus. Additionally, the number of employees from the sample test positive for the hepatitis C virus was 1 in the control group (0.2%), 0 in group A (0%), and 4 in group B (1.3%).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate the effect of full subsidies and nudge theory on hepatitis screening rates in Japanese worksites. These findings suggest that (i) making screenings free of charge led to the greatest increase in screening uptake, potentially saving many lives and (ii) modifying client reminders using nudge theory could produce a substantial increase in screening uptake at lower costs, making it a viable option in limited-resource settings.

Findings

Fully subsidized hepatitis virus screening had the highest screening rate of 86.3%, compared to 37.1% for the nudge-based reminder only and 21.2% for the control reminder. The effect of the intervention, expressed as the risk ratio to the control reminder, was 4.08 (95% CI 3.44–4.83) for full subsidy and 1.75 (95% CI 1.45–2.12) for the nudge-based reminder only. In addition, the parameters were significantly positive for both group A and group B, even after adjusting for heterogeneity due to clusters. Previous studies on cancer screening also showed a rise in screening uptake when fully subsidized [1820]. However, in our study, the cost of screening was only JPY 612 in the nudge-based client reminder only group, yet screening was 49.2% lower. This suggests that requiring a co-payment, albeit small, could discourage many people from undergoing screening. Therefore, if feasible, removing co-payments could promote screening in hard-to-reach populations.
Our study also suggests that applying nudge theory to client reminders increased the hepatitis virus screening rates in these worksites. In particular, our results suggest that providing too much information might reduce the readability of the message, while using nudge-based reminders may increase screening rates in this context. Likewise, a previous study demonstrated that nudge-based client reminders increased colorectal cancer screening rates [14]. To design the nudge-based reminders in this study, we referred to the EAST framework proposed by the UK BIT in their executive report [17], which consists of four principles: (i) “Easy,” (ii) “Attractive,” (iii) “Social,” and (iv) “Timely”. This study followed the principles of “Easy” and “Attractive”. UK BIT recommends, as part of the “Easy” principle, to simplify messages and make them easy to understand. In line with this recommendation, we reduced the number of words on the client reminder. The “Attractive” principle emphasized the importance of drawing attention to important aspects of a message. To this end, the discounted cost of the screening was made salient by striking out the original cost of screening (JPY 2040) and using capital letters for the new cost (JPY 612).
Although making the hepatitis virus screening free produced the highest hepatitis virus screening rates, this led to additional costs. Thus, we performed an incremental cost-effectiveness ratio (ICER) to evaluate the cost-effectiveness of these interventions. The ICER was JPY 1168.7 and JPY 172.5 for the full subsidies and nudge-based reminder only, respectively. Therefore, simply applying nudge theory could significantly increase hepatitis virus screening at lower costs per person, which is critical in low resource settings where offering free screening is not feasible [21, 22]. This finding has implications even at the policy level. In 2011, the Japanese government decided to offer a free coupon for hepatitis virus screening, spending as much as JPY 3.3 billion yen. However, screening increased only by 446,000 people compared to previous year [23, 24]. If the nudge-based reminder were used, it could have cost substantially less and with better cost-effectiveness.
These results also have implications that could affect social implementation. In our study, the ICER of group A is smaller than that of group B. However, it is possible for this relationship to be reversed if the design costs (more generally speaking, fixed costs) are very large. In fact, for the current sample size, number of screening takers, unit cost of full subsidy, and printing costs, that reversal will happen when the design cost exceeds JPY 914,789. On the other hand, a larger design cost means that the absolute value of the ICER will increase in both groups A and B, which means that the financial burden on the insurer will be greater than in the present case. In light of this, the intervention should be implemented with as little design or fixed costs as possible, and, in such cases, the nudge-based reminder only will most likely be more cost-effective.
Even though there were approximately 17 million JHIA subscribers in 2018, only 2 million people received hepatitis testing by the end of 2018 [25]. Therefore, we estimated the impact of scaling up the nudge-based client reminder intervention to all 17 million members of JHIA subscribers if screening rates for both HBV and HCV increased by 16% as shown in our study:
1.
The total cost to send nudge-based client reminders will be 341 million yen (20 yen per person for printing and 20,000 yen for designing).
 
2.
Approximately 16,408 HBV carriers and 8204 HCV carriers could be identified (infection rates among the general population for HBV and HCV are estimated at 0.6% and 0.3%, respectively, according to literature) [26, 27].
 
3.
If all the HCV carriers complete their treatment, an estimated 5824 cases of liver cirrhosis could be prevented (approximately 71% of HCV carriers develop liver cirrhosis [28]).
 
4.
About 1514 liver cancer cases can be prevented (26% of HCV liver cirrhosis cases typically develop into liver cancer [29]).
 

