Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Journal of Nutrition 3/2016

Open Access 11.01.2016 | Review

Increasing vegetable intakes: rationale and systematic review of published interventions

verfasst von: Katherine M. Appleton, Ann Hemingway, Laure Saulais, Caterina Dinnella, Erminio Monteleone, Laurence Depezay, David Morizet, F. J. Armando Perez-Cueto, Ann Bevan, Heather Hartwell

Erschienen in: European Journal of Nutrition | Ausgabe 3/2016

Abstract

Purpose

While the health benefits of a high fruit and vegetable consumption are well known and considerable work has attempted to improve intakes, increasing evidence also recognises a distinction between fruit and vegetables, both in their impacts on health and in consumption patterns. Increasing work suggests health benefits from a high consumption specifically of vegetables, yet intakes remain low, and barriers to increasing intakes are prevalent making intervention difficult. A systematic review was undertaken to identify from the published literature all studies reporting an intervention to increase intakes of vegetables as a distinct food group.

Methods

Databases—PubMed, PsychInfo and Medline—were searched over all years of records until April 2015 using pre-specified terms.

Results

Our searches identified 77 studies, detailing 140 interventions, of which 133 (81 %) interventions were conducted in children. Interventions aimed to use or change hedonic factors, such as taste, liking and familiarity (n = 72), use or change environmental factors (n = 39), use or change cognitive factors (n = 19), or a combination of strategies (n = 10). Increased vegetable acceptance, selection and/or consumption were reported to some degree in 116 (83 %) interventions, but the majority of effects seem small and inconsistent.

Conclusions

Greater percent success is currently found from environmental, educational and multi-component interventions, but publication bias is likely, and long-term effects and cost-effectiveness are rarely considered. A focus on long-term benefits and sustained behaviour change is required. Certain population groups are also noticeably absent from the current list of tried interventions.
Abkürzungen
C
Comparison
I
Intervention
V
Vegetable
vs
Versus
w
Weeks
m
Months

Introduction

Health benefits of high fruit and vegetable intakes

The health benefits of a high consumption of fruits and vegetables are well known [1]. Associations with all-cause mortality [2, 3] and mortality from cardiovascular disease [2, 3], including coronary heart disease [4] and stroke [5, 6], are well evidenced. Associations also suggest a reduced risk of hypertension [7], osteoporosis [8], body weight and adiposity [9, 10], dementia and cognitive decline [11, 12], and some cancers [1315], although the evidence for cancers is less consistent [1, 3]. Intervention studies increasing the consumption of fruits and vegetables also demonstrate improved microvascular function [16], improved microvascular function and inflammatory status [17], improved profiles in inflammatory and oxidative stress [18], improved immune response [19], and improved weight maintenance [20].

Consideration of fruits and vegetables as different food groups

However, while fruits and vegetables share health benefits as a result of the provision and interaction of a number of bioactive compounds, including vitamins, minerals, antioxidants, carotenoids and flavenoids [21, 22], the specific bioactive compounds in fruits and in vegetables can vary greatly [2125]. Their contribution to other dietary features also vary. Fruits typically contain greater dietary sugars, with potential negative impacts on both health and on public willingness to consume them [22, 26]. Vegetables, by comparison, can contain more protein and fibre [22] and are more often processed prior to consumption. This processing can both increase and decrease micronutrient bioavailability and activity, again impacting on health benefits [22, 25, 2731]. Many studies that separate fruits and vegetables find different effects of the different food classes on health outcomes [2, 12, 3235]. These differences between fruits and vegetables argue for the consideration of fruits and vegetables, in terms of health, as separate and different food types.
While differing in their potential health benefits, fruits and vegetables also taste very different, are generally of a different texture and are typically consumed in different manners [3639]. Fruit is generally sweet, is typically softer in texture, is more often consumed raw, is more frequently consumed and is generally considered more acceptable as a snack, as a drink or as dessert [3639]. Vegetables, by comparison, can taste bitter, are generally harder in texture, are more often cooked, are more typically consumed and considered more acceptable as part of a meal [3639], and thus are also more often consumed with other foods as opposed to alone [40]. These different consumption patterns suggest that fruit and vegetable consumption may be differentially determined. Glasson et al. [41] directly compared the determinants of fruit consumption and vegetable consumption in an Australian population, to find fruit consumption to be largely prevented by cost, food preferences, quality, availability and wastage concerns, while vegetable consumption was more frequently prevented by food preferences, lack of time, cost and taste. Differential determinants again argue for the consideration of fruits and vegetables, as separate and different food types, and suggest the potential need for different intervention strategies for increasing fruit and for increasing vegetable consumption.
Furthermore, while population levels of both fruit and vegetable consumption remain below World Health Organization recommendations across the world [42, 43], interventions to increase fruit and vegetable intakes more often target fruit, and typically report greater success for fruit consumption compared to that for vegetables, for both children and adults [4446]. These findings suggest not only a need for different intervention strategies for increasing fruit and increasing vegetable consumption, but a real need for strategies that achieve successful increases in vegetable-specific consumption. This paper focuses specifically on vegetable consumption.

Vegetable-specific consumption

Health benefits of high vegetable-specific intakes

Various studies demonstrate health benefits from a high consumption specifically of vegetables (i.e. from vegetables alone, as opposed to in combination with fruits, as occurs when considering fruits and vegetables together). Observational studies demonstrate reduced risk of cardiovascular disease [2], type II diabetes [34], non-gallstone-related acute pancreatitis [33], various cancers [2, 4750] and cognitive decline [32]. Meta-analyses of observational studies demonstrate associations between a higher vegetable consumption and reduced risk of stroke [6], dementia and cognitive decline [12], and from various cancers [14, 15, 35, 51, 52], although again the evidence for cancers is inconsistent. Meta-analyses of prospective studies find no benefits for breast cancer risk [53], gastric cancer risk [54], pancreatic cancer risk [55] and bladder cancer risk [56]. Meta-analyses also report no benefits of overall vegetable consumption for type II diabetes [5759], although dose–response meta-analyses also suggest benefit up to 2–3 servings/day and a threshold beyond this where type II diabetes risk does not reduce further [60].
Specific vegetable groups or types of vegetables have also been associated with improved health outcomes. Intakes of dark green leafy vegetables have been associated with reduced risk for type II diabetes [5759], reduced risk for a number of cancers [48, 49, 61] and with reduced depression [62]. High intakes of cruciferous vegetables have been associated with reduced risk from various cancers [6370]. Intakes of beta-carotene-rich vegetables, yellow- and red-pigmented vegetables, and fruiting vegetables have also been associated with reduced risk from various cancers [4850, 61], and root vegetable consumption has been associated with reduced type II diabetes risk [60].
Much of this evidence, however, stems from limited studies, and the body of evidence is far from conclusive [64, 66]. Prospective and cross-sectional studies are easily criticised for potential confounding, case control comparisons may suffer from bias towards differences between groups due to comparisons between cases and health conscious (and consequently) healthy controls [71], and study designs do not allow determination of causality. Considerable further work is required before conclusions can be drawn. The majority of studies investigating effects of vegetable consumption, furthermore, do not investigate vegetable consumption independent of fruit consumption or other aspects of the diet. While fruits and vegetables are frequently consumed together, associations may reflect not just associations with vegetables, but associations with produce consumption in general, or with a healthier diet/lifestyle [1, 72]. A recent systematic review by Fulton et al. [72] reports impacts for fruit and vegetable interventions not only from micronutrient intakes but also from changes to the whole dietary profile. Lifestyle factors are frequently included in studies as confounders, but it is often difficult to control for all potential confounders, and adjustment for other dietary aspects, particularly fruit consumption, is less common. Associations will also depend on the definition of vegetables used, and the inclusion or not of certain vegetables in certain categories. Potatoes, for example, are sometimes included among vegetables, sometimes included as ‘white’ vegetables, and sometimes not considered at all [21, 22].
Thus, for improved health, increasing intakes of vegetables are required. For intakes of vegetables to be increased, strategies and interventions are needed. These interventions should be based on in-depth understanding of the underlying determinants of low vegetable consumption.

Determinants of vegetable-specific consumption

Various research has been undertaken to understand the associations with, and reasons for, vegetable consumption, independent of fruit consumption. In young children, the bitter and undesirable taste of vegetables is often provided as a major barrier to vegetable consumption [7375], and food neophobia (the reluctance to eat, or the avoidance of, novel foods [76]) particularly, can interfere with young children’s acceptance of vegetables [7678]. This neophobia typically results in the rejection of bitter tasting foods and foods that do not ‘’look right’’ [76], of which vegetables are good examples.
As children age, taste, appearance and liking continue to be important [79], but low vegetable consumption is frequently also associated with various characteristics of the family environment. These factors include low parental education and socio-economic status [8082], low vegetable consumption by parents and caregivers [8385], low availability and negative perceptions of vegetables in the home [86] and a family environment that is unsupportive of vegetable consumption [83, 85, 87]. Vegetable consumption is higher, for example, in families where vegetables are disguised or sauces are used to mask undesirable tastes and appearances [79, 83, 85], where vegetables are more often incorporated into composite foods as opposed to consumed alone to dilute negative tastes and appearances [79], where meals are home cooked to accommodate individual preferences [83], and where games are played to encourage vegetable consumption [85]. The expression of neophobic behaviour towards vegetables also appears to be mitigated by high parental education and high socio-economic status [77, 88] and again by a positive and supportive environment [89, 90].
Taste, appearance, liking and the surrounding environment continue to be important as children become adolescents, but individual cognitions also gain increasing importance. Low vegetable consumption in adolescents has again been associated with low parental education and socio-economic status [91], low vegetable consumption by the parents [92], low availability and a family environment that is unsupportive of vegetable consumption [91, 93]. Vegetable consumption in adolescents, however, has also been associated with an awareness of the importance of vegetables for health, and a willingness and ability to ask for vegetables from parents [93].
In adults, higher vegetable consumption has been associated with higher liking for the taste of vegetables [41, 94, 95], higher appreciation of health and the value of a healthy diet [94], greater nutritional and culinary knowledge [96, 97], and with several related food habits and eating practices [98, 99], including usual consumption of meals as opposed to snacks [94, 100], increased time and willingness to prepare and cook home-cooked meals [41, 94, 95, 97], and a low consumption of fast food [94]. The transfer of childhood eating habits and food preferences into adulthood is well known, and adult vegetable intake is often related to childhood experiences [94]. Neophobic tendencies also typically last well into adulthood, and typically correlate negatively with liking for and frequency of vegetable consumption in adulthood [101103]. The individual preferences of one family member can also have impacts on the rest of the family, with most family units opting to cook only one meal of acceptability to all family members [104, 105]. Given the importance of adult consumption for children, many of the determinants of adult consumption will also impact on child consumption.
Alongside individual preferences, higher vegetable consumption in adults is also related to increased availability [98, 106, 107] and reduced cost [41, 95, 97, 108], and low consumption is largely associated with lower socio-economic status [109, 110], lower income [44, 107], living in a more deprived area or lower income neighbourhood (an indirect measure of socio-economic status) [44] and lower education [109, 111].
Research thus, suggests a variety of reasons behind low vegetable consumption, ranging from taste and pleasure, to individual cognitions and health beliefs, and to aspects beyond the individual including society and the environment. Many of these reasons have been targeted by interventions.

Strategies to increase vegetable-specific consumption: systematic review

Various reports of strategies to increase vegetable-specific consumption are available. A comprehensive collection of these interventions, and an evaluation of success, however, is currently lacking. The aim of this work was to systematically review the published literature to identify all published interventions aiming to increase vegetable-specific consumption.

Method

The objective of the review was to identify from the published literature all studies reporting an intervention to increase vegetable intakes, where vegetables were considered as a separate and distinct food group, and the intervention focused specifically on increasing intakes of this food group. Three databases: PubMed, PsychInfo, and Medline, were searched over all years of records for all studies with the terms ‘vegetable’ and ‘vegetables’ in the ‘title’. These search criteria were used to limit the search results to studies with a focus on vegetables. No other search criteria and no limits were used. All titles were screened for relevance and then all abstracts. Two review authors independently conducted all searches, screened all titles and screened all abstracts (KMA, AH or HH). Studies were included in the review if they involved an intervention designed primarily to increase vegetable intakes as a specific and distinct food group, and if they intended to change behaviour—vegetable selection, purchasing or vegetable consumption. Studies were not included if they did not include an intervention, if the intervention targeted fruit and vegetable intakes [112], if the intervention targeted vegetables and other foods, e.g. vegetables and wholegrains [113, 114]; if the intervention involved changing consumption as opposed to increasing consumption [115], or if they did not include a measure of behaviour, but instead only measured correlates of behaviour such as intentions, attitudes, and knowledge [116, 117]. Studies measuring tasting were included where tasting was voluntary, where amount tasted was voluntary and where tasting/amount was measured, but studies where tasting was compulsory and/or prespecified, e.g. to make hedonic judgements, were not included [118, 119]. Studies were included regardless of the use or not of a comparison for an intervention, or the type of comparison used. A study using a vegetable-specific intervention that is compared with a fruit-specific intervention, for example, is included [120] (all other criteria were also met). Relevant articles were also searched for other suitable studies. Searches of conference abstracts, book chapters, etc., were not undertaken, thus studies are only included if reported in full articles. Details from all studies were subsequently tabulated by one review author (KMA) and checked by an additional review author (AH or HH). All tables are provided in the “Results” section. No other data were extracted. Due to the early nature of the research area, the limited number of studies available per intervention type, and high heterogeneity between study methodologies, risk of bias was not assessed, and no attempt was made to combine studies, e.g. through meta-analysis. The review was undertaken using PRISMA guidelines and a PRISMA diagram illustrating the outcomes of the review process is given in Fig. 1.

Results

Searches were most recently conducted on 28 April 2015. The results of the searches are given in the PRISMA diagram in Fig. 1. A total of 77 studies were identified, reporting the impacts of 140 interventions. Details of all studies are presented by intervention type in Tables 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 and 6. Interventions have been broadly classified as those focussing on hedonic determinants of vegetable intake, such as taste, familiarity and liking—Tables 1, 2 and 3, those focussing on environmental determinants of vegetable intake—Table 4, those focussing on cognitive determinants—Table 5, and those using a combination of approaches—Table 6. Studies reporting two or more different interventions are included separately in separate tables, where appropriate. Within each table, studies are ordered by age of target audience. Of the interventions identified, 113 (81 %) interventions focus on improving intakes in children. Early intervention will maximize health benefits [45], and eating habits in childhood are likely to extend into adulthood [75, 94].

Interventions aiming to change or use hedonic factors

Eleven interventions focus on changing or using the taste or familiarity of a vegetable/vegetable product on a single occasion (Table 1). Six of these interventions suggest that the addition of a liked taste or flavour in the form of salt [121, 123] and in the form of a flavoured dip [122] or condiments [125] can increase vegetable consumption on a single occasion. Guidelines regarding salt intake and the possible impact of increasing preferences for salty flavours must also be considered, but these studies demonstrate a potentially useful strategy. The addition of fat to a vegetable product did not result in increased intakes [121], but increased intakes were found following the use of familiar as opposed to novel vegetables products [124].
Table 1
Published interventions utilising taste or familiarity on a single occasion to increase vegetable intake
Reference/intervention
Aim
Intervention
Comparison
Results
Conclusions
Bouhlal et al. [121]
Taste
To increase V intakes in children aged 18–37 months (n = 74)
1. Reduction in salt provision (0 %)
2. Increase in salt provision (1.2 %)
Usual salt provision (0.6 %)
Less V consumed in I1 vs C. No effects for I2
Salt addition should be limited, but its suppression in V, whose intake is to be promoted, should be considered cautiously
Bouhlal et al. [121]
Taste
To increase V intakes in children aged 18–37 months (n = 74)
1. Reduction in fat provision (0 %)
2. Increase in fat provision (5 %)
Usual fat provision (2.5 %)
No differences between conditions
Fat addition could be avoided in foods for children without having an impact on palatability
Savage et al. [122] Study 1
Taste
To increase willingness to taste, liking and consumption of V in children aged 3–5 years (n = 34)
Single exposure to 3 target V paired with:
1. Plain reduced-fat dip
2. Favoured flavoured reduced-fat dip
Single exposure to 3 target V paired with no dip
Increased willingness to taste in I vs C. No differences between I1 and I2
Offering V with reduced-fat dips containing familiar flavours can increase tasting and thereby promote liking, acceptance and consumption of V, including V previously rejected or disliked
Savage et al. [122] Study 2
Taste
To increase willingness to taste, liking and consumption of V in children aged 3–5 years (n = 26/27)
Single exposure to 2 target unfamiliar or disliked V (celery, squash) with a favoured flavoured reduced-fat dip
Single exposure to 2 target V with no dip
Increased intakes in I vs C
Offering V with reduced-fat dips containing familiar flavours can increase tasting and thereby promote liking, acceptance and consumption of V, including V previously rejected or disliked
Bouhlal et al. [123]
Taste
To increase V intakes in children aged 8–11 years (n = 75)
1. Reduction in salt provision (0 %)
2. Increase in salt provision (1.2 %)
Usual salt provision (0.6 %)
Less V consumed in I1 vs C. No effects for I2
Salt content has a positive and food-specific effect on intake
Morizet et al. [124]
Familiarity
To increase V dish selection in 8–11 year olds. School-based intervention
Novel V dishes with no label (carrots n = 60, broccoli n = 65)
Familiar V dishes with no label
Increased selection for C vs I
Adding a label with the V name can increase children’s willingness to select a novel V dish instead of a familiar one. Familiar V are otherwise more likely to be consumed
Ahearn [125]
Taste
To increase V consumption in a 14 year old boy with autism. Case study
Simultaneous presentation of 3 V with liked condiments
No control
I significantly increased intakes
Adding condiments increased food acceptance across three food items
Interventions ordered by age of target audience
C comparison, I intervention, V vegetable, vs versus
Fifty-two interventions focus on increasing liking and familiarity with repeated experience. These interventions use learning techniques, including repeated exposure (n = 23), ensuring that exposure is positive via pairing with liked flavours (n = 14), pairing with beneficial nutrients (n = 6), pairing with external reinforcement (n = 7), the use of positive models (n = 1) and the use of reinforcement plus models (n = 1) (Table 2). Many of these interventions demonstrate success by improving liking and/or consumption: 16 of 23 (70 %) using repeated exposure; 12 of 14 (86 %) using pairing with liked flavours; four of six (67 %) using pairing with nutrients; all seven (100 %) using pairing with reinforcement; and the one (100 %) using reinforcement plus modelling. Effects, however, are far from robust or consistent (i.e. effects are often found in one measure, but not in others), are often small, and tend to be limited to the specific vegetable used during exposure. Conditions within studies, furthermore, are often confounded, making mechanisms difficult to elucidate. In many studies that purport to investigate exposure, the exposure is in combination with other food components [129], modelling [75] or rewards in the form of praise or other positive interactions [139], thus effects may in fact occur partly due to conditioning. In many studies that purport to measure conditioning, exposure is not controlled for [136, 151]. Many of these interventions furthermore also involve children’s parents, and so may have benefits not just by allowing tasting and experience for the child, but also by improving parental perceptions of vegetables, improving attitudes towards vegetables in the home, and improving parental education and knowledge [75]. Interestingly, some of the interventions included in Table 2 report parental opinions of the intervention [75, 129], but as far as we can tell, none specifically tested parental knowledge or education as a result of the intervention for their children.
Table 2
Published interventions utilising learning (exposure, associative conditioning, instrumental conditioning or modelling) to increase vegetable intake
Reference/intervention
Aim
Intervention
Comparison
Results
Conclusions
Remy et al. [126]
Exposure
Conditioning
To increase V acceptance at complementary feeding (children aged 4–8 months)
1. RE—10 exposures to target V puree (n = 32)
2. FFL—10 exposures to target V puree paired with sweetener (n = 32)
3. FNL—10 exposures to target V puree paired with energy (n = 31)
Control V—limited experimental exposures
No differences in intake in I vs C postintervention. Intake of target V puree increased postintervention and at 3-m follow-up in I1 and I2, but not I3 vs pretest. Liking increased only in I1. No effects at 6-m follow-up
RE is as effective as and simpler to implement than FFL and more effective than FNL for increasing V acceptance at complementary feeding
Mennella et al. [127]
Study 2
Exposure
To increase V acceptance in infants aged 4–9 months
1. 8-day exposure to single V (n = 11)
2. 8-day exposure to several V in different meals (n = 12)
3. 8-day exposure to several V in the same meals (n = 12)
No control (pre-post comparison only)
Increased intakes of green beans, carrots and spinach in I3 compared to before. Trend towards increased intakes of green beans in I1 and I2 after intervention vs before
Repeated opportunities to taste a particular or a variety of foods may promote willingness to eat V
Mennella et al. [128]
Exposure
To increase V acceptance in infants aged 6–11 months
Milk formula flavoured with hydrolysate (similar taste to target V) (n = 24)
Milk formula (no hydrolysate taste) (n = 50)
Less target V (and relative to other V) was consumed by I vs C
Taste preferences are initially specific to the context they are learnt (in this case milk)
Hetherington et al. [129]
Exposure
To increase liking and acceptance of target V and unfamiliar V during weaning (children aged 6–12 months)
12 daily exposures to target V puree added to milk, then 12 × twice daily exposures to target V puree added to baby rice (n = 18)
Plain milk for 12 days, then plain rice for 12 days (n = 18)
Intake, liking and eating pace were greater for target V but not unfamiliar V for I vs C, at end of intervention
No differences at 6 or 18-m follow-up
Early exposure to vegetables in a step-by-step method could be included in complimentary feeding guidelines to enhance V intakes
Maier et al. [130]
Exposure
To increase V acceptance in children aged 7 months (n = 49)
1. 8 daily exposures to disliked V
2. 8 daily exposures to liked V
No control (pre-post comparison only)
Intakes increased in I1 and I2, greater increases for I1. Effects sustained for 9 months (self-report)
When a V is initially disliked it is worth persisting in feeding (exposure) for at least 8 subsequent meals
Caton et al. [131]
Exposure
Conditioning
To increase V acceptance in children aged 9–38 months
1. RE—10 exposures to target V puree (n = 22)
2. FFL—10 exposures to target V puree paired with sweetener (n = 25)
3. FNL—10 exposures to target V puree paired with energy (n = 25)
Control V—limited experimental exposures
Greater intake in I vs C, postintervention and 5-w follow-up. No differences between interventions postintervention. Higher intakes in I1 vs I2 at 5-w follow-up
RE, FFL and FNL were effective for increasing V acceptance, and equally so
Barends et al. [120]
Exposure
To increase V intakes in children aged 12–23 months
Started weaning with target V (green beans/artichoke), exclusive V for 18 days, 9 exposures to target V (n = 51)
Starting weaning with fruit, exclusive fruit for 18 days, 9 exposures to target fruit (n = 50)
Greater liking and intake of green beans, and greater intake of novel V, postintervention in I vs C, but no effects for artichoke
Weaning with V but not with fruits, may promote V acceptance in children
Barends et al. [132]
Follow-up of Barends et al. [120]
As above
As above
Greater reported liking and daily intake of V at 12 months in I vs C, but no differences at 23 months. No differences in measured intake at either time point
Weaning exclusively with V results in a higher daily V consumption until at least 12 months of age
Ahern et al. [133]
Exposure
Conditioning
To increase V acceptance in children aged 12–60 months (n = 29)
1. RE—8 exposures to target V puree
2. FFL—8 exposures to target V puree paired with apple puree for sweetness
Control V—0 exposures
Significant increases in V intake from pre- to postintervention. No differences between conditions
No effects of exposure
The addition of a familiar flavour (FFL learning) confers no advantage above exposure
Hausner et al. [134]
Exposure
Conditioning
To increase V acceptance in children aged 22–38 months
1. RE—10 exposures to target V puree (n = 32)
2. FFL—10 exposures to target V puree paired with sweetener (n = 33)
3. FNL—10 exposures to target V puree paired with energy (n = 39)
Control V—limited experimental exposures
Greater intake in I1 and I2 vs pretest, at postintervention, 3-m and 6-m follow-up. No effects in I3. No comparison with C
RE and FFL were effective for increasing V acceptance, and equally so. FNL was not effective
Bouhlal et al. [135]
Exposure
Conditioning
To increase V acceptance in children aged 2–3 years
1. RE—8 exposures to target V puree (n = 47)
2. FFL—8 exposures to target V puree paired with salt (n = 54)
3. FFL—8 exposures to target V puree paired with spice (n = 50)
Control V—no exposures
Greater intake in all I vs pretest, at postintervention, 1-, 3- and 6-m follow-up. Greater effects in I1 vs I2 and I3. Increases in liking also in I1 and I2. No change in C, but no statistical comparison with I provided
RE appears to be the simplest choice to increase V intake in the short and long term in toddlers
De Wild et al. [136]
Conditioning
To increase preferences and intake for target V in children aged 2–4 years (n = 28)
Seven twice weekly consumptions of target V soup (endive/spinach) paired with high energy
Seven twice weekly consumptions of target V soup (spinach/endive) paired with low energy
Increased preferences for I vs C immediately after intervention, no effects on intake. No effects at 2 and 6-month follow-up. Increase in intake for all V from pre- to postintervention
Results show an effect of exposure on intake, but not conditioning. Effects of conditioning are found in preferences
Holley et al. [137]
Exposure
Conditioning
Modelling
To increase acceptance of a disliked V in children aged 2–4 years
1. Exposure—daily exposure to target V for 14 days (n = 29)
2. Exposure + modelling—parent also consumed target V and gave positive comment for 14 days (n = 27)
3. Exposure + rewards—child given praise and non-food reward for tasting target V for 14 days (n = 29)
4. Exposure, modelling + rewards—all above strategies for 14 days (n = 27)
No exposure or other intervention
No differences between I and C when all groups analysed together. Significant increases in V intake and liking in I3 and I4 vs C in secondary analyses
Parent-led interventions based around modelling and offering incentives may present cost-efficient ways to increase children’s V consumption
Wardle et al. [75]
Exposure
To increase liking and intake for a previously disliked V in children aged 2–6 years. Home-based intervention
Exposure—child given a daily taste of V for 14 days (n = 50)
No intervention (wait list) (n = 45)
Greater increases in liking, ranking and consumption of V from pre- to postintervention in I vs C
A parent-led, exposure-based intervention involving daily tasting of a V holds promise for improving children’s acceptance of and increasing liking for a previously disliked V
Fildes et al. [138]
Exposure
Rewards
To increase V acceptance in children aged 3 years. Mailed intervention
Mailed instructions to offer children 14 daily tastes of a disliked V and sticker reward (n = 196)
No intervention (usual practice) (n = 246)
Intake and liking of V increased in I vs C. Acceptability of the protocol was also very high among I parents
Mailed instructions for taste exposure were effective in increasing children’s acceptance of an initially disliked vegetable
Anzman-Frasca et al. [139] Study 1
Exposure
Conditioning
To increase V liking and intakes in children aged 3–6 years (n = 41)
1. RE—twice weekly exposures to initially not-liked V for four weeks
2. AC—twice weekly exposures to initially not-liked V with a liked dip for four weeks
No exposure
Liking increased in I1 and I2, vs C, but no differences between I1 and I2
Administering few small tastes of V that are initially not liked, both with and without dip, can have a lasting impact on liking and intake of those V
Anzman-Frasca et al. [139] Study 2
Conditioning
To increase V liking and intakes in children aged 3–6 years (n = 43)
AC—twice weekly exposures to initially not-liked V with a liked dip for four weeks
RE—twice weekly exposures to initially not-liked V for four weeks
Liking increased in I and C, but no differences between them
Administering few small tastes of V that are initially not liked, both with and without dip, can have a lasting impact on liking and intake of those V
O’Connell et al. [140]
Exposure
To increase V intakes in children aged 3–6 years old
10 exposures of 3 different V at lunch over 30 days (30 exposures) (n = 50)
No exposure (n = 50)
No differences between conditions
Research should explore the conditions necessary for exposure to increase V intakes in preschool settings
Correia et al. [141]
Conditioning
To increase V intakes and willingness to try in pre-school children (3–5 years)
Lunch
Target V paired with familiar well-liked food (n = 43)
Lunch
Target V not paired with familiar well-liked food
No differences in intakes. Willingness to try increased marginally in I vs C
Further research should explore the strategy of pairing vegetables with liked foods
Fisher et al. [142]
Conditioning
To increase liking and intakes in bitter-sensitive and insensitive preschoolers
13 exposures to moderately liked V over 7 weeks with:
1. regular salad dressing as dip (n = 39)
2. light salad dressing as dip (n = 36)
3. regular dressing as sauce (n = 38)
13 exposures to moderately liked V over 7 weeks with: no dressing (n = 39)
No effects on intake in insensitive children. Higher liking and intakes in bitter-sensitive children in all I vs C. Effects vary based on whether regular or light dressing was provided as a dip or sauce
Offering dips can promote vegetable intake among some children who are sensitive to bitter tastes
Havermans and Jansen [143]
Conditioning
To increase liking and preference for a target V taste in children aged 4–6 years (n = 13)
6 conditioning trials—V juice paired with sweet taste
6 trials—different V juice not paired with sweet taste
Increase in liking and preference for I vs C
Flavour-flavour learning may be beneficial in increasing children’s liking and acceptance of vegetables
Hendy et al. [144]
Rewards
To increase (fruit and) V intakes in 1st, 2nd and 4th graders. School-based intervention
Rewards given for consumption of V at 12 meals (n = 96)
Rewards given for consumption of fruit at 12 meals (n = 96)
Increased intakes of V following I vs C. Increased preferences for V (marginal) after intervention vs before
The use of rewards as in the Kids Choice programme shows promise as a simple and effective method to increase children’s (fruit and) V acceptance
Cooke et al. [145]
Exposure
Rewards
To increase V likings and intakes in children aged 4–6 years
Twelve daily taste exposures to target (disliked) V with
1. tangible reward (n = 99)
2. social reward (n = 106)
3. no reward (exposure only) (n = 105)
No exposure (n = 112)
Liking increased in I1, I2, and I3 vs C, postintervention and 1- and 3-m follow-up. No differences between interventions. Consumption increased in I1 and I2 vs C postintervention and 1- and 3-m follow-up. Consumption increased in I3 vs C postintervention and 1-m follow-up only
Rewarding children for tasting an initially disliked V produced sustained increases in acceptance, with no negative effects on liking
Corsini et al. [146]
Exposure
Rewards
To increase liking and consumption of a disliked V in children aged 4–6 years
1. EO—Daily exposure for 2 weeks (n = 62)
2. ER—Daily exposure, plus sticker reward, for 2 weeks (n = 60)
No exposure (n = 66)
Increased liking at postintervention in I1 and I2 vs C, and no further change over 4-w and 3-m follow-ups. Target V consumption increased postintervention in all groups, and continued to increase for I2 at 4w and 3 m, and for C at 3 m
The findings support the effectiveness of using a reward with a repeated exposure strategy. In particular, such rewards can facilitate the tastings necessary to change liking
Noradilah et al. [147]
Exposure
To increase acceptance of a target disliked V in children aged 5–6 years (n = 42)
Target V served at lunch on 3 consecutive days
No control
Increased intakes of V in I from day 1 to 3. Parent reported child liking of V also increased
Multiple exposures to V could be a strategy to increase consumption of V among children
Wardle et al. [148]
Exposure
Rewards
To increase V acceptance in children aged 5–7 years
1. Exposure—8 daily offers to taste and eat target disliked V (n = 15)
2. Reward—8 daily offers to taste, eat and gain reward sticker for target disliked V (n = 16)
No exposure or reward (n = 18)
Increased liking and consumption in I1 vs C. Intermediate effects in I2. Increased intakes in all groups after intervention vs before
Repeated exposure to the taste of unfamiliar V is a promising strategy for promoting liking of previously disliked V in children
Lakkakula et al. [149]
Exposure
To increase liking for target V in fourth/fifth grade children (n = 360). Part of a wider school-based intervention
Offered cold carrots, tomatoes and bell peppers, and hot peas to taste once a week for 10 weeks
No control
For children who began the programme disliking the target V, I improved liking scores for carrots, tomatoes and peas; liking for bell peppers did not change. For children who began the study liking the target V, no changes were found
Repeated tasting of less-liked vegetables by children in a cafeteria-based setting is a strategy to promote liking of these items, that is effective in approximately half of the participants
Johnston et al. [150]
Conditioning
To increase V consumption and V variety in 6th grade children
V paired with a preferred taste (peanut butter) weekly for 4 months (n = 40)
V exposure weekly for 4 months (n = 38)
Significant increases in vegetable consumption, and variety of vegetables eaten in I vs C
Pairing of vegetables with a preferred taste may be an effective technique for increasing consumption
Zeinstra et al. [151]
Conditioning
To increase V preferences and consumption in children aged 7–8 years (n = 19)
Seven daily exposures to V juice paired with high energy
Seven daily exposures to V juice paired with low energy
No differences between I and C, but consumption was very low
The pure taste of vegetables is not acceptable enough to allow adequate consumption for flavour-nutrient conditioning to occur
Olsen et al. [152]
Exposure
Conditioning
To increase V intakes in children aged 9–11 years
1. Neutral V paired with liked V for 6 exposures, followed by 6 exposures to 3 V (n = 72)
2. Neutral V alone for 6 exposures, followed by 6 exposures to 3 V (n = 74)
3. Neutral V paired with disliked V for 6 exposures followed by 6 exposures to 3 V (n = 73)
No exposure
Increases in neutral V intake between I1 vs I2 and I1 vs I3. No other differences. No differences between conditions in liking
Pairing with a liked V increased neutral V consumption. Serving V that are mixed in this manner has potential for increasing intakes
Interventions ordered by age of target audience
C comparison, I intervention, V vegetable, vs versus, w weeks, m months
While largely successful, particularly over considerable exposures, exposure, however, is a relatively time-consuming practice that results in small changes, and typically only for the vegetable to which children have been exposed. Nine interventions have extended the use of exposure to consider exposure to vegetables via picture books and stories (Table 3). These procedures appear beneficial, although few studies have currently tested these ideas, and effects again appear small or unreliable, and typically only apply to the vegetable to which the child has been exposed. Little evidence suggests that effects generalise to other vegetables, so neither taste or visual exposure appears to encourage consumption of a variety of vegetables. Repeated exposure to many vegetables may result in increased consumption of many vegetables, and some studies are beginning to demonstrate these effects [156, 157], but generalisation of exposure to non-exposed vegetables has not yet been demonstrated reliably either through the use of taste or visual stimuli. The potential for exposure to multiple as opposed to single vegetables at one time point, however, may be greater using visual as opposed to taste stimuli.
Table 3
Published interventions utilising exposure to picture books containing vegetables to increase vegetable intake
Reference/intervention
Aim
Intervention
Comparison
Results
Conclusions
Heath et al. [153]
Exposure via picture books
To increase familiarity and liking for V in 20- to 24-month-old children
Reading a picture book every day for 2 weeks including:
1. a liked V (n = 19)
2. a disliked V (n = 19)
3. an unfamiliar V (n = 19)
Test procedures conducted on target V and control (non-exposed) V
Increased intakes, and reduced encouragement to try unfamiliar V in all I, and particularly I3 vs C. No effects in willingness to taste
Results confirm the potential for picture books to play a positive role in encouraging healthy eating in young children
Bryne and Nitzke [154]
Exposure via picture books
To improve attitudes and behaviours towards an unfamiliar V in children aged 3–5 years
1. Childrens book with positive messages about V (kohlrabi)
2. Childrens book with negative messages about V (kohlrabi) (n = 118)
No book
More V tasters in I1 vs C during the second posttest
Children’s books with positive messages can increase willingness to taste
De Droog et al. [155]
Exposure via picture books
Social activities
To increase carrot consumption in children aged 4–6 years
5 days exposure to
1. Picture book containing carrots and rabbit, passive reading (n = 26)
2. Picture book containing carrots and rabbit, active reading (n = 26)
3. Picture book containing carrots and turtle, passive reading (n = 26)
4. Picture book containing carrots and turtle, active reading (n = 26)
No exposure to book (n = 56)
More carrots consumed in all I vs C. Greater consumption with active v passive reading
Young children seem to enjoy this interactive shared reading, triggering positive feelings that increase children’s liking and consumption of the food promoted in the book
Interventions ordered by age of target audience
C comparison, I intervention, V vegetable, vs versus

Interventions based on changing the environment

An alternative approach to encourage vegetable consumption focuses on changing the environment and increasing consumption through increasing the provision of vegetables, or improving the manner in which provision is implemented (Table 4). Thirty-nine interventions were found using these strategies. All of these, with the exception of three interventions (one increasing variety [158] and two improving presentation [141, 164]) resulted in increased selection and/or consumption of vegetables in children and adults, through the increased provision of vegetables (n = 20), through the increased provision of a variety of vegetables (n = 7), through improved presentation (n = 5), through changing the location of vegetables (n = 1), through changing the order in which vegetables and other foods are served (n = 1) and through changing the serving order, while also increasing availability (n = 2). Increased consumption as a result of increased provision is unsurprising, but concerns have been raised regarding increased energy intakes as a result, and increased potential for food wastage. An absence of effects on overall energy intakes is reported in some studies [161], and concerns are mitigated if vegetables are substituted for other foods in the meal as opposed to simply added [161]. Suggestions to reduce potential food wastage include the use of family style serving dishes for individual meals [159, 161] or allowing differential selection, but again the cost-effectiveness of interventions that can increase waste will be questioned. Strategies that improve the presentation of vegetables may offer a valuable alternative. These interventions typically change the salience or likely appeal of vegetables [141, 164, 166], and have again demonstrated success, but relatively few studies are currently available. Exact mechanisms are again unclear—attractive labels may rely partly on modelling, effects due to serving order may rely partly on hunger and exposure, but the relative ease and low cost of these interventions add to their value.
Table 4
Published interventions using increased availability and variety of provided vegetables, and improved presentation of vegetables to increase vegetable intakes
Reference/intervention
Aim
Intervention
Comparison
Results
Conclusions
Coulthard et al. [156]
Variety
To increase V intakes in children aged 4–6 months
Exposure to a variety of V over 9 days
Exposure to 1 V over 9 days
Those weaned later (5–6 m) in I consumed significantly more novel V vs C. No effects in those weaned earlier (4–5 m)
Infants who are weaned at 6 m may benefit from being weaned onto a variety of tastes
Maier et al. [157]
Variety
To increase novel V acceptance in children aged 7 months
Phase 1
1. 3 daily exposures to 3 V (n = 46)
2. 3 alternated daily exposures to 3 V (n = 51)
Phase 2
All groups given 5 alternate exposures to 2 V
Phase 1
9 daily exposures to 1 V (n = 51)
Phase 2
5 alternate exposures to 2 V
Intakes and liking of novel V after phase 1 and phase 2 increased in I1 and I2 vs C, greater increases for I2
High variety produced greatest new food intake
De Wild et al. [158]
Variety
To increase V intakes in children aged 2–5 years. Home-based intervention
Exposure over 12 meals to 2 target V simultaneously (n = 34)
Exposure over 12 meals to 1 target V (n = 36)
I positively associated with higher intake than C but not significantly so
Choice-offering has some, but not a robust effect on increasing V intake in children
Spill et al. [159]
Availability
To increase V consumption (and decrease energy intake) in children aged 3–5 years (n = 51)
Lunch starter provided of:
1. 30 g carrots
2. 60 g carrots
3. 90 g carrots
No lunch starter
Greater target V and total V consumption in all I vs C, and in I2 and I3 vs I1. No differences in total energy intake
Increasing the portion size of a V served as a first course can be an effective strategy for increasing V intakes in preschool children
Spill et al. [160]
Availability
To increase V intakes in children aged 3–5 years (n = 72)
Lunch starter provided of:
1. 150 g tomato soup
2. 225 g tomato soup
3. 300 g tomato soup
No soup served
Greater intakes of V in I1, I2 and I3 vs C. Increasing the soup portion size increased soup and V intake
Serving low-energy-dense V soup as a first course is an effective strategy to increase V consumption at the meal
Spill et al. [161]
Availability
To increase V (and reduce energy) intakes in children aged 3–5 years (n = 40)
1. Pureed V (triple content) added to foods across 1 day to reduce energy density by 85 %
2. Pureed V (quadruple content) added to foods across 1 day to reduce energy density by 75 %
Energy density—100 %
V intake increased in I1 and I2 vs C. No compensatory effects on V side dish consumption
The incorporation of substantial amounts of puréed V is an effective strategy to increase daily V intake and decrease energy intake in young children
Correia et al. [141]
Presentation
To increase V intakes and willingness to try in preschool children (3–5 years)
Snack:
Target V arranged in appealing manner (n = 42)
Snack:
Target V not arranged in appealing manner
No effects of I
Further research should explore the strategy of pairing vegetables with liked foods, no effects of appearance
Mathias et al. [162]
Availability
Intervention to increase V intakes in children aged 4–6 years
Serving of 150 g V at a single meal
Serving of 75 g V at a single meal
Increased V intake in I vs C. Effects limited to those who liked V
Serving larger V portions at meals can be used to promote young children’s intake of V without influencing fruit or total energy intake
Bucher et al. [163]
Variety
To increase V intakes in children aged 7–10 years
Selection from buffet (fake foods) of pasta, chicken and 2 V (carrots, beans) (n = 34)
Selection from buffet containing pasta, chicken and 1 V (carrots (n = 32) or beans (n = 34))
Children served themselves more energy from V in I vs C. No differences in total meal energy
Variety is effective in increasing the V choice of school-aged children. Serving an assortment of V in school cafeterias might be a simple and effective strategy to improve children’s nutrition
Morizet et al. [124]
Presentation
To increase vegetable dish selection in 8- to 11-year-olds. School-based intervention
Novel V dishes with:
2. basic label (carrots n = 44, broccoli n = 72)
3. model-related label (carrots n = 41, broccoli n = 90)
Novel V dishes with no label
Increased intakes in I2 and I3 vs C [I reverses reduced selection for no label (I1, see Table 1)], and no differences between I2 and I3
Adding a label with the V name can increase children’s willingness to select a new V dish instead of a familiar one
Wansink et al. [164]
Study 1
Presentation
To increase V selection in children aged 8–11 years old. School-based intervention
1. V served at lunch with attractive label (n = 32)
2. V served at lunch with generic label (n = 38)
V served at lunch with no label (n = 45)
Increased intakes of V in I1 vs I2 and C. No effects on selection
Attractive names effectively and persistently increased healthy food consumption in elementary schools
Wansink et al. [164]
Study 2
Presentation
To increase V selection in children aged 8–11 years old. School-based intervention
V served with attractive name every day for 1 month (n = 742)
V served with no name every day for 1 month (n = 810)
Increased selection of V in I vs C
Attractive names effectively and persistently increased healthy food consumption in elementary schools
Just and Wansink [165]
Availability
To increase V selection in a school canteen
Introduction of a salad bar
No salad bar
Increase in number of children consuming salad in I vs C
Simple changes can increase selection
Just and Wansink [165]
Variety
To increase V intakes in a school canteen
Giving students a choice of 1 of 2 V
No choice—requiring students to take 1 V
Increased intakes of V in I vs C
Providing choice within forced selection improved intakes
Just and Wansink [165]
Location
To increase V selection in a school canteen
Salad bar positioned, so that children must walk round it
Salad bar positioned, to the side
Immediate increase in sales of salad following I vs C. Continued to increase
Simple changes can increase selection
Redden et al. [166]
Study 1
Availability
Serving order
To increase V intakes in 5th grade children
Mildly liked V provided in isolation while waiting for lunch (n = 755). V also provided for lunch
V only provided for lunch (n = 680)
Increased total V consumption in I vs C
Intervention increased target V consumption
Redden et al. [166]
Study 2
Availability
Serving order
To increase V intakes in 5th grade children
Mildly liked V provided in isolation while waiting for lunch (n = 486–530). V and control V also provided for lunch
V and control V only provided for lunch (n = 529–558)
Increased mildly liked V intake and total intake in I vs C. No change in control V consumption. Sustained effects over 3 days
Intervention increased target V consumption, and did not decrease other V consumption
Reicks et al. [167]
Presentation
To increase V intakes in elementary school children (kindergarten—5th grade) (n = approx.. 800)
Photographs of target V (carrots, green beans) were placed in lunch trays at one lunch
No photographs, same lunch served
Increased selection of V in I vs C, so increased consumption. Students selecting beans consumed the same in I and C, but students selecting carrots also consumed more in I vs C
Placing photographs in cafeteria lunch trays incurs minimal costs, but was associated with an increase in V consumption within the range of those found in more expensive interventions
Bucher et al. [168]
Variety
To enhance V choices and improve meal composition in college students
Buffet meal (fake food) of pasta, chicken and 2 V (carrots, beans) (n = 34)
Buffet meal (fake food) of pasta, chicken and 1 V (carrots (n = 29) or beans (n = 35))
Participants in I chose more energy and more  % energy from V vs C. No differences in total energy selected
Serving an assortment of V might be a simple and effective strategy to increase V intakes and improve meal composition
Blatt et al. [169]
Availability
To increase V (and reduce energy) intakes in adults (n = 41)
1. Pureed V (triple content) added to foods across 1 day to reduce energy density by 85 %
2. Pureed V (4.5 times content) added to foods across 1 day to reduce energy density by 75 %
Energy density—100 %
V intake increased and energy density decreased in I1 and 2 vs C
Large amounts of puréed V can be incorporated into foods to increase V intakes and reduce energy intakes
Meengs et al. [170]
Variety
To promote V intakes in adults (n = 66)
1 meal involving 200 g each of 3 V
Three meals involving 600 g of 1 V (same three V)
Increased V intake at I vs C. Increased V intake at I vs most preferred C
Increasing the variety of V served at a meal can be used to increase V intake
Redden et al. [166]
Study 3
Serving order
To increase V intakes in adults
Mildly liked V provided in advance of other more liked foods (n = 36)
Other foods provided:
1. in advance of V (n = 43)
2. simultaneously (n = 39)
More V consumed in I vs C1 and C2
Eating V first in isolation may prove useful for increasing V consumption in a wide range of individuals
Rolls et al. [171]
Availability
To increase V intakes (and facilitate weight management) in adults
Addition Study (n = 49)
1. 270 g V served
2. 360 g V served
Other meal components unchanged
Substitution Study (n = 48)
1. 270 g V served
2. 360 g V served
Other meal components reduced proportionally
Addition Study
180 g V served
Other meal components served
Substitution Study
180 g V served
Other meal components served
Greater V served led to greater V consumed in both studies
Effects on meal energy density and energy intake also in the substitution study
Serving more V, either by adding more or substituting them for other foods, is an effective strategy to increase V intake at a meal
Shenoy et al. [172]
Availability
To increase V intakes (and improve CVD health) in healthy adults
Education on the DASH diet and:
1. 8 oz V juice daily (n = 30)
2. 16 oz V juice daily (n = 30)
Education on the DASH diet only (n = 30)
I1 and I2 increased V intakes (and improved micronutrient profiles) vs C (and decreased blood pressure in prehypertensive adults)
Including 1–2 cups of vegetable juice daily was an effective and acceptable way for healthy adults to consume more V
Shenoy et al. [173]
Availability
To increase V intakes (and improve CVD health) in adults with metabolic syndrome
Education on the DASH diet and:
1. 8 oz V juice daily (n = 27)
2. 16 oz V juice daily (n = 27)
Education on the DASH diet only (n = 27)
I1 and I2 increased V intakes vs C (and decreased blood pressure. No effects on CVD measures)
Including 1–2 cups of vegetable juice daily was an effective and acceptable way for healthy adults to consume more V
Interventions ordered by age of target audience
C comparison, I intervention, V vegetable, vs versus

Interventions based on changing or using cognitive factors

Nineteen interventions were found that used information, education or other cognitive techniques (Table 5). These interventions are largely aimed at older audiences (those where cognitive factors have a greater impact on vegetable consumption and non-consumption), and used a range of techniques from providing information and education on nutrition (n = 6) or nutrition-related skills (n = 2), providing education plus a demonstration (n = 1) or gardening experience (n = 2), providing tailored information (n = 2), providing information on social norms (n = 1), invoking choice (n = 4) and invoking a memory (n = 1). With the exception of one intervention that aimed to educate [75], and two interventions that utilised choice [174], all of the studies using these types of strategy reported success to some degree, but multiple measures of impact were often taken, and success is not necessarily reported for all measures. The cost-effectiveness of these types of interventions is, however, also often questioned. Educational interventions can be costly, particularly those involving classes or courses to be delivered by a professional, but the long-term benefit of these interventions can also be difficult to assess. Knowledge accumulates over time and experience, and it can be difficult for individuals to pinpoint the exact source/sources of beneficial education.
Table 5
Published interventions using information provision, education and other cognitive strategies to increase vegetable intakes
Reference/intervention
Aim
Intervention
Comparison
Results
Conclusions
Wardle et al. [75]
Education
To increase liking and intake for a previously disliked V in children aged 2–6 years. Home-based intervention
Information—nutritional advice and leaflet (n = 48)
No intervention (wait-list) (n = 45)
No differences between I and C
A parent-led, exposure-based intervention involving daily tasting of a V holds promise for improving children’s acceptance of to increasing liking for a previously disliked V. No effects for information only
Zeinstra et al. [174]
Choice
Intervention to increase V intakes in children aged 4–6 years. Single restaurant meal intervention
1. Pre-meal choice—Single choice at the start of the meal of 1 of 2 V (n = 110)
2. At-meal choice—Repeated choices throughout the meal of 2 V (n = 97)
No choice—provision of 1 of 2 V (n = 96)
No differences in V liking or intake between conditions. Some effects of individual differences
Having a pre-meal choice was appreciated by the children but did not affect intake, liking, or motivation to eat vegetables
Dominguez et al. [175]
Choice
Intervention to increase V intakes in children aged 4–6 years
1. Single choice at the start of the meal of choice of 2 V (n = 50)
2. Repeated choices throughout the meal of choice of 2 V (n = 56)
No choice—provision of 2 V (n = 44)
Total V intakes were higher in I1 and I2 vs C
Results demonstrate the enhancing effect of providing choice to increase V intakes in young children
Gholami et al. [176]
Skills-based education
To increase V provision and consumption in 6–11 year old children
Theory based instructional leaflets to promote self-regulatory skills for providing healthy nutrition for children
No intervention (usual practice)
Increased V intake in I vs C, 2 weeks postintervention. No difference 3 months postintervention
Engaging mothers in self-regulatory health promotion programmes may facilitate more vegetable intake among their daughters
Morris and Zidenberg-Cherr [177]
Education
Exposure
Experience
To increase V intakes in 9–10 year olds (4th grade). School-based intervention
1.NL—nutrition education (n = 71 in school 1)
2. NG—nutrition education plus gardening activities (6 V planted) (n = 81 in school 2)
No formal nutrition or gardening education (n = 61 in school 3)
Knowledge and preferences for 2 V, increased, in I1 and I2 vs C, postintervention, and remained for 1 V per I at 6 months. Preferences for 1 V and 1 additional (non-planted) V increased in I2 vs I1 and C, postintervention and at 6 months. No differences in willingness to taste V
Garden-enhanced nutrition education is an effective tool for improving nutritional knowledge and V preferences
Morgan et al. [178]
Education
Experience
To increase V intakes, V preferences and FV knowledge in 11- to 12-year-olds. School-based intervention
1. NE—10 week nutrition education (n = 35)
2. NEG—10 week nutrition education and garden (n = 35)
No intervention (wait list) (n = 57)
Greater willingness to taste V and greater taste ratings for I1 and I2 vs C. No differences in V intakes
School gardens can impact positively on willingness to taste V and V ratings, but more comprehensive strategies are required to increase V intakes
Robinson et al. [179]
Study 3
Memory
To increase V intakes in university students
Recall positive V memory
Recall of other memories
Increased V intake in I vs C
Recall of previous eating experiences could be a potential strategy for altering food choices
Stok et al. [180] Study 1
Education
To increase V intakes in university students
Descriptive social norm—majority norm
Descriptive social norm—minority norm
Marginally significant increase in V intakes in I vs C
A norm describing the behaviour of a salient social group can impact on behaviour
Ogawa et al. [181]
Education
To increase V purchasing behaviour in adults
Point of purchase (POP) health information for V presented in supermarket store for 60 days
Control store (same supermarket chain)—no information
Increased sales at I vs C. Adjustments made for seasonal effects and number of customers
Health-related POP information for V in supermarkets can encourage customers to purchase V
Rahman et al. [182]
Education
To increase dark leafy green vegetable (DLGV) presentation to children aged 6–35 months by mothers
1. health education, plus feeding demonstration (n = 44)
2. health education only (n = 36)
No intervention (usual practice) (n = 80)
Increased number of mothers presented DLGV at an impromptu meal 8 weeks later in I1 and 2 vs C
Maternal literacy and family income controlled for
Education for mothers was effective at increasing DLGV intakes in children
Tabak et al. [183]
Education
To increase presentation of V by mothers, and V intakes in children aged 2–5 years
Parents sent 4 tailored newsletters and given 2 motivational phone calls over 4 months (n = 22)
Parents sent 4 children’s books (1/month) (n = 21)
Increased availability and offering of V in I vs C. No differences in intakes
Home-based interventions to alter parental feeding practices and the home environment may help towards increasing V intake in children
Wenrich et al. [105]
Skills-based education
To increase serving and consumption of deep-orange, cruciferous and DGLV in families
8 weekly interactive sessions for food preparers, including recipes and handouts
8 weekly mailings that included similar recipes and handouts
No differences in servings or intakes between I vs C, at end of intervention or three-month follow-up. More recipe use by I vs C
Tools to help the food preparer draw family members into recipe evaluation are useful
Clarke et al. [184]
Education
To increase V intakes in clients of community pantries
1. Tailored tips and recipes (n = 244)
2. Generic tips and recipes (n = 226)
No tips or recipes (n = 236)
Increased V use in I1 vs I2 and C
Results demonstrated benefits of tailoring over both generic and control conditions
Interventions ordered by type and age of target audience
C comparison, I intervention, V vegetable, vs versus

Multi-component interventions

Multi-component interventions involve a combination of strategies (Table 6). Ten of these interventions were found. Again all the published reports evaluating these interventions report success, but again multiple measures are often taken, which demonstrate varying degrees of benefit. These types of intervention can also be time-consuming and costly to implement. Success is furthermore not easily attributable to the combination of many strategies as opposed to the use of any single one.
Table 6
Published multi-component interventions using a variety of strategies to increase vegetable intakes
Reference/intervention
Aim
Intervention
Comparison
Results
Authors’ conclusions
Faber et al. [185]
Education
Availability
Exposure
Experience
To improve intakes of yellow and dark green leafy V (DGLV) in children aged 2–5 years. A rural home gardening intervention
Home gardening programme in a rural village, as part of a primary care activity (n = 126 home gardens, 1/3 households)
Neighbouring village with no home gardening programme
At 20-month follow-up, children from I consumed yellow and DGLV more often vs C. Maternal knowledge also improved in I
A home gardening programme that was integrated with a primary health care activity, linked to nutrition education, and focused on the production of yellow and DGLV improved the vitamin A status of 2- to 5-y-old children in a rural village in South Africa
Bai et al. [186]
Education
Exposure
Rewards
Experience
To increase attitudes, intentions and V consumption. School-based intervention in children in US third grade
Nutrition education, poster displays, featured V in canteen (1/month for 9 months), and daily V tasting (4 preparations) (n = 38)
No intervention (usual practice) (n = 35)
Self-report V intakes higher in I vs C. Attitudes predicted consumption in I, social norms predicted consumption in C. Schools matched for race and gender profiles
The Veggiecation programme generated a positive attitude to influence vegetable intake in school
Leak et al. [187] (protocol only)
To increase V intake, liking and variety in children aged 9–12 years. Intervention for caregivers
Intervention based on 9 behavioural economics strategies for 6 weeks (n = 36)
Usual practice for 6 weeks (n = 10)
Protocol only
Protocol only
Wright et al. [188]
Education
Exposure
Experience
To increase V selection at a salad bar in kindergarten—5th grade children. School-based intervention
Gardening programme for three weeks, including V growing, tasting and consuming, and increasing knowledge
No control
V selection increased during I and continued to rise post I to a lesser extent
Gardening intervention lessons and activities impacted on V intakes
Ratcliffe et al. [189]
Education
Experience
To increase V intakes in children aged 11–13 years. School-based intervention
Health and science education. Garden based education (n = 170, 2 schools)
Health and science education only (n = 150)
Increased self-report recognition of, attitudes towards, preferences for, willingness to taste, and V variety consumed in I vs C. No differences in taste test
Gardening improved recognition of, attitudes towards, preferences for, willingness to taste and variety of V eaten
Brown et al. [97]
Education
Exposure
Experience
To increase readiness to change, V self-efficacy and V intakes in college students. College based intervention (n = 186)
Online preparation videos and tasting of 4 V, one per month for 4 months (n = 186)
No control
Stage of change and V self-efficacy increased postintervention. Intakes for one V increased, but no effects for other V or all V
Online V demonstration videos may be an effective and cost-efficient intervention for increasing self-efficacy of V preparation and readiness to increase V consumption among college students
Carney et al. [190]
Education
Experience
Social activities
To increase V intakes (reduce food security and improve family relationships) in families
Community gardening programme, including education, gardening experiences, and social activities (n = 42 families, 163 individuals)
No control
I increased V intakes (reduced food insecurity and improved family relationships) from pre- to postintervention
A community gardening programme can increase V intakes, reduce food insecurity and improve family relationships
Schreinemachers et al. [191]
Availability
Education
Experience
To increase V production, consumption and dietary diversity in families
Garden training, education, seeds, cooking, and garden equipment (n = 103)
No intervention (wait-list) (n = 479)
I resulted in increased V production, consumption and dietary variety vs C
Women’s home gardens are an effective intervention for increasing supply and consumption of a range of V in poor households, and so contributing to nutrition security
Kushida and Murayama [192]
Availability
Education
To increase V consumption behaviours in adults in workplace cafeterias
12 informational table tents placed every 2 weeks on all cafeteria tables, posters and locally grown V included in cafeteria menu. Personal dietary feedback for all participants (n = 181)
No intervention, Personal dietary feedback for all participants (n = 168)
Increased V consumption in I vs C in the cafeteria, and across the day (self-report)
Findings suggest a beneficial effect of providing access to nutrition information about V consumption
Weatherly and Weatherly [193]
Availability
Education
Experience
To increase V consumption (and increased interaction and self-worth) in homebound adults
Container V garden
No control
Reports of improved interaction and self-worth. Participants received fresh produce, but no results provided for V consumption
The containerised V garden programme has many benefits, for homebound adults and volunteer helpers
Interventions ordered by age of target audience
C comparison, I intervention, V vegetable, vs versus

Discussion

While a variety of successful strategies for increasing vegetable intakes have been tried, evaluated and published, evaluation periods are typically short, effect sizes can be small, and those studies that use longer follow-up periods often report reductions in effect size as follow-up periods are extended. These findings are unsurprising and have persuaded many researchers to recommend repeated interventions or a combination of interventions with the hope of improving long-term benefits. However, cost-effectiveness is rarely considered, yet cost-effectiveness becomes an increasing concern in long-lasting and multi-component interventions. Further work is clearly still required. A greater number and variety of intervention evaluations would increase the evidence base, and more reliably inform future policies. Longer-term follow-ups for interventions are imperative, and consideration of more sustainable behaviours or the more sustainable elements of behaviour, such as habit formation or behavioural norm changes, would be of value.
While the review highlights strategies of benefit furthermore, the review also identified noticeable absences. Based on the search strategies and current literature, very few interventions were identified specifically for adolescents or older individuals. The eating attitudes, practices and intakes of these groups are known to differ from those of other members of the population, and the simple generalisation of successful strategies from other population groups may not occur. Adolescence is a period of rapid development, from physical, cognitive and social perspectives, and changes to eating practices and dietary intake during this period are well reported [194]. Studies in this group on barriers to consumption identify constraints largely similar to those in younger children, but also identify an increased recognition of cognitive factors. Strategies then that involve education may be particularly beneficial. Older individuals similarly will experience changes in physical and cognitive abilities, many of which will have an impact on eating practices and food intake [195197]. Barriers specifically to vegetable consumption in this group have not been identified as far as we are aware, but barriers to fruit and vegetable consumption are similar to those for other adults [195, 196], although the impact of demographic characteristics and environmental factors tend to be exacerbated. Changes to living circumstances for example, will impact negatively on existing impacts as a result of availability, cost and cooking abilities [195197].
Vegetable consumption is also known to be low in individuals of low education and of low socio-economic status [198], and these factors are specifically highlighted as barriers to increasing consumption, yet few of the interventions published to date focus on or even include individuals with these demographics. There are some exceptions—the intervention by Clarke et al. [184] focuses specifically on individuals using community pantries, and many of the studies in the developing world focus not only on increasing vegetable intakes but also on sustainable vegetable provision and improved food security [185, 191], but more work is clearly needed in relation to socio-economic disparities. Interventions that improve fruit and vegetable intakes are available [199]. Increased efforts to reduce socio-economic disparities, however, are often requested [200203], and concerns that intervention success is most easily achieved in those of little need of benefit are difficult to allay.
Consideration of the barriers to vegetable consumption suggests that many of the strategies that have shown success so far in certain groups may be beneficial for other groups. Almost all individuals will arguably benefit from increasing vegetable intakes, and the strategies found to be successful in one population group may easily transfer to another. Exposure type strategies to increase liking, for example, have shown effects for fruit consumption in older adults as well as children [204], although vegetables were not specifically investigated here. We recommend careful consideration of barriers however, and caution against a ‘one size fits all’ approach. While interventions may be successful across individual and population groups, testing is clearly required. At present, there is a real lack of comparisons between interventions—i.e. interventions have not been compared, e.g. in the same age group or population, with the exception of comparisons of differing exposure and conditioning strategies in young children. This lack of comparison may reflect the early nature of the field, but even where multi-component interventions have been successful, identification of the successful component/components is rarely undertaken. With a view to lasting impact and cost-effectiveness, comparison of interventions, or the identification of more effective intervention components would clearly be of value.
Several types of broader population-based interventions have also not yet been considered specifically for vegetable consumption. Strategies such as pricing and marketing interventions, improved product provision, government subsidies, and population-wide awareness and education campaigns [205207] specifically for vegetables do not yet exist or have not yet been evaluated as far as we are aware. In some countries, WHO recommendations have been separated for fruit and vegetables. Dutch consumers are asked to consume 2 + 2 (2 portions of fruit and 2 portions of vegetables per day), and Australian consumers are asked to aim for 2 + 5 (2 portions of fruit and 5 portion of vegetables per day), but these types of recommendations rely heavily on an individual’s ability to identify and categorise fruits and vegetables, and limited work suggests that consumers find this difficult [41, 104].

Limitations of the review

While our review has identified a number and variety of interventions, we have only considered the published literature, and our search strategy is likely to be biased towards articles published in English and away from related grey literature, such as lay publications and conference proceedings. Due to the early nature of the research area, publication bias is also highly likely. There is a noticeable absence of publications that report failures, or that demonstrate cost-inefficiency or other negative impacts of interventions. We also chose not to attempt to combine interventions. Due to the early nature of the research area, the limited number of studies available per intervention type, and high heterogeneity between study methodologies, we considered combination to be inappropriate.

Future directions

There is an urgent need for the development and evaluation of interventions to target all population groups. Interventions are particularly required for certain population groups, including adolescents, older adults and those of low socio-economic status. Interventions for groups ‘at risk’ of disease may also be beneficial, given the often increased success of interventions in these individuals [1]. Assessments of the long-term benefits, sustainability and cost-effectiveness of interventions are also clearly required. While many interventions report success, effect sizes are typically small, long-term follow-up is rarely undertaken and studies that do report follow-up often fail to find sustained benefits. Interventions with a focus on long-term benefits and sustained behaviour change are required, as is increased work understanding the principles underlying behaviour change and behaviour change maintenance. Comparisons between interventions, to identify those of greatest likely benefit, would also be of interest.

Conclusion

In conclusion, increasing evidence suggests health benefits from the increased consumption specifically of vegetables, yet barriers to increasing intakes are prevalent, and while successful interventions have been published, the true value of these, both in cost-efficiency and sustainability are yet to be determined. Considerable further work is needed in developing new and adapting existing interventions for all population groups, and in evaluating benefit and cost-efficiency over the longer term.

Acknowledgments

This work is part of EU/FP7 Funded VeggiEAT project (Grant No. PIAP-GA-2013-612326). The funder had no input into study design, data collection or analysis, or manuscript preparation and revision.

Contributions of authors

K.M.A. led and undertook the systematic review and wrote the manuscript, A.H. and H.H. also undertook the systematic review, and reviewed and offered critical comments on the manuscript. All other authors reviewed and offered critical comments on the manuscript.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

L. Depezay and D. Morizet are employees of Bonduelle, Villeneuve D’Ascq, France. There are no other competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

Neuer Inhalt

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Woodside JV, Young IS, McKinley MC (2013) Fruits and vegetables: measuring intake and encouraging increased consumption. Proc Nutr Soc 72:236–245CrossRef Woodside JV, Young IS, McKinley MC (2013) Fruits and vegetables: measuring intake and encouraging increased consumption. Proc Nutr Soc 72:236–245CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Oyebode O, Gordon-Dseagu V, Walker A, Mindell JS (2014) Fruit and vegetable consumption and all-cause, cancer and CVD mortality: analysis of Health Survey for England data. J Epidemiol Community Health 68:856–862CrossRef Oyebode O, Gordon-Dseagu V, Walker A, Mindell JS (2014) Fruit and vegetable consumption and all-cause, cancer and CVD mortality: analysis of Health Survey for England data. J Epidemiol Community Health 68:856–862CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang X, Ouyang Y, Liu J et al (2014) Fruit and vegetable consumption and mortality from all causes, cardiovascular disease, and cancer: systematic review and dose–response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. BMJ 349:g4490CrossRef Wang X, Ouyang Y, Liu J et al (2014) Fruit and vegetable consumption and mortality from all causes, cardiovascular disease, and cancer: systematic review and dose–response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. BMJ 349:g4490CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Dauchet L, Amouyel P, Hercberg S, Dallongeville J (2006) Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of coronary heart disease: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. J Nutr 136:2588–2593 Dauchet L, Amouyel P, Hercberg S, Dallongeville J (2006) Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of coronary heart disease: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. J Nutr 136:2588–2593
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Dauchet L, Amouyel P, Dallongeville J (2005) Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of stroke: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. Neurology 65:1193–1197CrossRef Dauchet L, Amouyel P, Dallongeville J (2005) Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of stroke: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. Neurology 65:1193–1197CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Hu D, Huang J, Wang Y, Zhang D, Qu Y (2014) Fruits and vegetables consumption and risk of stroke: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Stroke 45:1613–1619CrossRef Hu D, Huang J, Wang Y, Zhang D, Qu Y (2014) Fruits and vegetables consumption and risk of stroke: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Stroke 45:1613–1619CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Savica V, Bellinghieri G, Kopple JD (2010) The effect of nutrition on blood pressure. Ann Rev Nutr 30:365–401CrossRef Savica V, Bellinghieri G, Kopple JD (2010) The effect of nutrition on blood pressure. Ann Rev Nutr 30:365–401CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Lanham New SA (2006) Fruit and vegetables: the unexpected natural answer to the question of osteoporosis prevention? Am J Clin Nutr 83:1254–1255 Lanham New SA (2006) Fruit and vegetables: the unexpected natural answer to the question of osteoporosis prevention? Am J Clin Nutr 83:1254–1255
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Astrup A, Dyerberg J, Selleck M, Stender S (2008) Nutrition transition and its relationship to the development of obesity and related chronic diseases. Obes Rev 9(Suppl 1):48–52CrossRef Astrup A, Dyerberg J, Selleck M, Stender S (2008) Nutrition transition and its relationship to the development of obesity and related chronic diseases. Obes Rev 9(Suppl 1):48–52CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Ledoux TA, Hingle MD, Baranowksi T (2011) Relationship of fruit and vegetable intake with adiposity: a systematic review. Obes Rev 12:e143–e150CrossRef Ledoux TA, Hingle MD, Baranowksi T (2011) Relationship of fruit and vegetable intake with adiposity: a systematic review. Obes Rev 12:e143–e150CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Lamport DJ, Saunders C, Butler LT, Spencer JP (2014) Fruits, vegetables, 100 % juices, and cognitive function. Nutr Rev 72:774–789CrossRef Lamport DJ, Saunders C, Butler LT, Spencer JP (2014) Fruits, vegetables, 100 % juices, and cognitive function. Nutr Rev 72:774–789CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Loef M, Walach H (2012) Fruit, vegetables and prevention of cognitive decline or dementia: a systematic review of cohort studies. J Nutr Health Ageing 16:626–630CrossRef Loef M, Walach H (2012) Fruit, vegetables and prevention of cognitive decline or dementia: a systematic review of cohort studies. J Nutr Health Ageing 16:626–630CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat American Institute for Cancer Research and World Cancer Research Fund. Food, Nutrition, Physical Activity and the Prevention of Cancer: A Global Perspective. (2007) American Institute for Cancer Research, Washington, DC American Institute for Cancer Research and World Cancer Research Fund. Food, Nutrition, Physical Activity and the Prevention of Cancer: A Global Perspective. (2007) American Institute for Cancer Research, Washington, DC
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Jin J, Ouyang Z, Wang Z (2014) Association of fruit and vegetables with the risk of nasopharyngeal cancer: evidence from a meta-analysis. Sci Rep. 4:5229 Jin J, Ouyang Z, Wang Z (2014) Association of fruit and vegetables with the risk of nasopharyngeal cancer: evidence from a meta-analysis. Sci Rep. 4:5229
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Li B, Jiang G, Zhang G et al (2014) Intake of vegetables and fruit and risk of esophageal adenocarcinoma: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Eur J Nutr 53:1511–1521CrossRef Li B, Jiang G, Zhang G et al (2014) Intake of vegetables and fruit and risk of esophageal adenocarcinoma: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Eur J Nutr 53:1511–1521CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat McCall DO, McGartland CP, McKinley MC et al (2009) Dietary intake of fruit and vegetables improves microvascular function in hypertensive subjects in a dose-dependent manner. Circulation 119:2153–2160CrossRef McCall DO, McGartland CP, McKinley MC et al (2009) Dietary intake of fruit and vegetables improves microvascular function in hypertensive subjects in a dose-dependent manner. Circulation 119:2153–2160CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Macready AL, George TW, Chong MF et al (2014) Flavonoid-rich fruit and vegetables improve microvascular reactivity and inflammatory status in men at risk of cardiovascular disease—FLAVURS: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 99:479–489CrossRef Macready AL, George TW, Chong MF et al (2014) Flavonoid-rich fruit and vegetables improve microvascular reactivity and inflammatory status in men at risk of cardiovascular disease—FLAVURS: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 99:479–489CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Yeon JY, Kim HS, Sung MK (2012) Diets rich in fruits and vegetables suppress blood biomarkers of metabolic stress in overweight women. Prev Med 54(Suppl):S109–S115CrossRef Yeon JY, Kim HS, Sung MK (2012) Diets rich in fruits and vegetables suppress blood biomarkers of metabolic stress in overweight women. Prev Med 54(Suppl):S109–S115CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson A, Edgar JD, Neville CE et al (2012) Effect of fruit and vegetable consumption on immune function in older people: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 96:1429–1436CrossRef Gibson A, Edgar JD, Neville CE et al (2012) Effect of fruit and vegetable consumption on immune function in older people: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 96:1429–1436CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanumihardjo SA, Valentine AR, Zhang Z, Whigham LD, Lai HJ, Atkinson RL (2009) Strategies to increase vegetable or reduce energy and fat intake induce weight loss in adults. Exp Biol Med 234:542–552CrossRef Tanumihardjo SA, Valentine AR, Zhang Z, Whigham LD, Lai HJ, Atkinson RL (2009) Strategies to increase vegetable or reduce energy and fat intake induce weight loss in adults. Exp Biol Med 234:542–552CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu RH (2013) Health-promoting components of fruits and vegetables in the diet. Adv Nutr 4:384S–392SCrossRef Liu RH (2013) Health-promoting components of fruits and vegetables in the diet. Adv Nutr 4:384S–392SCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Slavin JL, Lloyd B (2012) Health benefits of fruits and vegetables. Adv Nutr 3:506–516CrossRef Slavin JL, Lloyd B (2012) Health benefits of fruits and vegetables. Adv Nutr 3:506–516CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat de Pee S, West CE, Permaesih D, Martuti S, Muhilal HJ (1998) Orange fruit is more effective than are dark-green, leafy vegetables in increasing serum concentrations of retinol and beta-carotene in schoolchildren in Indonesia. Am J Clin Nutr 68:1058–1067 de Pee S, West CE, Permaesih D, Martuti S, Muhilal HJ (1998) Orange fruit is more effective than are dark-green, leafy vegetables in increasing serum concentrations of retinol and beta-carotene in schoolchildren in Indonesia. Am J Clin Nutr 68:1058–1067
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Sharma S, Sheehy T, Kolonel L (2014) Sources of vegetables, fruits and vitamins A, C and E among five ethnic groups: results from a multiethnic cohort study. Eur J Clin Nutr 68:384–391CrossRef Sharma S, Sheehy T, Kolonel L (2014) Sources of vegetables, fruits and vitamins A, C and E among five ethnic groups: results from a multiethnic cohort study. Eur J Clin Nutr 68:384–391CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Słupski J, Gębczyński P, Korus A, Lisiewska Z (2014) Effect of the method of preparation for consumption on calcium retention, calcium:phosphorus ratio, nutrient density and recommended daily allowance in fourteen vegetables. Int J Food Sci Nutr 65:458–464CrossRef Słupski J, Gębczyński P, Korus A, Lisiewska Z (2014) Effect of the method of preparation for consumption on calcium retention, calcium:phosphorus ratio, nutrient density and recommended daily allowance in fourteen vegetables. Int J Food Sci Nutr 65:458–464CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Gunn CA, Weber JL, Coad J, Kruger MC (2013) Increasing fruits and vegetables in midlife women: a feasibility study. Nutr Res 33:543–551CrossRef Gunn CA, Weber JL, Coad J, Kruger MC (2013) Increasing fruits and vegetables in midlife women: a feasibility study. Nutr Res 33:543–551CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Bongoni R, Verkerk R, Dekker M, Steenbekkers LP (2014) Consumer behaviour towards vegetables: a study on domestic processing of broccoli and carrots by Dutch households. J Hum Nutr Diet 28:219–225CrossRef Bongoni R, Verkerk R, Dekker M, Steenbekkers LP (2014) Consumer behaviour towards vegetables: a study on domestic processing of broccoli and carrots by Dutch households. J Hum Nutr Diet 28:219–225CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Jiménez-Monreal AM, García-Diz L, Martínez-Tomé M, Mariscal M, Murcia MA (2009) Influence of cooking methods on antioxidant activity of vegetables. J Food Sci 74:H97–H103CrossRef Jiménez-Monreal AM, García-Diz L, Martínez-Tomé M, Mariscal M, Murcia MA (2009) Influence of cooking methods on antioxidant activity of vegetables. J Food Sci 74:H97–H103CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Knecht K, Sandfuchs K, Kulling SE, Bunzel D (2015) Tocopherol and tocotrienol analysis in raw and cooked vegetables: a validated method with emphasis on sample preparation. Food Chem 169:20–27CrossRef Knecht K, Sandfuchs K, Kulling SE, Bunzel D (2015) Tocopherol and tocotrienol analysis in raw and cooked vegetables: a validated method with emphasis on sample preparation. Food Chem 169:20–27CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Nagao A, Kotake-Nara E, Hase M (2013) Effects of fats and oils on the bioaccessibility of carotenoids and vitamin E in vegetables. Biosci Biotechnol Biochem 77:1055–1060CrossRef Nagao A, Kotake-Nara E, Hase M (2013) Effects of fats and oils on the bioaccessibility of carotenoids and vitamin E in vegetables. Biosci Biotechnol Biochem 77:1055–1060CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Vinha AF, Alves RC, Barreira SV, Costa AS, Oliveira MB (2015) Impact of boiling on phytochemicals and antioxidant activity of green vegetables consumed in the Mediterranean diet. Food Funct 6:1157–1163CrossRef Vinha AF, Alves RC, Barreira SV, Costa AS, Oliveira MB (2015) Impact of boiling on phytochemicals and antioxidant activity of green vegetables consumed in the Mediterranean diet. Food Funct 6:1157–1163CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris MC, Evans DA, Tangney CC, Bienias JL, Wilson RS (2006) Associations of vegetable and fruit consumption with age-related cognitive change. Neurology. 67:1370–1376CrossRef Morris MC, Evans DA, Tangney CC, Bienias JL, Wilson RS (2006) Associations of vegetable and fruit consumption with age-related cognitive change. Neurology. 67:1370–1376CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Oskarsson V, Sadr-Azodi O, Orsini N, Andrén-Sandberg Å, Wolk A (2013) Vegetables, fruit and risk of non-gallstone-related acute pancreatitis: a population-based prospective cohort study. Gut 62:1187–1192CrossRef Oskarsson V, Sadr-Azodi O, Orsini N, Andrén-Sandberg Å, Wolk A (2013) Vegetables, fruit and risk of non-gallstone-related acute pancreatitis: a population-based prospective cohort study. Gut 62:1187–1192CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Villegas R, Shu XO, Gao YT et al (2008) Vegetable but not fruit consumption reduces the risk of type 2 diabetes in Chinese women. J Nutr 138:574–580 Villegas R, Shu XO, Gao YT et al (2008) Vegetable but not fruit consumption reduces the risk of type 2 diabetes in Chinese women. J Nutr 138:574–580
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Yang Y, Zhang D, Feng N et al (2014) Increased intake of vegetables, but not fruit, reduces risk for hepatocellular carcinoma: a meta-analysis. Gastroenterology 147:1031–1042CrossRef Yang Y, Zhang D, Feng N et al (2014) Increased intake of vegetables, but not fruit, reduces risk for hepatocellular carcinoma: a meta-analysis. Gastroenterology 147:1031–1042CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson AS, Cox DN, McKellar S, Reynolds J, Lean MEJ, Mela DJ (1998) Take five: a nutrition education intervention to increase fruit and vegetable intakes: Impact on attitudes towards dietary change. Brit J Nutr 80:133–140CrossRef Anderson AS, Cox DN, McKellar S, Reynolds J, Lean MEJ, Mela DJ (1998) Take five: a nutrition education intervention to increase fruit and vegetable intakes: Impact on attitudes towards dietary change. Brit J Nutr 80:133–140CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Brug J, de Vet E, de Nooijer J, Verplanken B (2006) Predicting fruit consumption: cognitions, intention, and habits. J Nutr Educ Behav 38:73–81CrossRef Brug J, de Vet E, de Nooijer J, Verplanken B (2006) Predicting fruit consumption: cognitions, intention, and habits. J Nutr Educ Behav 38:73–81CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Ransley JK, Greenwood DC, Cade JE et al (2007) Does the school fruit and vegetable scheme improve children’s diet? A non-randomised controlled trial. J Epidemiol Community Health 61:699–703CrossRef Ransley JK, Greenwood DC, Cade JE et al (2007) Does the school fruit and vegetable scheme improve children’s diet? A non-randomised controlled trial. J Epidemiol Community Health 61:699–703CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Sandeno C, Wolf G, Drake T, Reicks M (2000) Behavioral strategies to increase fruit and vegetable intake by fourth- through sixth-grade students. J Am Diet Assoc 100:828–830CrossRef Sandeno C, Wolf G, Drake T, Reicks M (2000) Behavioral strategies to increase fruit and vegetable intake by fourth- through sixth-grade students. J Am Diet Assoc 100:828–830CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Trudeau E, Kristal AR, Patterson RE (1998) Demographic and psychosocial predictors of fruit and vegetable intakes differ. Implications for dietary interventions. JADA 98:1412–1417CrossRef Trudeau E, Kristal AR, Patterson RE (1998) Demographic and psychosocial predictors of fruit and vegetable intakes differ. Implications for dietary interventions. JADA 98:1412–1417CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Glasson C, Chapman K, James E (2011) Fruit and vegetables should be targeted separately in health promotion programmes: differences in consumption levels, barriers, knowledge and stages of readiness for change. Public Health Nutr 14:694–701CrossRef Glasson C, Chapman K, James E (2011) Fruit and vegetables should be targeted separately in health promotion programmes: differences in consumption levels, barriers, knowledge and stages of readiness for change. Public Health Nutr 14:694–701CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Kamphius CBM, Giskes K, de Bruijn GJ, Wendel-Vos W, Brug J, von Lenthe FJ (2006) Environmental determinants of fruit and vegetable determinants among adults: a systematic review. Brit J Nutr 96:620–635 Kamphius CBM, Giskes K, de Bruijn GJ, Wendel-Vos W, Brug J, von Lenthe FJ (2006) Environmental determinants of fruit and vegetable determinants among adults: a systematic review. Brit J Nutr 96:620–635
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Perry CL, Bishop DB, Taylor G et al (1998) Changing fruit and vegetable consumption among children: the 5-a-day power plus program in St Paul, Minnesota. Am J Public Health 88:603–609CrossRef Perry CL, Bishop DB, Taylor G et al (1998) Changing fruit and vegetable consumption among children: the 5-a-day power plus program in St Paul, Minnesota. Am J Public Health 88:603–609CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Neville CE, McKinley MC, Draffin CR et al (2015) Participating in a fruit and vegetable intervention trial improves longer term fruit and vegetable consumption and barriers to fruit and vegetable consumption: a follow-up of the ADIT study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 12:158CrossRef Neville CE, McKinley MC, Draffin CR et al (2015) Participating in a fruit and vegetable intervention trial improves longer term fruit and vegetable consumption and barriers to fruit and vegetable consumption: a follow-up of the ADIT study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 12:158CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Bosetti C, Filomeno M, Riso P et al (2012) Cruciferous vegetables and cancer risk in a network of case–control studies. Ann Oncol 23:2198–2203CrossRef Bosetti C, Filomeno M, Riso P et al (2012) Cruciferous vegetables and cancer risk in a network of case–control studies. Ann Oncol 23:2198–2203CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu YT, Dai JJ, Xu CH et al (2012) Greater intake of fruit and vegetables is associated with lower risk of nasopharyngeal carcinoma in Chinese adults: a case–control study. Cancer Causes Control 23:589–599CrossRef Liu YT, Dai JJ, Xu CH et al (2012) Greater intake of fruit and vegetables is associated with lower risk of nasopharyngeal carcinoma in Chinese adults: a case–control study. Cancer Causes Control 23:589–599CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Masala G, Assedi M, Bendinelli B et al (2012) Fruit and vegetables consumption and breast cancer risk: the EPIC Italy study. Breast Cancer Res Treat 132:1127–1136CrossRef Masala G, Assedi M, Bendinelli B et al (2012) Fruit and vegetables consumption and breast cancer risk: the EPIC Italy study. Breast Cancer Res Treat 132:1127–1136CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Polesel J, Serraino D, Negri E et al (2013) Consumption of fruit, vegetables, and other food groups and the risk of nasopharyngeal carcinoma. Cancer Causes Control 24:1157–1165CrossRef Polesel J, Serraino D, Negri E et al (2013) Consumption of fruit, vegetables, and other food groups and the risk of nasopharyngeal carcinoma. Cancer Causes Control 24:1157–1165CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Luo AJ, Wang FZ, Luo D et al (2015) Consumption of vegetables may reduce the risk of liver cancer: results from a meta-analysis of case–control and cohort studies. Clin Res Hepatol Gastroenterol 39:45–51CrossRef Luo AJ, Wang FZ, Luo D et al (2015) Consumption of vegetables may reduce the risk of liver cancer: results from a meta-analysis of case–control and cohort studies. Clin Res Hepatol Gastroenterol 39:45–51CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Yao B, Yan Y, Ye X et al (2014) Intake of fruit and vegetables and risk of bladder cancer: a dose–response meta-analysis of observational studies. Cancer Causes Control 25:1645–1658CrossRef Yao B, Yan Y, Ye X et al (2014) Intake of fruit and vegetables and risk of bladder cancer: a dose–response meta-analysis of observational studies. Cancer Causes Control 25:1645–1658CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Aune D, Chan DS, Vieira AR et al (2012) Fruits, vegetables and breast cancer risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Breast Cancer Res Treat 134:479–493CrossRef Aune D, Chan DS, Vieira AR et al (2012) Fruits, vegetables and breast cancer risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Breast Cancer Res Treat 134:479–493CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang Q, Chen Y, Wang X, Gong G, Li G, Li C (2014) Consumption of fruit, but not vegetables, may reduce risk of gastric cancer: results from a meta-analysis of cohort studies. Eur J Cancer 50:1498–1509CrossRef Wang Q, Chen Y, Wang X, Gong G, Li G, Li C (2014) Consumption of fruit, but not vegetables, may reduce risk of gastric cancer: results from a meta-analysis of cohort studies. Eur J Cancer 50:1498–1509CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Koushik A, Spiegelman D, Albanes D et al (2012) Intake of fruits and vegetables and risk of pancreatic cancer in a pooled analysis of 14 cohort studies. Am J Epidemiol 176:373–386CrossRef Koushik A, Spiegelman D, Albanes D et al (2012) Intake of fruits and vegetables and risk of pancreatic cancer in a pooled analysis of 14 cohort studies. Am J Epidemiol 176:373–386CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Vieira AR, Vingeliene S, Chan DS et al (2015) Fruits, vegetables, and bladder cancer risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cancer Med 4:136–146CrossRef Vieira AR, Vingeliene S, Chan DS et al (2015) Fruits, vegetables, and bladder cancer risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cancer Med 4:136–146CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Carter P, Gray LJ, Troughton J, Khunti K, Davies MJ (2010) Fruit and vegetable intake and incidence of type 2 diabetes mellitus: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 341:c4229CrossRef Carter P, Gray LJ, Troughton J, Khunti K, Davies MJ (2010) Fruit and vegetable intake and incidence of type 2 diabetes mellitus: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 341:c4229CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Cooper AJ, Forouhi NG, Ye Z et al (2012) Fruit and vegetable intake and type 2 diabetes: EPIC-InterAct prospective study and meta-analysis. Eur J Clin Nutr 66:1082–1092CrossRef Cooper AJ, Forouhi NG, Ye Z et al (2012) Fruit and vegetable intake and type 2 diabetes: EPIC-InterAct prospective study and meta-analysis. Eur J Clin Nutr 66:1082–1092CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Li M, Fan Y, Zhang X et al (2014) Fruit and vegetable intake and risk of type 2 diabetes mellitus: meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. BMJ Open 5(4):e005497CrossRef Li M, Fan Y, Zhang X et al (2014) Fruit and vegetable intake and risk of type 2 diabetes mellitus: meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. BMJ Open 5(4):e005497CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Wu Y, Zhang D, Jiang X, Jiang W (2015) Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of type 2 diabetes mellitus: a dose response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis 25:140–147CrossRef Wu Y, Zhang D, Jiang X, Jiang W (2015) Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of type 2 diabetes mellitus: a dose response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis 25:140–147CrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Takata Y, Xiang YB, Yang G et al (2013) Intakes of fruits, vegetables, and related vitamins and lung cancer risk: results from the Shanghai Men’s Health Study (2002–2009). Nutr Cancer 65:51–61CrossRef Takata Y, Xiang YB, Yang G et al (2013) Intakes of fruits, vegetables, and related vitamins and lung cancer risk: results from the Shanghai Men’s Health Study (2002–2009). Nutr Cancer 65:51–61CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Tsai AC, Chang TL, Chi SH (2012) Frequent consumption of vegetables predicts lower risk of depression in older Taiwanese—results of a prospective population-based study. Public Health Nutr 15:1087–1092CrossRef Tsai AC, Chang TL, Chi SH (2012) Frequent consumption of vegetables predicts lower risk of depression in older Taiwanese—results of a prospective population-based study. Public Health Nutr 15:1087–1092CrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen GC, Lv DB, Pang Z, Liu QF (2013) Fruits and vegetables consumption and risk of non-Hodgkin’s lymphoma: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Int J Cancer 133:190–200CrossRef Chen GC, Lv DB, Pang Z, Liu QF (2013) Fruits and vegetables consumption and risk of non-Hodgkin’s lymphoma: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Int J Cancer 133:190–200CrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Han B, Li X, Yu T (2014) Cruciferous vegetables consumption and the risk of ovarian cancer: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Diagn Pathol 9:7CrossRef Han B, Li X, Yu T (2014) Cruciferous vegetables consumption and the risk of ovarian cancer: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Diagn Pathol 9:7CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu X, Lv K (2013) Cruciferous vegetables intake is inversely associated with risk of breast cancer: a meta-analysis. Breast 22:309–313CrossRef Liu X, Lv K (2013) Cruciferous vegetables intake is inversely associated with risk of breast cancer: a meta-analysis. Breast 22:309–313CrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu B, Mao Q, Cao M, Xie L (2012) Cruciferous vegetables intake and risk of prostate cancer: a meta-analysis. Int J Urol 19:134–141CrossRef Liu B, Mao Q, Cao M, Xie L (2012) Cruciferous vegetables intake and risk of prostate cancer: a meta-analysis. Int J Urol 19:134–141CrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu B, Mao Q, Lin Y, Zhou F, Xie L (2013) The association of cruciferous vegetables intake and risk of bladder cancer: a meta-analysis. World J Urol 31:127–133CrossRef Liu B, Mao Q, Lin Y, Zhou F, Xie L (2013) The association of cruciferous vegetables intake and risk of bladder cancer: a meta-analysis. World J Urol 31:127–133CrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu B, Mao Q, Wang X et al (2013) Cruciferous vegetables consumption and risk of renal cell carcinoma: a meta-analysis. Nutr Cancer 65:668–676CrossRef Liu B, Mao Q, Wang X et al (2013) Cruciferous vegetables consumption and risk of renal cell carcinoma: a meta-analysis. Nutr Cancer 65:668–676CrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Wu QJ, Yang Y, Vogtmann E et al (2013) Cruciferous vegetables intake and the risk of colorectal cancer: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Ann Oncol 24:1079–1087CrossRef Wu QJ, Yang Y, Vogtmann E et al (2013) Cruciferous vegetables intake and the risk of colorectal cancer: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Ann Oncol 24:1079–1087CrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhao J, Zhao L (2013) Cruciferous vegetables intake is associated with lower risk of renal cell carcinoma: evidence from a meta-analysis of observational studies. PLoS ONE 8:e75732CrossRef Zhao J, Zhao L (2013) Cruciferous vegetables intake is associated with lower risk of renal cell carcinoma: evidence from a meta-analysis of observational studies. PLoS ONE 8:e75732CrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Key TJ (2011) Fruit and vegetables and cancer risk. Br J Cancer 104:6–11CrossRef Key TJ (2011) Fruit and vegetables and cancer risk. Br J Cancer 104:6–11CrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Fulton SL, McKinley MC, Young IS, Cardwell CR, Woodside JV (2014) The effect of increasing fruit and vegetable consumption on overall diet: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Critical Rev Food Sci Nutr. doi:10.1080/10408398.2012.727917 Fulton SL, McKinley MC, Young IS, Cardwell CR, Woodside JV (2014) The effect of increasing fruit and vegetable consumption on overall diet: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Critical Rev Food Sci Nutr. doi:10.​1080/​10408398.​2012.​727917
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Brug J, Tak NI, te Velde SJ, Bere E, de Bourdeaudhuij I (2008) Taste preferences, liking and other factors related to fruit and vegetable intakes among schoolchildren: results from observational studies. Br J Nutr 99(suppl 1):S7–S14 Brug J, Tak NI, te Velde SJ, Bere E, de Bourdeaudhuij I (2008) Taste preferences, liking and other factors related to fruit and vegetable intakes among schoolchildren: results from observational studies. Br J Nutr 99(suppl 1):S7–S14
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson EL, Wardle J, Watts CJ (1998) Fruit and vegetable consumption, nutritional knowledge and beliefs in mothers and children. Appetite 31:205–228CrossRef Gibson EL, Wardle J, Watts CJ (1998) Fruit and vegetable consumption, nutritional knowledge and beliefs in mothers and children. Appetite 31:205–228CrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Wardle J, Cooke LJ, Gibson EL, Sapochnik M, Sheiham A, Lawson M (2003) Increasing children’s acceptance of vegetables; a randomized controlled trial of parent-led exposure. Appetite 40:155–162CrossRef Wardle J, Cooke LJ, Gibson EL, Sapochnik M, Sheiham A, Lawson M (2003) Increasing children’s acceptance of vegetables; a randomized controlled trial of parent-led exposure. Appetite 40:155–162CrossRef
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Dovey TM, Staples PA, Gibson EL, Halford JCG (2008) Food neophobia and ‘picky/fussy’ eating in children: a review. Appetite 50:181–193CrossRef Dovey TM, Staples PA, Gibson EL, Halford JCG (2008) Food neophobia and ‘picky/fussy’ eating in children: a review. Appetite 50:181–193CrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Mustonen S, Oerlemans P, Tuorila H (2012) Familiarity with and affective responses to foods in 8–11-year-old children: the role of food neophobia and parental education. Appetite 58:777–780CrossRef Mustonen S, Oerlemans P, Tuorila H (2012) Familiarity with and affective responses to foods in 8–11-year-old children: the role of food neophobia and parental education. Appetite 58:777–780CrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Kelder SH, Perry CL, Klepp KI, Lytle L (1994) Longitudinal tracking of adolescent smoking, physical activity and food choice behaviors. Am J Pub Health 84:1121–1126CrossRef Kelder SH, Perry CL, Klepp KI, Lytle L (1994) Longitudinal tracking of adolescent smoking, physical activity and food choice behaviors. Am J Pub Health 84:1121–1126CrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Poelman AAM, Delahunty CM, de Graaf C (2015) Vegetable preparation practices for 5–6 years old Australian children as reported by their parents; relationships with liking and consumption. Food Qual Pref 42:20–26CrossRef Poelman AAM, Delahunty CM, de Graaf C (2015) Vegetable preparation practices for 5–6 years old Australian children as reported by their parents; relationships with liking and consumption. Food Qual Pref 42:20–26CrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Bjelland M, Brantsæter AL, Haugen M, Meltzer HM, Nystad W, Andersen LF (2013) Changes and tracking of fruit, vegetables and sugar-sweetened beverages intake from 18 months to 7 years in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. BMC Public Health 13:793CrossRef Bjelland M, Brantsæter AL, Haugen M, Meltzer HM, Nystad W, Andersen LF (2013) Changes and tracking of fruit, vegetables and sugar-sweetened beverages intake from 18 months to 7 years in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. BMC Public Health 13:793CrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Burnier D, Dubois L, Girard M (2011) Exclusive breastfeeding duration and later intake of vegetables in preschool children. Eur J Clin Nutr 65:196–202CrossRef Burnier D, Dubois L, Girard M (2011) Exclusive breastfeeding duration and later intake of vegetables in preschool children. Eur J Clin Nutr 65:196–202CrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Valmórbida JL, Vitolo MR (2014) Factors associated with low consumption of fruits and vegetables by preschoolers of low socio-economic level. J Pediatr 90:464–471CrossRef Valmórbida JL, Vitolo MR (2014) Factors associated with low consumption of fruits and vegetables by preschoolers of low socio-economic level. J Pediatr 90:464–471CrossRef
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Sweetman C, McGowan L, Croker H, Cooke L (2011) Characteristics of family mealtimes affecting children’s vegetable consumption and liking. JADA 111:269–273CrossRef Sweetman C, McGowan L, Croker H, Cooke L (2011) Characteristics of family mealtimes affecting children’s vegetable consumption and liking. JADA 111:269–273CrossRef
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Wroten KC, O’Neil CE, Stuff JE, Liu Y, Nicklas TA (2012) Resemblance of dietary intakes of snacks, sweets, fruit, and vegetables among mother–child dyads from low income families. Appetite 59:316–323CrossRef Wroten KC, O’Neil CE, Stuff JE, Liu Y, Nicklas TA (2012) Resemblance of dietary intakes of snacks, sweets, fruit, and vegetables among mother–child dyads from low income families. Appetite 59:316–323CrossRef
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Zeinstra GG, Koelen MA, Kok FJ, van der Laan, de Graaf C (2009) Parental child-feeding strategies in relation to Dutch children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Pub Health Nutr. 13:787–796CrossRef Zeinstra GG, Koelen MA, Kok FJ, van der Laan, de Graaf C (2009) Parental child-feeding strategies in relation to Dutch children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Pub Health Nutr. 13:787–796CrossRef
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Koui E, Jago R (2008) Associations between self-reported fruit and vegetable consumption and home availability of fruit and vegetables among Greek primary-school children. Pub Health Nutr 11:1142–1148CrossRef Koui E, Jago R (2008) Associations between self-reported fruit and vegetable consumption and home availability of fruit and vegetables among Greek primary-school children. Pub Health Nutr 11:1142–1148CrossRef
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Melbye EL, Øverby NC, Øgaard T (2012) Child consumption of fruit and vegetables: the roles of child cognitions and parental feeding practices. Pub Health Nutr 15:1047–1055CrossRef Melbye EL, Øverby NC, Øgaard T (2012) Child consumption of fruit and vegetables: the roles of child cognitions and parental feeding practices. Pub Health Nutr 15:1047–1055CrossRef
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Giskes K, Turrell G, Patterson C, Newman B (2002) Socioeconomic differences in fruit and vegetable consumption among Australian adolescents and adults. Publ Health Nutr 5:663–669CrossRef Giskes K, Turrell G, Patterson C, Newman B (2002) Socioeconomic differences in fruit and vegetable consumption among Australian adolescents and adults. Publ Health Nutr 5:663–669CrossRef
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Cullen KW, Baranowski T, Rittenberry L, Cosart C, Hebert D, de Moor C (2001) Child-reported family and peer influences on fruit, juice and vegetable consumption: reliability and validity of measures. Health Educ Res 16:187–200CrossRef Cullen KW, Baranowski T, Rittenberry L, Cosart C, Hebert D, de Moor C (2001) Child-reported family and peer influences on fruit, juice and vegetable consumption: reliability and validity of measures. Health Educ Res 16:187–200CrossRef
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Hendy HM, Raudenbush B (2000) Effectiveness of teacher modelling to encourage food acceptance in preschool children. Appetite 34:61–76CrossRef Hendy HM, Raudenbush B (2000) Effectiveness of teacher modelling to encourage food acceptance in preschool children. Appetite 34:61–76CrossRef
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Peltzer K, Pengpid S (2010) Fruits and vegetables consumption and associated factors among in-school adolescents in seven African countries. Int J Public Health 55:669–678CrossRef Peltzer K, Pengpid S (2010) Fruits and vegetables consumption and associated factors among in-school adolescents in seven African countries. Int J Public Health 55:669–678CrossRef
92.
Zurück zum Zitat Draxten M, Fulkerson JA, Friend S, Flattum CF, Schow R (2014) Parental role modeling of fruits and vegetables at meals and snacks is associated with children’s adequate consumption. Appetite 78:1–7CrossRef Draxten M, Fulkerson JA, Friend S, Flattum CF, Schow R (2014) Parental role modeling of fruits and vegetables at meals and snacks is associated with children’s adequate consumption. Appetite 78:1–7CrossRef
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Middlestadt SE, Lederer AM, Smith NK et al (2013) Determinants of middle-school students asking parents for fruits and vegetables: a theory-based salient belief elicitation. Public Health Nutr. 16:1971–1978CrossRef Middlestadt SE, Lederer AM, Smith NK et al (2013) Determinants of middle-school students asking parents for fruits and vegetables: a theory-based salient belief elicitation. Public Health Nutr. 16:1971–1978CrossRef
94.
Zurück zum Zitat Larson NI, Neumark-Sztainer DR, Harnack LJ, Wall MM, Story MT, Eisenberg ME (2008) Fruit and vegetable intake correlates during the transition to young adulthood. Am J Prev Med 35:33–37CrossRef Larson NI, Neumark-Sztainer DR, Harnack LJ, Wall MM, Story MT, Eisenberg ME (2008) Fruit and vegetable intake correlates during the transition to young adulthood. Am J Prev Med 35:33–37CrossRef
95.
Zurück zum Zitat Darian JC, Tucci L (2013) Developing marketing strategies to increase vegetable consumption. J Cons Marketing 5:427–435CrossRef Darian JC, Tucci L (2013) Developing marketing strategies to increase vegetable consumption. J Cons Marketing 5:427–435CrossRef
96.
Zurück zum Zitat McMahon AT, Tapsell L, Williams P, Jobling J (2013) Baby leafy green vegetables: providing insight into an old problem? An exploratory qualitative study examining influences on their consumption. Health Promot J Austr 24:68–71 McMahon AT, Tapsell L, Williams P, Jobling J (2013) Baby leafy green vegetables: providing insight into an old problem? An exploratory qualitative study examining influences on their consumption. Health Promot J Austr 24:68–71
97.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown KN, Wengreen HJ, Vitale TS, Anderson JB (2011) Increased self-efficacy for vegetable preparation following an online, skill-based intervention and in-class tasting experience as a part of a general education college nutrition course. Am J Health Promot. 26:14–20CrossRef Brown KN, Wengreen HJ, Vitale TS, Anderson JB (2011) Increased self-efficacy for vegetable preparation following an online, skill-based intervention and in-class tasting experience as a part of a general education college nutrition course. Am J Health Promot. 26:14–20CrossRef
98.
Zurück zum Zitat Vihotogbe-Sossa CN, Akissoe NH, Anihouvi VB et al (2012) Endogenous knowledge of four leafy vegetables used by rural populations in Benin. Ecol Food Nutr 51:22–39CrossRef Vihotogbe-Sossa CN, Akissoe NH, Anihouvi VB et al (2012) Endogenous knowledge of four leafy vegetables used by rural populations in Benin. Ecol Food Nutr 51:22–39CrossRef
99.
Zurück zum Zitat Kremer-Sadlik T, Morgenstern A, Peters C et al (2015) Eating fruits and vegetables. An ethnographic study of American and French family dinners. Appetite 89:84–92CrossRef Kremer-Sadlik T, Morgenstern A, Peters C et al (2015) Eating fruits and vegetables. An ethnographic study of American and French family dinners. Appetite 89:84–92CrossRef
100.
Zurück zum Zitat Myhre JB, Løken EB, Wandel M, Andersen LF (2014) Meal types as sources for intakes of fruits, vegetables, fish and whole grains among Norwegian adults. Public Health Nutr 11:1–11 Myhre JB, Løken EB, Wandel M, Andersen LF (2014) Meal types as sources for intakes of fruits, vegetables, fish and whole grains among Norwegian adults. Public Health Nutr 11:1–11
101.
Zurück zum Zitat Golding J, Steer C, Emmett P, Bartoshuk LM, Horwood J, Smith GF (2009) Associations between the ability to detect a bitter taste, dietary behaviour and growth: a preliminary report. Ann NY Acad Sci 1170:563–567CrossRef Golding J, Steer C, Emmett P, Bartoshuk LM, Horwood J, Smith GF (2009) Associations between the ability to detect a bitter taste, dietary behaviour and growth: a preliminary report. Ann NY Acad Sci 1170:563–567CrossRef
102.
Zurück zum Zitat Knaapila A, Silventoinen K, Broms U et al (2011) Food neophobia in young adults: genetic architecture and relation to personality, pleasantness and use frequency of foods, and body mass index—a twin study. Behav Genet 41:512–521CrossRef Knaapila A, Silventoinen K, Broms U et al (2011) Food neophobia in young adults: genetic architecture and relation to personality, pleasantness and use frequency of foods, and body mass index—a twin study. Behav Genet 41:512–521CrossRef
103.
Zurück zum Zitat Tornwall O, Silventoinen K, Hiekkalinna T (2014) Identifying flavor preference subgroups. Genetic basis and related eating behavior traits. Appetite 75:1–10CrossRef Tornwall O, Silventoinen K, Hiekkalinna T (2014) Identifying flavor preference subgroups. Genetic basis and related eating behavior traits. Appetite 75:1–10CrossRef
104.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown JL, Wenrich TR (2012) Intra-family role expectations and reluctance to change identified as key barriers to expanding vegetable consumption patterns during interactive family-based program for appalachian low-income food preparers. JAND 112:1188–1200 Brown JL, Wenrich TR (2012) Intra-family role expectations and reluctance to change identified as key barriers to expanding vegetable consumption patterns during interactive family-based program for appalachian low-income food preparers. JAND 112:1188–1200
105.
Zurück zum Zitat Wenrich TR, Brown JL, Wilson RT, Lengerich EJ (2012) Impact of a community-based intervention on serving and intake of vegetables among low-income, rural Appalachian families. J Nutr Educ Behav 44:36–45CrossRef Wenrich TR, Brown JL, Wilson RT, Lengerich EJ (2012) Impact of a community-based intervention on serving and intake of vegetables among low-income, rural Appalachian families. J Nutr Educ Behav 44:36–45CrossRef
106.
Zurück zum Zitat Izumi BT, Zenk SN, Schulz AJ, Mentz GB, Wilson C (2011) Associations between neighborhood availability and individual consumption of dark-green and orange vegetables among ethnically diverse adults in Detroit. JADA. 111:274–279CrossRef Izumi BT, Zenk SN, Schulz AJ, Mentz GB, Wilson C (2011) Associations between neighborhood availability and individual consumption of dark-green and orange vegetables among ethnically diverse adults in Detroit. JADA. 111:274–279CrossRef
107.
Zurück zum Zitat Yen ST, Tan AK (2012) Who are eating and not eating fruits and vegetables in Malaysia? Int J Pub Health 57:945–951CrossRef Yen ST, Tan AK (2012) Who are eating and not eating fruits and vegetables in Malaysia? Int J Pub Health 57:945–951CrossRef
108.
Zurück zum Zitat Claro RM, Monteiro CA (2010) Family income, food prices, and household purchases of fruits and vegetables in Brazil. Rev Saude Publica 44:1014–1020CrossRef Claro RM, Monteiro CA (2010) Family income, food prices, and household purchases of fruits and vegetables in Brazil. Rev Saude Publica 44:1014–1020CrossRef
109.
Zurück zum Zitat Franchini B, Poínhos R, Klepp KI, Vaz de Almeida MD (2013) Fruit and vegetables: intake and sociodemographic determinants among Portuguese mothers. Ann Nutr Metab 63:131–138CrossRef Franchini B, Poínhos R, Klepp KI, Vaz de Almeida MD (2013) Fruit and vegetables: intake and sociodemographic determinants among Portuguese mothers. Ann Nutr Metab 63:131–138CrossRef
110.
Zurück zum Zitat Kiadaliri AA (2013) Demographic and socioeconomic differences in fruit and vegetables consumption, 2007–2009: a province-level study in Iran. Int J Prev Med 4:831–840 Kiadaliri AA (2013) Demographic and socioeconomic differences in fruit and vegetables consumption, 2007–2009: a province-level study in Iran. Int J Prev Med 4:831–840
111.
Zurück zum Zitat Dijkstra SC, Neter JE, Brouwer IA, Huisman M, Visser M (2014) Adherence to dietary guidelines for fruit, vegetables and fish among older Dutch adults; the role of education, income and job prestige. J Nutr Health Ageing 18:115–121CrossRef Dijkstra SC, Neter JE, Brouwer IA, Huisman M, Visser M (2014) Adherence to dietary guidelines for fruit, vegetables and fish among older Dutch adults; the role of education, income and job prestige. J Nutr Health Ageing 18:115–121CrossRef
112.
Zurück zum Zitat Gatto NM, Ventura EE, Cook LT, Gyllenhammer LE, Davis JN (2012) LA Sprouts: a garden-based nutrition intervention pilot program influences motivation and preferences for fruits and vegetables in Latino youth. JAND. 112:913–920 Gatto NM, Ventura EE, Cook LT, Gyllenhammer LE, Davis JN (2012) LA Sprouts: a garden-based nutrition intervention pilot program influences motivation and preferences for fruits and vegetables in Latino youth. JAND. 112:913–920
113.
Zurück zum Zitat Sweitzer SJ, Ranjit N, Calloway EE et al (2014) Examining how adding a booster to a behavioral nutrition intervention prompts parents to pakc more vegetables and whole grains in their preschool children’s sack lunches. Behav Med 1–9 [Epub ahead of print] Sweitzer SJ, Ranjit N, Calloway EE et al (2014) Examining how adding a booster to a behavioral nutrition intervention prompts parents to pakc more vegetables and whole grains in their preschool children’s sack lunches. Behav Med 1–9 [Epub ahead of print]
114.
Zurück zum Zitat Uglem S, Stea TH, Kjøllesdal MK, Frølich W, Wandel M (2013) A nutrition intervention with a main focus on vegetables and bread consumption among young men in the Norwegian National Guard. Food Nutr Res 57. doi:10.3402/fnr.v57i0.21036 Uglem S, Stea TH, Kjøllesdal MK, Frølich W, Wandel M (2013) A nutrition intervention with a main focus on vegetables and bread consumption among young men in the Norwegian National Guard. Food Nutr Res 57. doi:10.​3402/​fnr.​v57i0.​21036
115.
Zurück zum Zitat Imai S, Matsuda M, Hasegawa G et al (2011) A simple meal plan of ‘eating vegetables before carbohydrate’ was more effective for achieving glycemic control than an exchange-based meal plan in Japanese patients with type 2 diabetes. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 20:161–168 Imai S, Matsuda M, Hasegawa G et al (2011) A simple meal plan of ‘eating vegetables before carbohydrate’ was more effective for achieving glycemic control than an exchange-based meal plan in Japanese patients with type 2 diabetes. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 20:161–168
116.
Zurück zum Zitat Snee LS, Nerukar VR, Dooley DA, Efird JT, Shovic AC, Nerukar PV (2011) Strategies to improve palatability and increase consumption intentions for Momordica charantia (bitter melon): a vegetable commonly used for diabetes management. Nutr J 10:78CrossRef Snee LS, Nerukar VR, Dooley DA, Efird JT, Shovic AC, Nerukar PV (2011) Strategies to improve palatability and increase consumption intentions for Momordica charantia (bitter melon): a vegetable commonly used for diabetes management. Nutr J 10:78CrossRef
117.
Zurück zum Zitat Wall DE, Least C, Gromis J, Lohse B (2012) Nutrition education intervention improves vegetable-related attitude, self-efficacy, preference and knowledge of fourth-grade students. J School Health 82:37–43CrossRef Wall DE, Least C, Gromis J, Lohse B (2012) Nutrition education intervention improves vegetable-related attitude, self-efficacy, preference and knowledge of fourth-grade students. J School Health 82:37–43CrossRef
118.
Zurück zum Zitat Poelman AAM, Delahunty CM (2011) The effect of preparation method and typicality of colour on children’s acceptance for vegetables. Food Qual Pref 22:355–364CrossRef Poelman AAM, Delahunty CM (2011) The effect of preparation method and typicality of colour on children’s acceptance for vegetables. Food Qual Pref 22:355–364CrossRef
119.
Zurück zum Zitat Poelman AAM, Delahunty CM, de Graaf C (2013) Cooking time, but not cooking method affects children’s acceptance of Brassica vegetables. Food Qual Pref 28:441–448CrossRef Poelman AAM, Delahunty CM, de Graaf C (2013) Cooking time, but not cooking method affects children’s acceptance of Brassica vegetables. Food Qual Pref 28:441–448CrossRef
120.
Zurück zum Zitat Barends C, de Vries JH, Mojet J, de Graaf C (2013) Effects of repeated exposure to either vegetables or fruit on infant’s vegetable and fruit acceptance at the beginning of weaning. Food Qual Pref 29:157–165CrossRef Barends C, de Vries JH, Mojet J, de Graaf C (2013) Effects of repeated exposure to either vegetables or fruit on infant’s vegetable and fruit acceptance at the beginning of weaning. Food Qual Pref 29:157–165CrossRef
121.
Zurück zum Zitat Bouhlal S, Issanchou S, Nicklaus S (2011) The impact of salt, fat and sugar levels on toddler food intake. Br J Nutr 105:645–653CrossRef Bouhlal S, Issanchou S, Nicklaus S (2011) The impact of salt, fat and sugar levels on toddler food intake. Br J Nutr 105:645–653CrossRef
122.
Zurück zum Zitat Savage JS, Peterson J, Marini M, Bordi PL Jr, Birch LL (2013) The addition of a plain or herb-flavored reduced-fat dip is associated with improved preschoolers’ intake of vegetables. JAND. 113:1090–1095 Savage JS, Peterson J, Marini M, Bordi PL Jr, Birch LL (2013) The addition of a plain or herb-flavored reduced-fat dip is associated with improved preschoolers’ intake of vegetables. JAND. 113:1090–1095
123.
Zurück zum Zitat Bouhlal S, Chabanet C, Issanchou S et al (2013) Salt content impacts food preferences and intake among children. PLoS One 8:e53971CrossRef Bouhlal S, Chabanet C, Issanchou S et al (2013) Salt content impacts food preferences and intake among children. PLoS One 8:e53971CrossRef
124.
Zurück zum Zitat Morizet D, Depezay L, Combris P, Picard D, Giboreau A (2012) Effect of labeling on new vegetable dish acceptance in preadolescent children. Appetite 59:399–402CrossRef Morizet D, Depezay L, Combris P, Picard D, Giboreau A (2012) Effect of labeling on new vegetable dish acceptance in preadolescent children. Appetite 59:399–402CrossRef
125.
Zurück zum Zitat Ahearn WH (2003) Using simultaneous presentation to increase vegetables consumption in a mildly selective child with autism. J Appl Behav Anal 36:361–365CrossRef Ahearn WH (2003) Using simultaneous presentation to increase vegetables consumption in a mildly selective child with autism. J Appl Behav Anal 36:361–365CrossRef
126.
Zurück zum Zitat Remy E, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Nicklaus S (2013) Repeated exposure of infants at complementary feeding to a vegetable puree increases acceptance as effectively as flavor–flavor learning and more effectively than flavor–nutrient learning. J Nutr 143:1194–1200CrossRef Remy E, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Nicklaus S (2013) Repeated exposure of infants at complementary feeding to a vegetable puree increases acceptance as effectively as flavor–flavor learning and more effectively than flavor–nutrient learning. J Nutr 143:1194–1200CrossRef
127.
Zurück zum Zitat Mennella JA, Nicklaus S, Jagolino AL, Yourshaw LM (2008) Variety is the spice of life: strategies for promoting fruit and vegetable acceptance during infancy. Physiol Behav 94:29–38CrossRef Mennella JA, Nicklaus S, Jagolino AL, Yourshaw LM (2008) Variety is the spice of life: strategies for promoting fruit and vegetable acceptance during infancy. Physiol Behav 94:29–38CrossRef
128.
Zurück zum Zitat Mennella JA, Kennedy JM, Beauchamp GK (2006) Vegetable acceptance by infants: effects of formula flavors. Early Hum Dev 82:463–468CrossRef Mennella JA, Kennedy JM, Beauchamp GK (2006) Vegetable acceptance by infants: effects of formula flavors. Early Hum Dev 82:463–468CrossRef
129.
Zurück zum Zitat Hetherington MM, Schwartz C, Madrelle J et al (2015) A step-by-step introduction to vegetables at the beginning of complementary feeding. The effects of early and repeated exposure. Appetite 84:280–290CrossRef Hetherington MM, Schwartz C, Madrelle J et al (2015) A step-by-step introduction to vegetables at the beginning of complementary feeding. The effects of early and repeated exposure. Appetite 84:280–290CrossRef
130.
Zurück zum Zitat Maier A, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Issanchou S, Leathwood P (2007) Effects of repeated exposure on acceptance of initially disliked vegetables in 7-month old infants. Food Qual Pref. 18:1023–1032CrossRef Maier A, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Issanchou S, Leathwood P (2007) Effects of repeated exposure on acceptance of initially disliked vegetables in 7-month old infants. Food Qual Pref. 18:1023–1032CrossRef
131.
Zurück zum Zitat Caton SJ, Ahern SM, Remy E, Nicklaus S, Blundell P, Hetherington MM (2013) Repetition counts: repeated exposure increases intake of a novel vegetable in UK pre-school children compared to flavour–flavour and flavour–nutrient learning. Br J Nutr 109:2089–2097CrossRef Caton SJ, Ahern SM, Remy E, Nicklaus S, Blundell P, Hetherington MM (2013) Repetition counts: repeated exposure increases intake of a novel vegetable in UK pre-school children compared to flavour–flavour and flavour–nutrient learning. Br J Nutr 109:2089–2097CrossRef
132.
Zurück zum Zitat Barends C, de Vries JH, Mojet J, de Graaf C (2014) Effects of starting weaning exclusively with vegetables on vegetable intake at the age of 12 and 23 months. Appetite 81:193–199CrossRef Barends C, de Vries JH, Mojet J, de Graaf C (2014) Effects of starting weaning exclusively with vegetables on vegetable intake at the age of 12 and 23 months. Appetite 81:193–199CrossRef
133.
Zurück zum Zitat Ahern SM, Caton SJ, Blundell P, Hetherington MM (2014) The root of the problem: increasing root vegetable intake in preschool children by repeated exposure and flavour learning. Appetite 80:154–160CrossRef Ahern SM, Caton SJ, Blundell P, Hetherington MM (2014) The root of the problem: increasing root vegetable intake in preschool children by repeated exposure and flavour learning. Appetite 80:154–160CrossRef
134.
Zurück zum Zitat Hausner H, Olsen A, Møller P (2012) Mere exposure and flavour–flavour learning increase 2–3 year-old children’s acceptance of a novel vegetable. Appetite 58:1152–1159CrossRef Hausner H, Olsen A, Møller P (2012) Mere exposure and flavour–flavour learning increase 2–3 year-old children’s acceptance of a novel vegetable. Appetite 58:1152–1159CrossRef
135.
Zurück zum Zitat Bouhlal S, Issanchou S, Chabanet C et al (2014) ‘Just a pinch of salt’. An experimental comparison of the effect of repeated exposure and flavor–flavor learning with salt or spice on vegetable acceptance in toddlers. Appetite 83:209–217CrossRef Bouhlal S, Issanchou S, Chabanet C et al (2014) ‘Just a pinch of salt’. An experimental comparison of the effect of repeated exposure and flavor–flavor learning with salt or spice on vegetable acceptance in toddlers. Appetite 83:209–217CrossRef
136.
Zurück zum Zitat de Wild VW, de Graaf C, Jager G (2013) Effectiveness of flavour nutrient learning and mere exposure as mechanisms to increase toddler’s intake and preference for green vegetables. Appetite 64:89–96CrossRef de Wild VW, de Graaf C, Jager G (2013) Effectiveness of flavour nutrient learning and mere exposure as mechanisms to increase toddler’s intake and preference for green vegetables. Appetite 64:89–96CrossRef
137.
Zurück zum Zitat Holley CE, Haycraft E, Farrow C (2015) ‘Why don’t you try it again?’ A comparison of parent led, home based interventions aimed at increasing children’s consumption of a disliked vegetable. Appetite 87:215–222CrossRef Holley CE, Haycraft E, Farrow C (2015) ‘Why don’t you try it again?’ A comparison of parent led, home based interventions aimed at increasing children’s consumption of a disliked vegetable. Appetite 87:215–222CrossRef
138.
Zurück zum Zitat Fildes A, van Jaarsveld CH, Wardle J, Cooke L (2014) Parent-administered exposure to increase children’s vegetable acceptance: a randomized controlled trial. JAND 114:881–888 Fildes A, van Jaarsveld CH, Wardle J, Cooke L (2014) Parent-administered exposure to increase children’s vegetable acceptance: a randomized controlled trial. JAND 114:881–888
139.
Zurück zum Zitat Anzman-Frasca S, Savage JS, Marini ME, Fisher JO, Birch LL (2012) Repeated exposure and associative conditioning promote preschool children’s liking of vegetables. Appetite 58:543–553CrossRef Anzman-Frasca S, Savage JS, Marini ME, Fisher JO, Birch LL (2012) Repeated exposure and associative conditioning promote preschool children’s liking of vegetables. Appetite 58:543–553CrossRef
140.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Connell ML, Henderson KE, Luedicke J, Schwartz MB (2012) Repeated exposure in a natural setting: a preschool intervention to increase vegetable consumption. JAND 112:230–234 O’Connell ML, Henderson KE, Luedicke J, Schwartz MB (2012) Repeated exposure in a natural setting: a preschool intervention to increase vegetable consumption. JAND 112:230–234
141.
Zurück zum Zitat Correia DC, O’Connell M, Irwin ML, Henderson KE (2014) Pairing vegetables with a liked food and visually appealing presentation: promising strategies for increasing vegetable consumption among preschoolers. Child Obes 10:72–76 Correia DC, O’Connell M, Irwin ML, Henderson KE (2014) Pairing vegetables with a liked food and visually appealing presentation: promising strategies for increasing vegetable consumption among preschoolers. Child Obes 10:72–76
142.
Zurück zum Zitat Fisher JO, Mennella JA, Hughes SO, Liu Y, Mendoza PM, Patrick H (2012) Offering “dip” promotes intake of a moderately-liked raw vegetable among preschoolers with genetic sensitivity to bitterness. JAND. 112:235–245 Fisher JO, Mennella JA, Hughes SO, Liu Y, Mendoza PM, Patrick H (2012) Offering “dip” promotes intake of a moderately-liked raw vegetable among preschoolers with genetic sensitivity to bitterness. JAND. 112:235–245
143.
Zurück zum Zitat Havermans RC, Jansen A (2007) Increasing children’s liking of vegetables through flavour–flavour learning. Appetite 48:259–262CrossRef Havermans RC, Jansen A (2007) Increasing children’s liking of vegetables through flavour–flavour learning. Appetite 48:259–262CrossRef
144.
Zurück zum Zitat Hendy HM, Williams KE, Camise TS (2005) “Kids Choice” school lunch program increases children’s fruit and vegetable acceptance. Appetite 45:250–263CrossRef Hendy HM, Williams KE, Camise TS (2005) “Kids Choice” school lunch program increases children’s fruit and vegetable acceptance. Appetite 45:250–263CrossRef
145.
Zurück zum Zitat Cooke LJ, Chambers LC, Añez EV et al (2011) Eating for pleasure or profit: the effect of incentives on children’s enjoyment of vegetables. Psychol Sci 22:190–196CrossRef Cooke LJ, Chambers LC, Añez EV et al (2011) Eating for pleasure or profit: the effect of incentives on children’s enjoyment of vegetables. Psychol Sci 22:190–196CrossRef
146.
Zurück zum Zitat Corsini N, Slater A, Harrison A, Cooke L, Cox DN (2013) Rewards can be used effectively with repeated exposure to increase liking of vegetables in 4–6-year-old children. Pub Health Nutr 16:942–951CrossRef Corsini N, Slater A, Harrison A, Cooke L, Cox DN (2013) Rewards can be used effectively with repeated exposure to increase liking of vegetables in 4–6-year-old children. Pub Health Nutr 16:942–951CrossRef
147.
Zurück zum Zitat Noradilah MJ, Zahara AM (2012) Acceptance of a test vegetable after repeated exposures among preschoolers. Malays J Nutr 18:67–75 Noradilah MJ, Zahara AM (2012) Acceptance of a test vegetable after repeated exposures among preschoolers. Malays J Nutr 18:67–75
148.
Zurück zum Zitat Wardle J, Herrera ML, Cooke L, Gibson EL (2003) Modifying children’s food preferences: the effects of exposure and reward on acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable. Eur J Clin Nutr 57:341–348CrossRef Wardle J, Herrera ML, Cooke L, Gibson EL (2003) Modifying children’s food preferences: the effects of exposure and reward on acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable. Eur J Clin Nutr 57:341–348CrossRef
149.
Zurück zum Zitat Lakkakula A, Geaghan J, Zanovec M, Pierce S, Tuuri G (2010) Repeated taste exposure increases liking for vegetables by low-income elementary school children. Appetite 55:226–231CrossRef Lakkakula A, Geaghan J, Zanovec M, Pierce S, Tuuri G (2010) Repeated taste exposure increases liking for vegetables by low-income elementary school children. Appetite 55:226–231CrossRef
150.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnston CA, Palcic JL, Tyler C, Stansberry S, Reeves RS, Foreyt JP (2011) Increasing vegetable intake in Mexican-American youth: a randomized controlled trial. JADA 111:716–720CrossRef Johnston CA, Palcic JL, Tyler C, Stansberry S, Reeves RS, Foreyt JP (2011) Increasing vegetable intake in Mexican-American youth: a randomized controlled trial. JADA 111:716–720CrossRef
151.
Zurück zum Zitat Zeinstra GG, Koelen MA, Kok FJ, de Graaf C (2009) Children’s hard-wired aversion to pure vegetable tastes. A ‘failed’ flavour–nutrient learning study. Appetite 52:528–530CrossRef Zeinstra GG, Koelen MA, Kok FJ, de Graaf C (2009) Children’s hard-wired aversion to pure vegetable tastes. A ‘failed’ flavour–nutrient learning study. Appetite 52:528–530CrossRef
152.
Zurück zum Zitat Olsen A, Ritz C, Kraaij LW, Møller P (2012) Children’s liking and intake of vegetables: a school-based intervention study. Food Qual Pref 23:90–98CrossRef Olsen A, Ritz C, Kraaij LW, Møller P (2012) Children’s liking and intake of vegetables: a school-based intervention study. Food Qual Pref 23:90–98CrossRef
153.
Zurück zum Zitat Heath P, Houston-Price C, Kennedy OB (2014) Let’s look at leeks! Picture books increase toddlers’ willingness to look at, taste and consume unfamiliar vegetables. Front Psychol 5:191CrossRef Heath P, Houston-Price C, Kennedy OB (2014) Let’s look at leeks! Picture books increase toddlers’ willingness to look at, taste and consume unfamiliar vegetables. Front Psychol 5:191CrossRef
154.
Zurück zum Zitat Byrne E, Nitzke S (2002) Preschool children’s acceptance of a novel vegetable following exposure to messages in a storybook. J Nutr Educ Behav 34:211–213CrossRef Byrne E, Nitzke S (2002) Preschool children’s acceptance of a novel vegetable following exposure to messages in a storybook. J Nutr Educ Behav 34:211–213CrossRef
155.
Zurück zum Zitat de Droog SM, Buijzen M, Valkenburg PM (2014) Enhancing children’s vegetable consumption using vegetable-promoting picture books. The impact of interactive shared reading and character-product congruence. Appetite 73:73–80CrossRef de Droog SM, Buijzen M, Valkenburg PM (2014) Enhancing children’s vegetable consumption using vegetable-promoting picture books. The impact of interactive shared reading and character-product congruence. Appetite 73:73–80CrossRef
156.
