Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 1/2016

Open Access 01.12.2016 | Research

Limited impact of Cntn4 mutation on autism-related traits in developing and adult C57BL/6J mice

verfasst von: Remco T. Molenhuis, Hilgo Bruining, Esther Remmelink, Leonie de Visser, Maarten Loos, J. Peter H. Burbach, Martien J. H. Kas

Erschienen in: Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders | Ausgabe 1/2016

Abstract

Background

Mouse models offer an essential tool to unravel the impact of genetic mutations on autism-related phenotypes. The behavioral impact of some important candidate gene models for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has not yet been studied, and existing characterizations mostly describe behavioral phenotypes at adult ages, disregarding the developmental nature of the disorder. In this context, the behavioral influence of CNTN4, one of the strongest suggested ASD candidate genes, is unknown. Here, we used our recently established developmental test battery to characterize the consequences of disruption of contactin 4 (Cntn4) on neurological, sensory, cognitive, and behavioral phenotypes across different developmental stages.

Methods

C57BL/6J mice with heterozygous and homozygous disruption of Cntn4 were studied through an extensive, partially longitudinal, test battery at various developmental stages, including various paradigms testing social and restricted repetitive behaviors.

Results

Developmental neurological and cognitive screenings revealed no significant differences between genotypes, and ASD-related behavioral domains were also unchanged in Cntn4-deficient versus wild-type mice. The impact of Cntn4-deficiency was found to be limited to increased startle responsiveness following auditory stimuli of different high amplitudes in heterozygous and homozygous Cntn4-deficient mice and enhanced acquisition in a spatial learning task in homozygous mice.

Conclusions

Disruption of Cntn4 in the C57BL/6J background does not affect specific autism-related phenotypes in developing or adult mice but causes subtle non-disorder specific changes in sensory behavioral responses and cognitive performance.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​s11689-016-9140-2) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

RTM participated in the design of the study, carried out the experiments and genotyping, analyzed the data, interpreted the results, and wrote the manuscript. HB provided input into the study design, interpreted the results, and wrote the manuscript. ER and LV participated in the design of the study and coordination of the experiments. ML participated in the design of the study, data analysis, and interpretation of the results. JPHB aid in the genotyping and participated in the interpretation of the results. MJHK contributed to the design of the study, development of the methods, coordination of the statistical analyses, and interpretation of the data. All authors contributed to the reviewing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Abkürzungen
ASD
autism spectrum disorder
eSHIRPA
extended SmithKline Beecham, Harwell, Imperial College and Royal London Hospital phenotype assessment
PPI
pre-pulse inhibition

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a behaviorally defined developmental disorder with a strong genetic component [1, 2]. The identification of genetic risk factors such as common genetic variants, rare inherited and de novo mutations have lead to the implication of hundreds of different genes [3, 4]. These findings illustrate the complexity and heterogeneity of the genetic architecture of ASD.
Subsequently, mouse models are being used to unravel the functional impact of implicated genes on ASD phenotypes in a controlled genetic and environmental background. However, knowledge of the impact on cognitive and behavioral development of the majority of these genes is missing or incomplete [5], and most behavioral characterizations of animal models are limited to adult phenotypes disregarding the developmental nature of ASD [6].
Here, we characterize the impact of contactin 4 (Cntn4) null mutation on behavioral development, using our recently developed longitudinal test battery for mice that tests a wide array of neurological, cognitive, and behavioral parameters across development starting from 3 weeks of age [6].
CNTN4 is an axonal glycoprotein belonging to the contactin family, a six-member subgroup of the immunoglobulin superfamily of cell adhesion molecules [7]. CNTN4 is known to act as an axon guidance molecule in the establishment of olfactory neural circuitry during neural development [8] and promotes target-specific axon arborization of a subset of retinal ganglion cells onto the nucleus of the optic tract [9]. Knowledge of the neurobiological functions of CNTN4 in normal and abnormal development of brain systems are far from complete.
The CNTN4 gene has been implicated in ASD due to its presence in the genetic locus of the 3p-deletion syndrome, a mental retardation syndrome [10]. Subsequently, evidence for a role of CNTN4 has been accumulated [1114] but has also been questioned [15, 16]. Three cases carrying a copy number variant (CNV) in the CNTN4 gene were reported by the Autism Genome Project Consortium (AGP) [12]. Deletions and duplications in the CNTN4 gene or its promoter region were found in 10 families of the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange (AGRE) collection [13]. In the Autism Case-Control cohort (ACC), deletion in the promoter region of CNTN4 was found in three cases but not in controls [13]. Association of CNTN4 with developmental disorders such as ASD further seems supported by the protein’s neurobiological functions [2, 7].
Follwoing these observations, we performed a careful longitudinal functional characterization of homozygous and heterozygous disruptions of Cntn4 to resolve the impact of this gene on behavioral and cognitive development.

Methods

Generation and breeding of Cntn4 mice

Cntn4-deficient mice were kindly provided by Dr. Yoshihiro Yoshihara (RIKEN, Japan) [8]. These mice were generated using a standard gene-targeting method as previously described. A targeting vector was designated to mutate the translation start codon (ATG) in the exon 2 of the Cntn4 gene into a stop codon (TAG) and introduce a pgk-neo selection marker. Consequently, these mice were backcrossed with C57BL/6 mice more than nine times. Upon arrival in the University Medical Center Utrecht, the mice were re-derived, followed by heterozygous breeding for the use in our experiments.
All animals were born and weaned at the University Medical Center Utrecht. Average nest size of Cntn4 litters was 7.2, and the litters larger than 10 animals per litter were culled back to average 7. The minimum litter size used for behavioral experiments was four animals per litter. Detailed information on the genotyping of Cntn4 mice is provided in Additional file 1.
Given the extensive number of tests, the mice were spread over four different batches. Table 1 provides the order of behavioral testing and the number of animals per genotype per batch. Phenotypic assessments in batches 1, 2, and 3 were performed at the University Medical Center Utrecht. Batch 4 was transported and tested at Sylics (Synaptologics BV, Amsterdam, The Netherlands).
Table 1
Overview of the behavioral tests per batch
Age
Task
Batch
Subjects (n)
3 weeks
Juvenile social interaction
Batch 1 + 2
6–7 genotype-matched pairs per genotype
4 weeks
Extended SHIRPA screen
Batch 1 + 2
19–26 per genotype
6 weeks
Extended SHIRPA screen
Batch 1 + 2
19–26 per genotype
8 weeks
Extended SHIRPA screen
Batch 1 + 2
19–26 per genotype
Adult
Extended SHIRPA screen
Batch 1
10–15 per genotype
 
Open field
Batch 1
10–15 per genotype
 
Elevated plus maze
Batch 1
10–15 per genotype
 
Social discrimination
Batch 1
10–15 per genotype
 
Buried food test
Batch 2
9–11 per genotype
 
Set shifting-reversal task
Batch 2
9–11 per genotype
 
Social approach in 3-chamber
Batch 3
12 per genotype
 
Novel object exploration task
Batch 3
12 per genotype
 
Barnes maze-reversal task
Batch 4
16 per genotype
 
Pre-pulse inhibition
Batch 4
16 per genotype
Animals in batch 1 + 2 were weaned at post-natal day 21. Batch 3 + 4 animals were weaned at post-natal day 28
All experiments were approved by the ethical committee for animal experimentation of the University Medical Center Utrecht and Free University Amsterdam and performed according to the institutional guidelines that are in full compliance with the European Council Directive (86/609/EEC).

Developmental neurological and behavioral screening

Cntn4 −/− , Cntn4 +/− , and wild-type male littermates were subjected to our previously described longitudinal screening strategy (extended SHIRPA battery) testing an array of neurological, behavioral, and cognitive parameters at 4, 6, 8, and 11 weeks of age [6, 17]. The longitudinal test battery includes the assessment of autism-related traits such as motor stereotypies (e.g., self-grooming) and sensorimotor coordination (e.g., latency to fall from the rotarod). Detailed information on behavioral testing is provided in Additional file 1.

Screening of social behaviors and restricted repetitive behaviors

Abnormalities in social interaction behaviors were assessed in the juvenile social interaction test (3 weeks of age) [18], followed by a three-chamber social approach [18], and a 2-day social discrimination paradigm in adult age animals [19]. Stereotypic movements, restricted interests, and repetitive patterns of behavior were analyzed in the novel object investigation task during exposure to four novel toys [20]. Cognitive flexibility was assessed by multi-trial associative learning in an extensive set-shifting paradigm [6], as well as a Barnes maze spatial learning task including reversal [21]. Acoustic startle response and sensorimotor gating were assessed in the pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) test [19]. Anxiety-related behaviors were tested in the elevated plus maze and open field [6]. Statistical analyses are described in the Additional file 1.

Results

eSHIRPA assays

The eSHIRPA (extended SmithKline Beecham, Harwell, Imperial College and Royal London Hospital phenotype assessment) screen did not show differences between the Cntn4 +/− mice, Cntn4 −/− mice, and wild-type controls at 4, 6, 8, or 10 weeks of age in general health, body weight, and neurological reflexes nor in the development of various locomotor parameters including total distance moved, movement velocity, and movement duration (Figs. 1ad, Table 2). Moreover, we found no developmental differences in the amount of self-grooming or sensorimotor coordination on the rotarod (Figs. 1ef).
Table 2
Physical and neurological features during the different phases of the extended SHIRPA primary screen.
Age (weeks)
4
6
8
Adult
Test
WT
+/−
−/−
Sig.
WT
+/−
−/−
Sig.
WT
+/−
−/−
Sig.
WT
+/−
−/−
Sig.
Subjects (n)
19
26
22
 
19
26
22
 
19
26
22
 
13
15
10
 
Body position (active)
95
96
100
0.22
100
100
100
1.00
100
96
100
0.46
100
100
100
1.00
Body position (inactive)
0
4
0
 
0
0
0
 
0
4
0
 
0
0
0
 
Body position (excessive activity)
5
0
0
 
0
0
0
 
0
0
0
 
0
0
0
 
Tremor (absent)
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Palpebral closure (eyes open)
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Coat appearance tidy and groomed
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Whiskers (present)
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Lacrimation (absent)
100
96
100
0.45
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Defecation (quantity)
1.9
1.6
2.0
0.47
3.1
2.5
1.9
0.06
2.8
2.3
2.4
0.61
3.8
3.7
3.2
0.70
± SEM
0.3
0.3
0.3
 
0.3
0.3
0.3
 
0.4
0.3
0.4
 
0.7
0.7
0.9
 
Transfer arousal (brief freeze)
11
15
14
0.70
11
19
9
0.71
11
8
0
0.16
8
0
10
0.50
Transfer arousal (immediate movement)
89
85
86
 
89
81
91
 
89
92
100
 
92
100
90
 
Gait (fluid)
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Tail elevation (horizontal extension)
100
100
100
1.00
95
100
100
0.28
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Tail elevation (straub tail)
0
0
0
 
5
0
0
 
0
0
0
 
0
0
0
 
Startle response (preyer reflex)
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Touch escape (flight prior to touch)
79
73
77
0.88
89
85
73
0.35
63
88
68
0.05
85
87
100
0.45
Touch escape (response to touch)
21
27
23
 
11
15
27
 
37
12
32
 
15
13
0
 
Positional passivity (struggles)
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Skin color (pink)
95
100
100
0.28
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Skin color (blanched)
5
0
0
 
0
0
0
 
0
0
0
 
0
0
0
 
Trunk curl (absent)
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Limb grasping (absent)
100
92
100
0.20
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Pinna reflex (present)
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Corneal reflex (present)
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Contact righting reflex (present)
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Evidence of biting (none)
79
77
77
0.99
84
88
100
0.18
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Grip (OK)
100
96
100
0.36
95
96
100
0.59
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
90
0.24
Full puberty
89
81
100
0.30
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
100
100
100
1.00
Cntn4 +/−Cntn4 −/−, and wild-type (WT) control mice were screened in the Perspex jar for body position (active, inactive, or excessively active), tremor (present or not), palpebral closure (eyes open or not), coat appearance (well groomed or irregularities like piloerection), whiskers (intact or trimmed), and lacrimation (present or not). In the arena, the mice were screened for transfer arousal (freezing or immediate movement), gait (fluid or not), tail elevation (dragging, horizontal, or straub tail), startle response (preyer reflex, no response, or additional response), and touch escape (response to touch or flight prior to touch while finger approaches). Mice were transferred out of the arena to observe positional passivity (struggling by different types of handling), skin color (color of plantar surface of forelimbs), trunk curl (forward curling with head to abdomen), limb grasping (clasping of rear limbs), pinna reflex (presence of ear retraction), corneal reflex (presence of eyeblink), contact righting reflex, evidence of biting, grip (grasping of grid), vocalization, and puberty (presence of sex organs). Sig. represents statistical significance of between-genotype differences. Data are presented as a percentage of the total number of animals per genotypes, except for defecation (count of the fecal boli)

Social interaction behavior

The juvenile social interaction test revealed no differences in the amount of social sniffing, anogenital sniffing, or social grooming (Fig. 1g) between the Cntn4 +/− , Cntn4 −/− , and wild-type control mice. In the adult three-chamber test, all genotypes showed a clear preference for exploration of a mouse over an object, and no genotype differences in the amount of social exploration were observed (Fig. 1h). Furthermore, genotypes were equally capable to distinguish between a familiar mouse and a novel mouse in a social recognition paradigm, both at 5 min and 24 h after initial exposure (Fig. 1i).

Restricted and repetitive behaviors

There were no differences in the grooming behavior between the Cntn4 +/− , Cntn4 −/− , and wild-type control mice at adult age, in line with the amount of grooming observed in the longitudinal eSHIRPA screening (Fig. 2a). We also found no genotype differences in restricted interest or in repetitive patterns of behavior in the novel object investigation task during the exploration of the four novel toys (Fig. 2b, c).
We additionally analyzed cognitive flexibility through the assessment of reversal learning in a set-shifting task. Prior to this task, we ascertained intact olfactory capacities, as we found no genotype differences in the latency to find a buried piece of food. In the set-shifting task, all genotypes were equally able to associate a food reward with a specific digging material or odor, as was evident through the performance of simple, compound discrimination and intra- and extra-dimensional shifts (Fig. 2d).
The reversal-learning phase of this test yielded an inconclusive result, as the genotype effect that we observed on reversal learning was only significant (ANOVA p = 0.04) in one of the two outcome measures (i.e., the number of trials to reach the criterion but not in errors to reach criterion). Moreover, a shift cost in wild-type mice was observed in errors to reach criterion, although not in the number of trials to reach criterion. Given this inconclusive result, we tested reversal-learning performance in a different paradigm. In this Barnes maze reversal-learning paradigm, we confirmed that Cntn4 does not affect reversal learning, as all genotypes needed equal amount of time as well as distance before reaching the re-located escape hole (Fig. 2f). In contrast to the set-shifting test, we observed a shift cost that was observed in all genotypes in the reversal phase of the Barnes maze test (Fig. 2e, f).

Responses to sensory stimuli and anxiety-related behaviors

The startle response was consistently increased in the Cntn4 +/− and Cntn4 −/− mice at different high amplitudes, although the startle threshold was not significantly different between genotypes (Figs. 2g, h). No significant effects were found on pre-pulse inhibition at both inter-stimulus intervals of 30 and 100 ms (Figs. 2i, j).
The increased startle response to auditory stimuli of the different high amplitudes seemed not to result from increased anxiety levels, as Cntn4 +/− , Cntn4 −/− mice and wild-type controls did not differ in their elevated plus maze (Fig. 2k) and open-field exploratory behaviors (Figs. 2l).

Discussion

We present a comprehensive assessment of the impact of the ASD candidate gene CNTN4 on a variety of neurological, behavioral, and cognitive aspects of development. We found no effect of Cntn4 deficiency on ASD-related behavioral mouse paradigms such as the juvenile social interaction test, the three-chamber test, grooming behavior, and sensorimotor coordination. We also did not observe developmental neurological, behavioral, or cognitive abnormalities in the extended SHIRPA screen.
The reversal-learning phase of the set-shifting task yielded an inconclusive result, as no shift cost was observed in wild-type animals for the number of trials to reach criterion. Indeed, the genotype difference that was observed in the number of trials to reach the criterion in the reversal-learning phase was rather caused by lack of shift cost in wild-type animals than a reversal-learning deficit in the Cntn4 −/− mice. In line with this reasoning, when we compared the outcome of Cntn4 −/− mice in the reversal-learning phase with the results obtained for C57BL/6J mice obtained in a previous study, we find no genotype difference in this phase of the set-shifting task [6]. In the Barnes maze reversal-learning paradigm, we also did not find a reversal-learning deficit. Based on the summary of these data, we conclude that Cntn4 disruption has no effect on cognitive flexibility.
Non-ASD-specific effects at adult ages were indicated by increased startle response to auditory stimuli of different high amplitudes and by faster escape hole finding during subsequent days of acquisition in the Barnes maze.
Hyperresponsivity to acoustic stimuli is related to many neurodevelopmental disorders and has also been reported in ASD [22]. In addition, acoustic hyperresponsivity in patients with fragile X syndrome is known to be consistent with animal model data [23]. Similar to our findings, an increased startle response to acoustic stimuli was recently described in children with ASD [24], although these were in response to weak stimuli in contrast to the high amplitudes we found. The observed hyperresponsivity in Cntn4-deficient mice was unrelated to anxiety levels, as mice showed similar exploratory behaviors in classical anxiety behavioral tests, such as the elevated plus maze and open field. Although speculative, the increased startle response together with the enhanced acquisition in the Barnes maze could indicate that Cntn4 deficiency in the C57BL/6J background leads to a state of increased behavioral responsiveness without overt anxiety or avoidance behavior in mice [25].
Together, the findings show that in the C57BL/6J background, disruption of Cntn4 does not lead to substantial behavioral defects related to autistic development. A limited behavioral penetrance of CNTN4 mutations on autistic development may be consistent with a recent study that revisited the association of contactins, including CNTN4, with ASD [16]. The behavioral phenotype of Cntn4 mice contrasts other previously studied genetic ASD models [18]. For instance, Shank3 or Pten mice show extensive impairments in social interaction and sensorimotor phenotypes and are therefore regarded as translational models for ASD [2628].
Although ASD-related phenotypes in Cntn4-deficient mice were observed, our findings do have relevance for neurodevelopmental disorder research. The specific phenotypes observed in Cntn4-deficient mice may be used to study the mechanisms underlying increased responsiveness or vigilance, a trait observed across many different human disorders such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, and schizophrenia [2931]. Cntn4-deficient mice may serve as a model to study the mechanistic underpinnings of behavioral states in which vigilance is altered. Indeed, common single-nucleotide variants in the CNTN4 locus have recently been associated with other neuropsychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia [32], perhaps pointing to a non-disorder specific contribution of this cell adhesion gene in neuropsychiatric pathogenesis. In addition, a role for Cntn4, was recently shown in target-specific arborization during development of the accessory optic system [9]. Our study shows the importance of detailed developmental neurological, behavioral, and cognitive characterization of genetic animal models to complement human genetic studies in ASD and related disorders.

Conclusions

In our test battery, disruption of Cntn4, a prominent ASD candidate gene, had no effect on cognitive and behavioral development or ASD-specific phenotypes. At adult age, we could detect an effect of Cntn4 disruption on an adult sensory behavioral and a spatial cognitive feature.

Acknowledgements

We thank Y. Yoshihara for the generous gift of Cntn4-deficient mice, M. Brandt for the help with the three-chamber sociability test, H. Oppelaar and M. Broekhoven for the technical assistance, and A. Oguro-Ando for the discussion. This study was supported by a funding by the European Autism Interventions—A Multicentre Study for Developing New Medications (EU-AIMS) to Dr. Martien J. Kas. The research of EU-AIMS receives support from the Innovative Medicines Initiative Joint Undertaking under Grant agreement number 115300, resources of which are composed of financial contributions from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013), from the European Federation of Pharmaceutical Industries and Associations companies in-kind contributions, and from Autism Speaks.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