Limitations

This research has several limitations. First, this study was conducted on employees who applied for and attended general health checkups. Because of the nature of these participants, hepatitis virus screening behavior might differ from that of the general population or from employees who did not sign up for the general health checkups. Second, the positive rates for HCV (0.2%, 0%, and 1.3% for the control group, group A, and group B, respectively) and the HBV (0% for all three groups) were different from our earlier expectations of 0.6% for HBV, and 0.3% for HCV based on previous studies [26]. However, the limited reliability of our measured positive rates due to a small sample size makes it difficult to draw valid comparisons. Third, the demographic data of the sample population is limited to sex and age because the transportation company is not permitted under the Industrial Safety and Health Act to obtain the results of optional tests since companies could identify employees who tested positive and put them at a disadvantage [16]. Hence, further studies are needed to address these limitations. Fourth, the analysis only included 13 clusters. However, after adjusting for heterogeneity of each cluster, our study found significant intervention effects. Nevertheless, increasing the number of clusters might allow us for more precise estimations of effects.

Conclusions

Full subsidies led to the highest increase in hepatitis virus screening rates and could encourage hard-to-reach populations to undergo screening. However, when full subsidies cannot be afforded by health insurance associations in Japan, most of which are facing severe financial deficits, revising the message of the client reminder using nudge theory could be a more cost-effective means for increasing hepatitis virus screening rates.