Zurück zum Zitat Coulthard H, Harris G, Fogel A (2014) Exposure to vegetable variety in infants weaned at different ages. Appetite 78:89–94CrossRef Coulthard H, Harris G, Fogel A (2014) Exposure to vegetable variety in infants weaned at different ages. Appetite 78:89–94CrossRef
157.
Zurück zum Zitat Maier AS, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Leathwood PD, Issanchou SN (2008) Breastfeeding and experience with variety early in weaning increase infants’ acceptance of new foods for up to two months. Clin Nutr. 27:849–857CrossRef Maier AS, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Leathwood PD, Issanchou SN (2008) Breastfeeding and experience with variety early in weaning increase infants’ acceptance of new foods for up to two months. Clin Nutr. 27:849–857CrossRef
158.
Zurück zum Zitat de Wild VW, de Graaf C, Boshuizen HC, Jager G (2015) Influence of choice on vegetable intake in children: an in-home study. Appetite 91:1–6CrossRef de Wild VW, de Graaf C, Boshuizen HC, Jager G (2015) Influence of choice on vegetable intake in children: an in-home study. Appetite 91:1–6CrossRef
159.
Zurück zum Zitat Spill MK, Birch LL, Roe LS, Rolls BJ (2010) Eating vegetables first: the use of portion size to increase vegetable intake in preschool children. Am J Clin Nutr 91:1237–1243CrossRef Spill MK, Birch LL, Roe LS, Rolls BJ (2010) Eating vegetables first: the use of portion size to increase vegetable intake in preschool children. Am J Clin Nutr 91:1237–1243CrossRef
160.
Zurück zum Zitat Spill MK, Birch LL, Roe LS, Rolls BJ (2011) Serving large portions of vegetable soup at the start of a meal affected children’s energy and vegetable intake. Appetite 57:213–219CrossRef Spill MK, Birch LL, Roe LS, Rolls BJ (2011) Serving large portions of vegetable soup at the start of a meal affected children’s energy and vegetable intake. Appetite 57:213–219CrossRef
161.
Zurück zum Zitat Spill MK, Birch LL, Roe LS, Rolls BJ (2011) Hiding vegetables to reduce energy density: an effective strategy to increase children’s vegetable intake and reduce energy intake. Am J Clin Nutr 94:735–741CrossRef Spill MK, Birch LL, Roe LS, Rolls BJ (2011) Hiding vegetables to reduce energy density: an effective strategy to increase children’s vegetable intake and reduce energy intake. Am J Clin Nutr 94:735–741CrossRef
162.
Zurück zum Zitat Mathias KC, Rolls BJ, Birch LL et al (2012) Serving larger portions of fruits and vegetables together at dinner promotes intake of both foods among young children. JAND 112:266–270 Mathias KC, Rolls BJ, Birch LL et al (2012) Serving larger portions of fruits and vegetables together at dinner promotes intake of both foods among young children. JAND 112:266–270
163.
Zurück zum Zitat Bucher T, Siegrist M, van der Horst K (2014) Vegetable variety: an effective strategy to increase vegetable choice in children. Pub Health Nutr 17:1232–1236CrossRef Bucher T, Siegrist M, van der Horst K (2014) Vegetable variety: an effective strategy to increase vegetable choice in children. Pub Health Nutr 17:1232–1236CrossRef
164.
Zurück zum Zitat Wansink B, Just DR, Payne CR, Klinger MZ (2012) Attractive names sustain increased vegetable intake in schools. Prev Med 55:330–332CrossRef Wansink B, Just DR, Payne CR, Klinger MZ (2012) Attractive names sustain increased vegetable intake in schools. Prev Med 55:330–332CrossRef
165.
Zurück zum Zitat Just DR, Wansink B (2009) Smarter lunchrooms: using behavioural economics to improve meal selection. Choices Mag Food Farm Resour Issues 24:19–24 Just DR, Wansink B (2009) Smarter lunchrooms: using behavioural economics to improve meal selection. Choices Mag Food Farm Resour Issues 24:19–24
166.
Zurück zum Zitat Redden JP, Mann T, Vickers Z, Mykerezi E, Reicks M, Elsbernd S (2015) Serving first in isolation increases vegetable intake among elementary school children. PLoS One 10:e0121283CrossRef Redden JP, Mann T, Vickers Z, Mykerezi E, Reicks M, Elsbernd S (2015) Serving first in isolation increases vegetable intake among elementary school children. PLoS One 10:e0121283CrossRef
167.
Zurück zum Zitat Reicks M, Redden JP, Mann T, Mykerezi E, Vickers Z (2012) Photographs in lunch tray compartments and vegetable consumption among children in elementary school cafeterias. JAMA 307:784–785CrossRef Reicks M, Redden JP, Mann T, Mykerezi E, Vickers Z (2012) Photographs in lunch tray compartments and vegetable consumption among children in elementary school cafeterias. JAMA 307:784–785CrossRef
168.
Zurück zum Zitat Bucher T, van der Horst K, Siegrist M (2011) Improvement of meal composition by vegetable variety. Public Health Nutr 14:1357–1363CrossRef Bucher T, van der Horst K, Siegrist M (2011) Improvement of meal composition by vegetable variety. Public Health Nutr 14:1357–1363CrossRef
169.
Zurück zum Zitat Blatt AD, Roe LS, Rolls BJ (2011) Hidden vegetables: an effective strategy to reduce energy intake and increase vegetable intake in adults. Am J Clin Nutr 93:756–763CrossRef Blatt AD, Roe LS, Rolls BJ (2011) Hidden vegetables: an effective strategy to reduce energy intake and increase vegetable intake in adults. Am J Clin Nutr 93:756–763CrossRef
170.
Zurück zum Zitat Meengs JS, Roe LS, Rolls BJ (2012) Vegetable variety: an effective strategy to increase vegetable intake in adults. JAND 112:1211–1215 Meengs JS, Roe LS, Rolls BJ (2012) Vegetable variety: an effective strategy to increase vegetable intake in adults. JAND 112:1211–1215
171.
Zurück zum Zitat Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS (2010) Portion size can be used strategically to increase vegetable consumption in adults. Am J Clin Nutr 91:913–922CrossRef Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS (2010) Portion size can be used strategically to increase vegetable consumption in adults. Am J Clin Nutr 91:913–922CrossRef
172.
Zurück zum Zitat Shenoy SF, Kazaks AG, Holt RR et al (2010) The use of a commercial vegetable juice as a practical means to increase vegetable intake: a randomized controlled trial. Nutr J 9:38CrossRef Shenoy SF, Kazaks AG, Holt RR et al (2010) The use of a commercial vegetable juice as a practical means to increase vegetable intake: a randomized controlled trial. Nutr J 9:38CrossRef
173.
Zurück zum Zitat Shenoy SF, Poston WS, Reeves RS et al (2010) Weight loss in individuals with metabolic syndrome given DASH diet counseling when provided a low sodium vegetable juice: a randomized controlled trial. Nutr J 9:8CrossRef Shenoy SF, Poston WS, Reeves RS et al (2010) Weight loss in individuals with metabolic syndrome given DASH diet counseling when provided a low sodium vegetable juice: a randomized controlled trial. Nutr J 9:8CrossRef
174.
Zurück zum Zitat Zeinstra GG, Renes RJ, Koelen MA, Kok FJ, de Graaf C (2010) Offering choice and its effects on Dutch children’s liking and consumption of vegetables: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 91:349–356CrossRef Zeinstra GG, Renes RJ, Koelen MA, Kok FJ, de Graaf C (2010) Offering choice and its effects on Dutch children’s liking and consumption of vegetables: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 91:349–356CrossRef
175.
Zurück zum Zitat Domínguez PR, Gámiz F, Gil M et al (2013) Providing choice increases children’s vegetable intake. Food Qual Pref 30:108–113CrossRef Domínguez PR, Gámiz F, Gil M et al (2013) Providing choice increases children’s vegetable intake. Food Qual Pref 30:108–113CrossRef
176.
Zurück zum Zitat Gholami M, Wiedemann A, Knoll N, Schwarzer R (2015) Mothers improve their daughters’ vegetable intake: a randomized controlled trial. Psychol Health Med 20:1–7CrossRef Gholami M, Wiedemann A, Knoll N, Schwarzer R (2015) Mothers improve their daughters’ vegetable intake: a randomized controlled trial. Psychol Health Med 20:1–7CrossRef
177.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris JL, Zidenberg-Cherr S (2002) Garden-enhanced nutrition curriculum improves fourth-grade school children’s knowledge of nutrition and preferences for some vegetables. JADA 102:91–93CrossRef Morris JL, Zidenberg-Cherr S (2002) Garden-enhanced nutrition curriculum improves fourth-grade school children’s knowledge of nutrition and preferences for some vegetables. JADA 102:91–93CrossRef
178.
Zurück zum Zitat Morgan PJ, Warren JM, Lubans DR, Saunders KL, Quick GI, Collins CE (2010) The impact of nutrition education with and without a school garden on knowledge, vegetable intake and preferences and quality of school life among primary-school students. Pub Health Nutr 13:1931–1940CrossRef Morgan PJ, Warren JM, Lubans DR, Saunders KL, Quick GI, Collins CE (2010) The impact of nutrition education with and without a school garden on knowledge, vegetable intake and preferences and quality of school life among primary-school students. Pub Health Nutr 13:1931–1940CrossRef
179.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson E, Blissett J, Higgs S (2011) Recall of vegetable eating affects future predicted enjoyment and choice of vegetables in British University undergraduate students. JADA 111:1543–1548CrossRef Robinson E, Blissett J, Higgs S (2011) Recall of vegetable eating affects future predicted enjoyment and choice of vegetables in British University undergraduate students. JADA 111:1543–1548CrossRef
180.
Zurück zum Zitat Stok FM, Verkooijen KT, de Ridder DTD, de Wit JBF, de Vet E (2014) How norms work: self-identification, attitude, and self-efficacy mediate the relation between descriptive social norms and vegetable intake. Appl Psychol Health Well-Being 6:230–250CrossRef Stok FM, Verkooijen KT, de Ridder DTD, de Wit JBF, de Vet E (2014) How norms work: self-identification, attitude, and self-efficacy mediate the relation between descriptive social norms and vegetable intake. Appl Psychol Health Well-Being 6:230–250CrossRef
181.
Zurück zum Zitat Ogawa Y, Tanabe N, Honda A et al (2011) Point-of-purchase health information encourages customers to purchase vegetables: objective analysis by using a point-of-sales system. Environ Health Prev Med 16:239–246CrossRef Ogawa Y, Tanabe N, Honda A et al (2011) Point-of-purchase health information encourages customers to purchase vegetables: objective analysis by using a point-of-sales system. Environ Health Prev Med 16:239–246CrossRef
182.
Zurück zum Zitat Rahman MM, Islam MA, Mahalanabis D, Chowdhury S, Biswas E (1994) Impact of health education on the feeding of green leafy vegetables at home to children of the urban poor mothers of Bangladesh. Pub Health 108:211–218CrossRef Rahman MM, Islam MA, Mahalanabis D, Chowdhury S, Biswas E (1994) Impact of health education on the feeding of green leafy vegetables at home to children of the urban poor mothers of Bangladesh. Pub Health 108:211–218CrossRef
183.
Zurück zum Zitat Tabak RG, Tate DF, Stevens J, Siega-Riz AM, Ward DS (2012) Family ties to health program: a randomized intervention to improve vegetable intake in children. J Nutr Educ Behav 44:166–171CrossRef Tabak RG, Tate DF, Stevens J, Siega-Riz AM, Ward DS (2012) Family ties to health program: a randomized intervention to improve vegetable intake in children. J Nutr Educ Behav 44:166–171CrossRef
184.
Zurück zum Zitat Clarke P, Evans SH, Hovy EH (2011) Indigenous message tailoring increases consumption of fresh vegetables by clients of community pantries. Health Commun 26:571–582CrossRef Clarke P, Evans SH, Hovy EH (2011) Indigenous message tailoring increases consumption of fresh vegetables by clients of community pantries. Health Commun 26:571–582CrossRef
185.
Zurück zum Zitat Faber M, Phungula MA, Venter SL, Dhansay MA, Benadé AJ (2002) Home gardens focusing on the production of yellow and dark-green leafy vegetables increase the serum retinol concentrations of 2–5-y-old children in South Africa. Am J Clin Nutr 76:1048–1054 Faber M, Phungula MA, Venter SL, Dhansay MA, Benadé AJ (2002) Home gardens focusing on the production of yellow and dark-green leafy vegetables increase the serum retinol concentrations of 2–5-y-old children in South Africa. Am J Clin Nutr 76:1048–1054
186.
Zurück zum Zitat Bai Y, Suriano L, Wunderlich SM (2014) Veggiecation: a novel approach to improve vegetable consumption among school-aged children. J Nutr Educ Behav 46:320–321CrossRef Bai Y, Suriano L, Wunderlich SM (2014) Veggiecation: a novel approach to improve vegetable consumption among school-aged children. J Nutr Educ Behav 46:320–321CrossRef
187.
Zurück zum Zitat Leak TM, Swenson A, Vickers Z et al (2015) Testing the effectiveness of in-home behavioral economics strategies to increase vegetable intake, liking, and variety among children residing in households that receive food assistance. J Nutr Educ Behav 47:E1–E9CrossRef Leak TM, Swenson A, Vickers Z et al (2015) Testing the effectiveness of in-home behavioral economics strategies to increase vegetable intake, liking, and variety among children residing in households that receive food assistance. J Nutr Educ Behav 47:E1–E9CrossRef
188.
Zurück zum Zitat Wright W, Rowell L (2010) Examining the effect of gardening on vegetable consumption among youth in kindergarten through fifth grade. WMJ 109:125–129 Wright W, Rowell L (2010) Examining the effect of gardening on vegetable consumption among youth in kindergarten through fifth grade. WMJ 109:125–129
189.
Zurück zum Zitat Ratcliffe MM, Merrigan KA, Rogers BL, Goldberg JP (2011) The effects of school garden experiences on middle school-aged students’ knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors associated with vegetable consumption. Health Promot Pract 12:36–43CrossRef Ratcliffe MM, Merrigan KA, Rogers BL, Goldberg JP (2011) The effects of school garden experiences on middle school-aged students’ knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors associated with vegetable consumption. Health Promot Pract 12:36–43CrossRef
190.
Zurück zum Zitat Carney PA, Hamada JL, Rdesinski R et al (2012) Impact of a community gardening project on vegetable intake, food security and family relationships: a community-based participatory research study. J Community Health 37:874–881CrossRef Carney PA, Hamada JL, Rdesinski R et al (2012) Impact of a community gardening project on vegetable intake, food security and family relationships: a community-based participatory research study. J Community Health 37:874–881CrossRef
191.
Zurück zum Zitat Schreinemachers P, Patalagsa MA, Islam MR et al (2015) The effect of women’s home gardens on vegetable production and consumption in Bangladesh. Food Secur 7:97–107CrossRef Schreinemachers P, Patalagsa MA, Islam MR et al (2015) The effect of women’s home gardens on vegetable production and consumption in Bangladesh. Food Secur 7:97–107CrossRef
192.
Zurück zum Zitat Kushida O, Murayama N (2014) Effects of environmental intervention in workplace cafeterias on vegetable consumption by male workers. J Nutr Educ Behav 46:350–358CrossRef Kushida O, Murayama N (2014) Effects of environmental intervention in workplace cafeterias on vegetable consumption by male workers. J Nutr Educ Behav 46:350–358CrossRef
193.
Zurück zum Zitat Weatherly L, Weatherly K (1990) Containerized vegetable gardening for homebound patients. Caring 9:52–54 Weatherly L, Weatherly K (1990) Containerized vegetable gardening for homebound patients. Caring 9:52–54
194.
Zurück zum Zitat Mooney E, Farley H, Strugnell C (2004) Dieting among adolescent females—some emerging trends. Int J Cons Stud 28:347–354CrossRef Mooney E, Farley H, Strugnell C (2004) Dieting among adolescent females—some emerging trends. Int J Cons Stud 28:347–354CrossRef
195.
Zurück zum Zitat Appleton KM, McGill R, Neville C, Woodside JV (2010) Barriers to increasing fruit and vegetable intakes in the older population of Northern Ireland: low levels of liking and low awareness of current recommendations. Pub Health Nutr 13:514–521CrossRef Appleton KM, McGill R, Neville C, Woodside JV (2010) Barriers to increasing fruit and vegetable intakes in the older population of Northern Ireland: low levels of liking and low awareness of current recommendations. Pub Health Nutr 13:514–521CrossRef
196.
Zurück zum Zitat Appleton KM, McGill R, Woodside JV (2009) Fruit and vegetable consumption in older people in Northern Ireland: levels and patterns. Br J Nutr 102:949–953CrossRef Appleton KM, McGill R, Woodside JV (2009) Fruit and vegetable consumption in older people in Northern Ireland: levels and patterns. Br J Nutr 102:949–953CrossRef
197.
Zurück zum Zitat Best RL, Appleton KM (2013) Investigating protein consumption in older adults: a focus group study. J Nutr Educ Behav 45:751–755CrossRef Best RL, Appleton KM (2013) Investigating protein consumption in older adults: a focus group study. J Nutr Educ Behav 45:751–755CrossRef
198.
Zurück zum Zitat Giskes K, Avendan M, Brug J, Kunst AE (2009) A systematic review of studies on socioeconomic inequalities in dietary intakes associated with weight gain and overweight/obesity conducted among European adults. Obes Rev 658:413–429CrossRef Giskes K, Avendan M, Brug J, Kunst AE (2009) A systematic review of studies on socioeconomic inequalities in dietary intakes associated with weight gain and overweight/obesity conducted among European adults. Obes Rev 658:413–429CrossRef
199.
Zurück zum Zitat AbuSabha R, Namjoshi D, Klein A (2013) Increasing access and affordability of produce improves perceived consumption of vegetables in low-income seniors. JADA 111:1549–1555CrossRef AbuSabha R, Namjoshi D, Klein A (2013) Increasing access and affordability of produce improves perceived consumption of vegetables in low-income seniors. JADA 111:1549–1555CrossRef
200.
Zurück zum Zitat Agudo A, Ibanez R, Amiano P et al (2008) Consumption of cruciferous vegetables and glucosinolates in a Spanish adult population. Eur J Clin Nutr 62:324–331CrossRef Agudo A, Ibanez R, Amiano P et al (2008) Consumption of cruciferous vegetables and glucosinolates in a Spanish adult population. Eur J Clin Nutr 62:324–331CrossRef
201.
Zurück zum Zitat Galobardes BC, Irala-Estevez JD, Groth M et al (2000) A systematic review of socioeconomic differences in food habits in Europe: consumption of fruit and vegetables. Eur J Clin Nutr 54:706–714CrossRef Galobardes BC, Irala-Estevez JD, Groth M et al (2000) A systematic review of socioeconomic differences in food habits in Europe: consumption of fruit and vegetables. Eur J Clin Nutr 54:706–714CrossRef
202.
Zurück zum Zitat Roos G, Johansson L, Kasmel A, Klumbiene J, Prattala R (2001) Disparities in vegetable and fruit consumption: European cases from the north to the south. Pub Health Nutr 4:35–43CrossRef Roos G, Johansson L, Kasmel A, Klumbiene J, Prattala R (2001) Disparities in vegetable and fruit consumption: European cases from the north to the south. Pub Health Nutr 4:35–43CrossRef
203.
Zurück zum Zitat Tohill BC, Seymour J, Serdula M, Kettel-Khan L, Rolls BJ (2004) What epidemiologic studies tell us about the relationship between fruit and vegetable consumption and body weight. Nutr Rev 62:365–374CrossRef Tohill BC, Seymour J, Serdula M, Kettel-Khan L, Rolls BJ (2004) What epidemiologic studies tell us about the relationship between fruit and vegetable consumption and body weight. Nutr Rev 62:365–374CrossRef
204.
Zurück zum Zitat Appleton KM (2013) Increases in fruit intakes in low consumers of fruit following two community-based repeated exposure interventions. Br J Nutr 109:795–801CrossRef Appleton KM (2013) Increases in fruit intakes in low consumers of fruit following two community-based repeated exposure interventions. Br J Nutr 109:795–801CrossRef
205.
Zurück zum Zitat Curhan RC (1974) The effects of merchandising and temporary promotional activities on the sales of fresh fruits and vegetables in supermarkets. J Marketing Res 11:286–294CrossRef Curhan RC (1974) The effects of merchandising and temporary promotional activities on the sales of fresh fruits and vegetables in supermarkets. J Marketing Res 11:286–294CrossRef
206.
Zurück zum Zitat Glanz K, Yaroch AL (2004) Strategies for increasing fruit and vegetable intake in grocery stores and communities: policy, pricing, and environmental change. Prev Med 39 suppl 2:S75–S80CrossRef Glanz K, Yaroch AL (2004) Strategies for increasing fruit and vegetable intake in grocery stores and communities: policy, pricing, and environmental change. Prev Med 39 suppl 2:S75–S80CrossRef
207.
Zurück zum Zitat Pérez-Cueto FJ, Aschemann-Witzel J, Shankar B et al (2012) Assessment of evaluations made to healthy eating policies in Europe: a review within the EATWELL Project. Pub Health Nutr 15:1489–1496CrossRef Pérez-Cueto FJ, Aschemann-Witzel J, Shankar B et al (2012) Assessment of evaluations made to healthy eating policies in Europe: a review within the EATWELL Project. Pub Health Nutr 15:1489–1496CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Increasing vegetable intakes: rationale and systematic review of published interventions
verfasst von
Katherine M. Appleton
Ann Hemingway
Laure Saulais
Caterina Dinnella
Erminio Monteleone
Laurence Depezay
David Morizet
F. J. Armando Perez-Cueto
Ann Bevan
Heather Hartwell
Publikationsdatum
11.01.2016
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Journal of Nutrition / Ausgabe 3/2016
Print ISSN: 1436-6207
Elektronische ISSN: 1436-6215
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00394-015-1130-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2016

European Journal of Nutrition 3/2016 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Innere Medizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Innere Medizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.