RTM participated in the design of the study, carried out the experiments and genotyping, analyzed the data, interpreted the results, and wrote the manuscript. HB provided input into the study design, interpreted the results, and wrote the manuscript. ER and LV participated in the design of the study and coordination of the experiments. ML participated in the design of the study, data analysis, and interpretation of the results. JPHB aid in the genotyping and participated in the interpretation of the results. MJHK contributed to the design of the study, development of the methods, coordination of the statistical analyses, and interpretation of the data. All authors contributed to the reviewing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Am. Psychiatr. Assoc.: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC; 2013. Am. Psychiatr. Assoc.: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC; 2013.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen JA, Peñagarikano O, Belgard TG, Swarup V, Geschwind DH. The emerging picture of autism spectrum disorder: genetics and pathology. Annu Rev Pathol: Mech Dis. 2015;10:111–44.CrossRef Chen JA, Peñagarikano O, Belgard TG, Swarup V, Geschwind DH. The emerging picture of autism spectrum disorder: genetics and pathology. Annu Rev Pathol: Mech Dis. 2015;10:111–44.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Pinto D, Delaby E, Merico D, Barbosa M, Merikangas A, Klei L, et al. Convergence of genes and cellular pathways dysregulated in autism spectrum disorders. Am J Hum Genet. 2014;94:677–94.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pinto D, Delaby E, Merico D, Barbosa M, Merikangas A, Klei L, et al. Convergence of genes and cellular pathways dysregulated in autism spectrum disorders. Am J Hum Genet. 2014;94:677–94.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
4.
Zurück zum Zitat De Rubeis S, He X, Goldberg AP, Poultney CS, Samocha K, Ercument Cicek A, et al. Synaptic, transcriptional and chromatin genes disrupted in autism. Nature. 2014;515:209–15.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral De Rubeis S, He X, Goldberg AP, Poultney CS, Samocha K, Ercument Cicek A, et al. Synaptic, transcriptional and chromatin genes disrupted in autism. Nature. 2014;515:209–15.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Abrahams BS, Arking DE, Campbell DB, Mefford HC, Morrow EM, Weiss LA, et al. SFARI Gene 2.0: a community-driven knowledgebase for the autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). Mol Autism. 2013;4:36.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Abrahams BS, Arking DE, Campbell DB, Mefford HC, Morrow EM, Weiss LA, et al. SFARI Gene 2.0: a community-driven knowledgebase for the autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). Mol Autism. 2013;4:36.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Molenhuis RT, de Visser L, Bruining H, Kas MJ. Enhancing the value of psychiatric mouse models; differential expression of developmental behavioral and cognitive profiles in four inbred strains of mice. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014;24:945–54.CrossRefPubMed Molenhuis RT, de Visser L, Bruining H, Kas MJ. Enhancing the value of psychiatric mouse models; differential expression of developmental behavioral and cognitive profiles in four inbred strains of mice. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014;24:945–54.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Zuko A, Kleijer KTE, Oguro-Ando A, Kas MJH, Van Daalen E, Van Der Zwaag B, et al. Contactins in the neurobiology of autism. Eur J Pharmacol. 2013;719:63–74.CrossRefPubMed Zuko A, Kleijer KTE, Oguro-Ando A, Kas MJH, Van Daalen E, Van Der Zwaag B, et al. Contactins in the neurobiology of autism. Eur J Pharmacol. 2013;719:63–74.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaneko-Goto T, Yoshihara S-I, Miyazaki H, Yoshihara Y. BIG-2 mediates olfactory axon convergence to target glomeruli. Neuron. 2008;57:834–46.CrossRefPubMed Kaneko-Goto T, Yoshihara S-I, Miyazaki H, Yoshihara Y. BIG-2 mediates olfactory axon convergence to target glomeruli. Neuron. 2008;57:834–46.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Osterhout JA, Stafford BK, Nguyen PL, Yoshihara Y, Huberman AD. Contactin-4 mediates axon-target specificity and functional development of the accessory optic system. Neuron. 2015;86:985–99.CrossRefPubMed Osterhout JA, Stafford BK, Nguyen PL, Yoshihara Y, Huberman AD. Contactin-4 mediates axon-target specificity and functional development of the accessory optic system. Neuron. 2015;86:985–99.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Fernandez T, Morgan T, Davis N, Klin A, Morris A, Farhi A, et al. Disruption of contactin 4 (CNTN4) results in developmental delay and other features of 3p deletion syndrome. Am J Hum Genet. 2004;74:1286–93.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fernandez T, Morgan T, Davis N, Klin A, Morris A, Farhi A, et al. Disruption of contactin 4 (CNTN4) results in developmental delay and other features of 3p deletion syndrome. Am J Hum Genet. 2004;74:1286–93.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Roohi J, Montagna C, Tegay DH, Palmer LE, DeVincent C, Pomeroy JC, et al. Disruption of contactin 4 in three subjects with autism spectrum disorder. J Med Genet. 2009;46:176–82.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Roohi J, Montagna C, Tegay DH, Palmer LE, DeVincent C, Pomeroy JC, et al. Disruption of contactin 4 in three subjects with autism spectrum disorder. J Med Genet. 2009;46:176–82.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Pinto D, Pagnamenta AT, Klei L, Anney R, Merico D, Regan R, et al. Functional impact of global rare copy number variation in autism spectrum disorders. Nature. 2010;466:368–72.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pinto D, Pagnamenta AT, Klei L, Anney R, Merico D, Regan R, et al. Functional impact of global rare copy number variation in autism spectrum disorders. Nature. 2010;466:368–72.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Glessner JT, Wang K, Cai G, Korvatska O, Kim CE, Wood S, et al. Autism genome-wide copy number variation reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature. 2009;459:569–73.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Glessner JT, Wang K, Cai G, Korvatska O, Kim CE, Wood S, et al. Autism genome-wide copy number variation reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature. 2009;459:569–73.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Guo H, Xun G, Peng Y, Xiang X, Xiong Z, Zhang L, et al. Disruption of contactin 4 in two subjects with autism in Chinese population. Gene. 2012;505:201–5.CrossRefPubMed Guo H, Xun G, Peng Y, Xiang X, Xiong Z, Zhang L, et al. Disruption of contactin 4 in two subjects with autism in Chinese population. Gene. 2012;505:201–5.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Cottrell CE, Bir N, Varga E, Alvarez CE, Bouyain S, Zernzach R, et al. Contactin 4 as an autism susceptibility locus. Autism Res. 2011;4:189–99.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cottrell CE, Bir N, Varga E, Alvarez CE, Bouyain S, Zernzach R, et al. Contactin 4 as an autism susceptibility locus. Autism Res. 2011;4:189–99.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Murdoch JD, Gupta AR, Sanders SJ, Walker MF, Keaney J, Fernandez TV, et al. No evidence for association of autism with rare heterozygous point mutations in contactin-associated protein-like 2 (CNTNAP2), or in other contactin-associated proteins or contactins. PLoS Genet. 2015;11:e1004852.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Murdoch JD, Gupta AR, Sanders SJ, Walker MF, Keaney J, Fernandez TV, et al. No evidence for association of autism with rare heterozygous point mutations in contactin-associated protein-like 2 (CNTNAP2), or in other contactin-associated proteins or contactins. PLoS Genet. 2015;11:e1004852.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Rogers DC, Jones DNC, Nelson PR, Jones CM, Quilter CA, Robinson TL, et al. Use of SHIRPA and discriminant analysis to characterise marked differences in the behavioural phenotype of six inbred mouse strains. Behav Brain Res. 1999;105:207–17.CrossRefPubMed Rogers DC, Jones DNC, Nelson PR, Jones CM, Quilter CA, Robinson TL, et al. Use of SHIRPA and discriminant analysis to characterise marked differences in the behavioural phenotype of six inbred mouse strains. Behav Brain Res. 1999;105:207–17.CrossRefPubMed
18.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Bruining H, Matsui A, Oguro-Ando A, Kahn RS, Spijker HM V’t, Akkermans G, et al. Genetic mapping in mice reveals the involvement of Pcdh9 in long-term social and object recognition, and sensorimotor development. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78:485–95.CrossRefPubMed Bruining H, Matsui A, Oguro-Ando A, Kahn RS, Spijker HM V’t, Akkermans G, et al. Genetic mapping in mice reveals the involvement of Pcdh9 in long-term social and object recognition, and sensorimotor development. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78:485–95.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Pearson BL, Pobbe RLH, Defensor EB, Oasay L, Bolivar VJ, Blanchard DC, et al. Motor and cognitive stereotypies in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse model of autism. Genes Brain Behav. 2011;10:228–35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pearson BL, Pobbe RLH, Defensor EB, Oasay L, Bolivar VJ, Blanchard DC, et al. Motor and cognitive stereotypies in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse model of autism. Genes Brain Behav. 2011;10:228–35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Seigers R, Loos M, Van Tellingen O, Boogerd W, Smit AB, Schagen SB. Cognitive impact of cytotoxic agents in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2015;232(1):17–37. Seigers R, Loos M, Van Tellingen O, Boogerd W, Smit AB, Schagen SB. Cognitive impact of cytotoxic agents in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2015;232(1):17–37.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Green S, Hernandez L, Tottenham N, Krasileva K, Bookheimer S, Dapretto M: Neurobiology of sensory overresponsivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(8):778-786. Green S, Hernandez L, Tottenham N, Krasileva K, Bookheimer S, Dapretto M: Neurobiology of sensory overresponsivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(8):778-786.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen L, Toth M. Fragile X mice develop sensory hyperreactivity to auditory stimuli. Neuroscience. 2001;103:1043–50.CrossRefPubMed Chen L, Toth M. Fragile X mice develop sensory hyperreactivity to auditory stimuli. Neuroscience. 2001;103:1043–50.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Takahashi H, Nakahachi T, Komatsu S, Ogino K, Iida Y, Kamio Y. Hyperreactivity to weak acoustic stimuli and prolonged acoustic startle latency in children with autism spectrum disorders. Mol Autism. 2014;5:23.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Takahashi H, Nakahachi T, Komatsu S, Ogino K, Iida Y, Kamio Y. Hyperreactivity to weak acoustic stimuli and prolonged acoustic startle latency in children with autism spectrum disorders. Mol Autism. 2014;5:23.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Blanchard DC, Griebel G, Blanchard RJ. Mouse defensive behaviors: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panic. Eur J Pharmacol. 2003;463:97–116.CrossRefPubMed Blanchard DC, Griebel G, Blanchard RJ. Mouse defensive behaviors: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panic. Eur J Pharmacol. 2003;463:97–116.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Peça J, Feliciano C, Ting JT, Wang W, Wells MF, Venkatraman TN, et al. Shank3 mutant mice display autistic-like behaviours and striatal dysfunction. Nature. 2011;472:437–42.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Peça J, Feliciano C, Ting JT, Wang W, Wells MF, Venkatraman TN, et al. Shank3 mutant mice display autistic-like behaviours and striatal dysfunction. Nature. 2011;472:437–42.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Ey E, Leblond CS, Bourgeron T. Behavioral profiles of mouse models for autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 2011;4:5–16.CrossRefPubMed Ey E, Leblond CS, Bourgeron T. Behavioral profiles of mouse models for autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 2011;4:5–16.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Parker JDA, Majeski SA, Collin VT. ADHD symptoms and personality: relationships with the five-factor model. Pers Individ Dif. 2004;36:977–87.CrossRef Parker JDA, Majeski SA, Collin VT. ADHD symptoms and personality: relationships with the five-factor model. Pers Individ Dif. 2004;36:977–87.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Dalgleish T, Moradi AR, Taghavi MR, Neshat-Doost HT, Yule W. An experimental investigation of hypervigilance for threat in children and adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med. 2001;31:541–7.CrossRefPubMed Dalgleish T, Moradi AR, Taghavi MR, Neshat-Doost HT, Yule W. An experimental investigation of hypervigilance for threat in children and adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med. 2001;31:541–7.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Freedman R, Waldo M, Bickford-Wimer P, Nagamoto H. Elementary neuronal dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1991;4:233–43.CrossRefPubMed Freedman R, Waldo M, Bickford-Wimer P, Nagamoto H. Elementary neuronal dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1991;4:233–43.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature. 2014;511:421–7.CrossRefPubMedCentral Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature. 2014;511:421–7.CrossRefPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Limited impact of Cntn4 mutation on autism-related traits in developing and adult C57BL/6J mice
verfasst von
Remco T. Molenhuis
Hilgo Bruining
Esther Remmelink
Leonie de Visser
Maarten Loos
J. Peter H. Burbach
Martien J. H. Kas
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2016
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders / Ausgabe 1/2016
Print ISSN: 1866-1947
Elektronische ISSN: 1866-1955
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-016-9140-2

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2016

Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 1/2016 Zur Ausgabe