Acknowledgments

We thank Mr. Takao Takeshita, Mr. Shoichiro Habu, and Mr. Tomoaki Tahara from Japan Anti-tuberculosis Association for helping us with collecting and interpreting the data.
This study analyzes the data from a trial program, in which a transportation company conducted the hepatitis prevention activities to address the concerns that the company’s employees might have many undiagnosed hepatitis virus infections. To the employees, it was declared on the reminders that the results of this trial might be used for research purposes after anonymization and, for those who did not want to participate, their data could be excluded from the analysis upon their requests. For this analysis, approval was obtained from the Institutional Review Board of the National Center for Global Health and Medicine (No. 2216).
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Bray F, Ferlay J, Soerjomataram I, et al. Global cancer statistics 2018: GLOBOCAN estimates of incidence and mortality worldwide for 36 cancers in 185 countries. CA Cancer J Clin. 2018;68:394–424.CrossRef Bray F, Ferlay J, Soerjomataram I, et al. Global cancer statistics 2018: GLOBOCAN estimates of incidence and mortality worldwide for 36 cancers in 185 countries. CA Cancer J Clin. 2018;68:394–424.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Kudo M, Izumi N, Ichida T, et al. Report of the 19th follow-up survey of primary liver cancer in Japan. Hepatol Res. 2016;46:372–90.CrossRef Kudo M, Izumi N, Ichida T, et al. Report of the 19th follow-up survey of primary liver cancer in Japan. Hepatol Res. 2016;46:372–90.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Oza N, Isoda H, Ono T, et al. Current activities and future directions of comprehensive hepatitis control measures in Japan: the supportive role of the Hepatitis Information Center in building a solid foundation. Hepatol Res. 2017;47:487–96.CrossRef Oza N, Isoda H, Ono T, et al. Current activities and future directions of comprehensive hepatitis control measures in Japan: the supportive role of the Hepatitis Information Center in building a solid foundation. Hepatol Res. 2017;47:487–96.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Sugiyama A, Fujii T, Nagashima S, et al. Pilot study for hepatitis virus screening among employees as an effective approach to encourage employees who screened positive to receive medical care in Japan. Hepatol Res. 2018;48:291–302.CrossRef Sugiyama A, Fujii T, Nagashima S, et al. Pilot study for hepatitis virus screening among employees as an effective approach to encourage employees who screened positive to receive medical care in Japan. Hepatol Res. 2018;48:291–302.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Brouwers MC, De Vito C, Bahirathan L, et al. What implementation interventions increase cancer screening rates? A systematic review. Implement Sci. 2011;6:111.CrossRef Brouwers MC, De Vito C, Bahirathan L, et al. What implementation interventions increase cancer screening rates? A systematic review. Implement Sci. 2011;6:111.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Ueda Y, Sobue T, Morimoto A, et al. Evaluation of a free-coupon program for cervical cancer screening among the young: a nationally funded program conducted by a local government in Japan. J Epidemiol. 2015;25:50–6.CrossRef Ueda Y, Sobue T, Morimoto A, et al. Evaluation of a free-coupon program for cervical cancer screening among the young: a nationally funded program conducted by a local government in Japan. J Epidemiol. 2015;25:50–6.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee JK, Groessl EJ, Ganiats TG, et al. Cost-effectiveness of a mailed educational reminder to increase colorectal cancer screening. BMC Gastroenterol. 2011;11:93.CrossRef Lee JK, Groessl EJ, Ganiats TG, et al. Cost-effectiveness of a mailed educational reminder to increase colorectal cancer screening. BMC Gastroenterol. 2011;11:93.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Sun Y, Sarma EA, Moyer A, et al. Promoting mammography screening among Chinese American women using a message-framing intervention. Patient Educ Couns. 2015;98(7):878–83.CrossRef Sun Y, Sarma EA, Moyer A, et al. Promoting mammography screening among Chinese American women using a message-framing intervention. Patient Educ Couns. 2015;98(7):878–83.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Ferrer RA, Klein WM, Zajac LE, et al. An affective booster moderates the effect of gain- and loss-framed messages on behavioral intentions for colorectal cancer screening. J Behav Med. 2012;35:452–61.CrossRef Ferrer RA, Klein WM, Zajac LE, et al. An affective booster moderates the effect of gain- and loss-framed messages on behavioral intentions for colorectal cancer screening. J Behav Med. 2012;35:452–61.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Daniel JO, Jakob DJ. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease detection behaviors: a meta-analytic review. J Commun. 2009;59:296–316.CrossRef Daniel JO, Jakob DJ. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease detection behaviors: a meta-analytic review. J Commun. 2009;59:296–316.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Thaler R, Sunstein C. Nudge. Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven: Yale University Press; 2008. Thaler R, Sunstein C. Nudge. Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven: Yale University Press; 2008.
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Misawa D, Fukuyoshi J, Sengoku S. Cancer prevention using machine learning, nudge theory and social impact bond. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17:790.CrossRef Misawa D, Fukuyoshi J, Sengoku S. Cancer prevention using machine learning, nudge theory and social impact bond. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17:790.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Okeke EN, Adepiti CA, Ajenifuja KO. What is the price of prevention? New evidence from a field experiment. J Health Econ. 2012;32(1):207–18.CrossRef Okeke EN, Adepiti CA, Ajenifuja KO. What is the price of prevention? New evidence from a field experiment. J Health Econ. 2012;32(1):207–18.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Skaer TL, Robinson LM, Sclar DA, Harding GH. Financial incentive and the use of mammography among Hispanic migrants to the United States. Health Care Women Int. 1996;17(4):281–91.CrossRef Skaer TL, Robinson LM, Sclar DA, Harding GH. Financial incentive and the use of mammography among Hispanic migrants to the United States. Health Care Women Int. 1996;17(4):281–91.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Kiefe CI, McKay SV, Halevy A, Brody BA. Is cost a barrier to screening mammography for low-income women receiving Medicare benefits? A randomized trial. Arch Intern Med. 1994;154(11):1217–24.CrossRef Kiefe CI, McKay SV, Halevy A, Brody BA. Is cost a barrier to screening mammography for low-income women receiving Medicare benefits? A randomized trial. Arch Intern Med. 1994;154(11):1217–24.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Fukuma S, Ikenoue T, Sasaki S, Saigusa Y, Misumi T, Saito Y, Yamada Y, Goto R, Taguri M. Nudging patients with chronic kidney disease at screening to visit physicians: a protocol of a pragmatic randomized controlled trial. Contemp Clin Trials Commun. 2019;16:100429.CrossRef Fukuma S, Ikenoue T, Sasaki S, Saigusa Y, Misumi T, Saito Y, Yamada Y, Goto R, Taguri M. Nudging patients with chronic kidney disease at screening to visit physicians: a protocol of a pragmatic randomized controlled trial. Contemp Clin Trials Commun. 2019;16:100429.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Ishikawa Y, Hirai K, Saito H, et al. Cost-effectiveness of a tailored intervention designed to increase breast cancer screening among a non-adherent population: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:760.CrossRef Ishikawa Y, Hirai K, Saito H, et al. Cost-effectiveness of a tailored intervention designed to increase breast cancer screening among a non-adherent population: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:760.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Korenaga M, Ooe C, Kamimura K, et al. Tailored message interventions using social marketing approach increase the number of participants in viral hepatitis screening for Japanese workers -multicenter trial of 880,000 general checkup applicants. Hepatology. 2019;70(Suppl 1):460A. Korenaga M, Ooe C, Kamimura K, et al. Tailored message interventions using social marketing approach increase the number of participants in viral hepatitis screening for Japanese workers -multicenter trial of 880,000 general checkup applicants. Hepatology. 2019;70(Suppl 1):460A.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Seeff LB. Natural history of chronic hepatitis C. Hepatology. 2002;36(5 Suppl 1):S35–46.PubMed Seeff LB. Natural history of chronic hepatitis C. Hepatology. 2002;36(5 Suppl 1):S35–46.PubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Ikeda K, Saitoh S, Suzuki Y, et al. Disease progression and hepatocellular carcinogenesis in patients with chronic viral hepatitis: a prospective observation of 2215 patients. J Hepatol. 1998;28:930–8.CrossRef Ikeda K, Saitoh S, Suzuki Y, et al. Disease progression and hepatocellular carcinogenesis in patients with chronic viral hepatitis: a prospective observation of 2215 patients. J Hepatol. 1998;28:930–8.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Increasing hepatitis virus screening uptake at worksites in Japan using nudge theory and full subsidies
verfasst von
Jun Fukuyoshi
Masaaki Korenaga
Yui Yoshii
Lek Hong
Soichiro Kashihara
Byron Sigel
Toru Takebayashi
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2021
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine / Ausgabe 1/2021
Print ISSN: 1342-078X
Elektronische ISSN: 1347-4715
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12199-021-00940-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe