Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 9/2015

Open Access 01.09.2015 | Original Contribution

Where are the strongest associations between autistic traits and traits of ADHD? evidence from a community-based twin study

verfasst von: Mark J. Taylor, Tony Charman, Angelica Ronald

Erschienen in: European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry | Ausgabe 9/2015

Abstract

Autism spectrum conditions (ASC) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) regularly co-occur. Twin studies increasingly indicate that these conditions may have overlapping genetic causes. Less is known about the degree to which specific autistic traits relate to specific behaviours characteristic of ADHD. We hence tested, using the classical twin design, whether specific dimensional autistic traits, including social difficulties, communication atypicalities and repetitive behaviours, would display differential degrees of aetiological overlap with specific traits of ADHD, including hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention. Parents of approximately 4,000 pairs of 12-year-old twins completed the Childhood Autism Spectrum Test and Conners’ Parent Rating Scale. These measures were divided into subscales corresponding to different types of autistic and ADHD behaviours. Twin model fitting suggested that the degree of genetic overlap was particularly strong between communication difficulties and traits of ADHD (genetic correlations = .47−.51), while repetitive behaviours and social difficulties showed moderate (genetic correlations = .12−.33) and modest (.05−.11) genetic overlap respectively. Environmental overlap was low across all subscales (correlations = .01−.23). These patterns were also apparent at the extremes of the general population, with communication difficulties showing the highest genetic overlap with traits of ADHD. These findings indicate that molecular genetic studies seeking to uncover the shared genetic basis of ASC and ADHD would benefit from taking a symptom-specific approach. Furthermore, they could also help to explain why studies of the communication abilities of individuals with ASC and ADHD have produced overlapping findings.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1007/​s00787-014-0666-0) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Introduction

Based on their diagnostic criteria, autism spectrum conditions (ASC) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have little in common. ASC are characterised by atypical social interaction, communication difficulties, and behavioural inflexibility, while ADHD is defined by excessive hyperactivity, impulsivity, and/or inattentiveness [1]. Yet it is common for these conditions to co-occur; ADHD is thought to present in 30–60 % of individuals with ASC [2, 3], while autistic traits appear elevated in individuals with ADHD [4]. It is thus necessary for research to explore why these conditions so often co-occur with one another.
ASC and ADHD are highly heritable [5, 6]. One possible explanation for their co-occurrence could be that their genetic and environmental causes overlap with one another. This assertion has been supported by a multitude of recent twin studies. These studies have reported a moderate degree of genetic overlap between ASC and ADHD, both as dimensional characteristic traits and diagnosed conditions, in middle childhood [7], early adolescence [810], early adulthood [11], and adulthood [10]. These studies also report a modest [79, 11] to moderate [10] degree of environmental overlap across these traits.
There is converging evidence from family and twin studies that the core autistic traits, social and communication difficulties and restricted repetitive behaviours, have largely different causes [1216]; that is, certain genes or environments may be associated with social difficulties, for example, but may not cause communication difficulties or rigid behavioural patterns. There is also evidence of some genetic specificity across traits of ADHD [1719]. As such, it is possible that the degree of aetiological overlap between autistic traits and ADHD behaviours differs across particular types of these traits. Two recent twin studies tested this possibility. A Dutch study reported that attention switching difficulties, rather than social autistic traits, showed particularly strong genetic overlap with traits of ADHD [20]. A study of a Swedish cohort found that repetitive routines and behaviours showed elevated genetic overlap with traits of ADHD [21]. Together, these studies indicate that traits of ADHD are particularly associated with non-social autistic traits.
There have yet to be any twin studies of these associations in extreme-scoring groups. Individuals displaying extreme scores on trait measures typically display scores that are more comparable to clinical groups, thus indicating whether or not we would expect trait findings to extend to clinical groups. For this reason, we conducted a twin study aimed at testing the degree of genetic overlap between specific autistic traits, including social difficulties, communication atypicalities, and repetitive routines and behaviours, and specific traits of ADHD, including hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention, at the extremes of the general population. We also further tested genetic and environmental overlap between these traits across the general population. We hypothesised that the degree of aetiological overlap between autistic traits and traits of ADHD would differ according to different types of autistic and ADHD trait. We further expected repetitive routines and behaviours to show stronger aetiological overlap with traits of ADHD than social and communicative autistic traits.

Method

Participants

Parents of 5,879 twins participating in the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) completed and returned questionnaires when twins were aged 12. TEDS is a community-based, population representative study of twins born in England and Wales between 1994 and 1996 [22]. Exclusions were conducted for genetic syndromes, including Fragile X and cystic fibrosis, chromosomal abnormalities, including Down Syndrome and cerebral palsy, extreme perinatal or prenatal difficulties, and missing first contact or zygosity data. Participants with confirmed ASC (N = 80) were included. The final sample comprised 5,689 twin pairs, including 927 monozygotic (MZ) male pairs, 1,124 MZ female pairs, 865 dizygotic (DZ) male pairs, 989 DZ female pairs, and 1,784 DZ opposite-sex pairs. Zygosity was ascertained through DNA testing and parent ratings of twin resemblance [23]. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation. TEDS has full ethical approval from the King’s College London Research Ethics Committee.

Measures

Autistic traits

Parents completed the Childhood Autism Spectrum Test (CAST [24]), a 30-item questionnaire that enquires about various autistic traits, such as lack of social interest, pronounced interests, and difficulties with conversation. Each question was answered ‘yes’ or ‘no’, meaning the maximum possible score was 30. Scores over 15 predict ASC diagnoses with 100 % sensitivity and 97 % specificity [25]. The measure was divided into three scales based on DSM-IV criteria, which were employed in previous studies [13, 14]: social difficulties (‘Social’), communication atypicalities (‘Communication’), and repetitive, restricted behaviours and interests (RRBI). The number of items, possible range of scores, and Cronbach’s α values for each subscale are shown in Table 1.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics, phenotypic correlations, and twin correlations
Measure
Number of items
Cronbach’s α
Skewness
\(\overline{x}\) Full sample
(95 % CI)
\(\overline{x}\) 5 % extremes
(95 % CI)
Probands (N)
CAST social
11
0.54
1.42 (−0.01)
1.61 (1.58/1.64)
5.91 (5.80/6.01)
546
CAST communication
12
0.64
1.36 (0.02)
1.90 (1.87/1.93)
7.01 (6.91/7.12)
590
CAST RRBI
7
0.49
.96 (−0.05)
1.41 (1.39/1.44)
4.47 (4.42/4.57)
747
Conners ADHD hyperactivity/impulsivity
9
0.84
1.70 (−0.09)
4.30 (4.22/4.38)
16.85 (16.57/17.14)
656
Conners ADHD inattention
9
0.90
1.38 (−0.32)
5.58 (5.48/5.67)
19.89 (19.63/20.14)
632
Skewness values are first given for untransformed scales; the values in parentheses are for transformed, standardised residuals used in the analyses
CAST Childhood Autism Spectrum Test, CAST social CAST social difficulties subscale, CAST communication CAST communication atypicalities subscale; CAST RRBI CAST repetitive, restricted behaviours and interests subscale

Traits of ADHD

Parents completed the ADHD subscale of the Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (‘Conners ADHD’ [26] ), a measure formed of 18 statements. Each statement described a behaviour characteristic of ADHD; parents rated, on a three-point scale, how true each statement was of their child. The highest possible score was 54. Conners ADHD is a valid assessment of ADHD traits; individuals with ADHD display significantly higher scores on the measure than controls [26]. Consistent with previous recommendations [26], the measure was divided into two subscales, which closely corresponded to DSM-IV criteria for ADHD: Hyperactivity/Impulsivity and Inattention. Possible ranges of scores, number of items, and Cronbach’s α for both subscales are provided in Table 1.

Data analysis

Twin analyses of the full sample

Data were prepared for analysis by first log transforming any skewed subscales (see Table 1), and then regressing the effects of sex and age out of all measures, as is standard behavioural genetic procedure [27]. Analyses were then performed on standardised residual scores using Mx [28].
First, phenotypic associations between the CAST and Conners ADHD subscales were established using phenotypic correlations (r ph), correlation coefficients estimated from structural equation models. Second, twin correlations, which are the foundation of twin analysis, were estimated. Cross-trait cross-twin correlations provide one with an initial indication of the extent to which covariance between two traits is influenced by genetic and environmental factors. These involved correlating one twin’s score on a CAST subscale with their co-twin’s score on a Conners ADHD subscale. Cross-trait cross-twin correlations were estimated separately for MZ and DZ twins. These estimates cannot exceed r ph between two traits. As MZ twins are assumed to share all of their segregating DNA code with one another, while DZ twins share, on average, 50 %, additive genetic (A) influences on the covariance between two traits are implicated if the MZ cross-trait cross-twin correlation exceeds the DZ estimate. As MZ twins are assumed to be genetically identical, any within pair MZ differences are assumed to be caused by nonshared environment (E), environmental factors that differ across twins in a pair and create differences between them (measurement error is included in this term). Hence, if the MZ cross-trait cross-twin correlation between two traits is less than r ph between them, E is implicated on their covariance. Shared environmental (C) influences, on the other hand, are common to both twins in a pair and heighten their similarity; these influences are indicated if the DZ cross-trait cross-twin correlation exceeds half the MZ statistic. Alternatively, non-additive genetic (D) influences are implicated if the DZ cross-trait cross-twin correlation is less than half the MZ estimate. These correlations were estimated from a constrained saturated model (see below).
Finally, multivariate structural equation twin models were fitted to data to formally estimate A, C, D, and E. Cholesky decompositions, presented here as correlated factors solutions [29], were tested. For each trait, A, C (or D, if this is suggested by the twin correlations), and E were estimated. The additive genetic correlation (r A) then estimates A overlap between traits. This estimate falls between 0 and 1; estimates of 1 suggest total genetic overlap across two traits, while estimates of 0 would suggest that the genetic influences on two traits are totally independent of one another. Shared environmental (r C), nonshared environmental (r E), and non-additive genetic (r D) correlations can also be computed, and operate in the same manner. The model then computes bivariate heritability, which estimates the extent to which A can explain r ph between two traits, from the univariate A estimates and r A. Equivalent statistics are calculated for C, E, and D.
ACE and ADE models were tested. Parameters in these models can either be equated across sexes, or allowed to differ in magnitude across sexes (quantitative sex differences). Such differences are indicated when male and female twin correlations differ. In order to test for quantitative sex differences, a saturated model that allowed twin correlations to differ by sex was fitted. These estimates were then equated across sexes.
The fit of each model was compared against that of a saturated model of the observed data. For each model, the −2LL fit statistic was computed. Model fit was assessed using the likelihood-ratio test (LRT), which capitalises on the fact that differences in −2LL between two models are χ2 distributed, with degrees of freedom equal to the difference in number of estimated parameters. Significant χ2 results suggest that a given model is a significantly poorer fit than the saturated model. Model fit was further assessed using Akaike’s Information Criteria (AIC) and Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC). Lower, preferably negative, AIC values indicate good model fit, while models with more negative BIC estimates are to be favoured. Selection of the best-fitting model was based on BIC, which performs better than either the LRT or AIC in larger samples [30]; when comparing two models, the model with the more negative BIC estimate is regarded as better-fitting, with differences of 10 or more argued to show that a given model is a good fit [31].

Analysis of the extremes

DeFries–Fulker analysis [32, 33] was used to analyse extreme scores on the CAST and Conners ADHD subscales. DeFries–Fulker analysis is a regression analysis of means. The univariate procedure is designed to estimate group heritability (\(h^{ 2}_{\text{g}}\)), which refers to genetic influences on extreme scores [30]. Bivariate DeFries–Fulker analysis is concerned with testing for bivariate heritability (h 2.xy); in these analyses, this refers to the extent to which the genes that cause extreme scores on one trait influence continuous scores on another trait [32]. In performing univariate and bivariate DeFries–Fulker analyses, it is possible to estimate genetic correlations between extreme scores on two measures [33].
For the univariate DeFries–Fulker analyses, extreme scorers, termed probands, were selected from the sample used in the twin analyses on the basis of scoring within the highest 5 % of the z-score distribution on a measure of interest. Scores were then transformed so that the mean proband score on the measure of interest was 1, while the control mean was 0. In transforming scores in this manner, one can gain an indication of \(h^{ 2}_{\text{g}}\) by examining the mean scores of co-twins of probands. If the DZ co-twin mean is closer to 0 than the MZ co-twin mean, then greater resemblance between MZ twins than DZ twins is suggested. Much like how one interprets twin correlations, this can be taken as evidence of \(h^{2}_{\text{g}}\) on a trait. Univariate DeFries-Fulker analysis more formally estimates \(h^{2}_{\text{g}}\) through a regression equation for predicting co-twin scores on a given measure from proband scores and zygosity. The regression coefficient of co-twin scores on zygosity is an estimate of \(h^{2}_{\text{g}}\). The estimate of \(h^{2}_{\text{g}}\) should not exceed the mean scores of MZ co-twins, unless non-additive genetic effects are influential. In this instance, estimates were fixed to equal the mean MZ co-twin score.
In bivariate DeFries–Fulker analysis, probands were selected on the basis of scoring within the highest 5 % of the z-score distribution on a given measure; the measure used to select probands is the selection variable. The outcome variable is the second measure of interest. Thus, the bivariate procedure is directional in that a CAST subscale could be used as the selection variable and a Conners ADHD subscale used as the outcome variable, and vice versa.
Prior to conducting bivariate DeFries–Fulker analysis, the phenotypic associations between the selection and outcome variables were tested using phenotypic group correlations. These were calculated by dividing proband’s mean z-score on the selection variable by their mean z-score on the outcome variable, and can be interpreted in the same manner as correlation coefficients. Subsequently, data were prepared for bivariate analysis by dividing scores on both measures by the zygosity-specific proband mean for the selection variable [34]. Transforming scores in this manner meant that the mean score on the selection variable for probands was 1, while the mean score for controls on the outcome measure was 0. The co-twin means provide an indication of the extent of h 2.xy between two measures; if the DZ co-twin mean on the outcome variable is closer to 0 than MZ co-twin means, h 2.xy between the selection and outcome variables is indicated. The bivariate DeFries–Fulker regression equation, which predicts co-twin scores on the outcome measure from proband scores on the selection measure and zygosity, was then used to formally estimate h 2.xy. The regression coefficient of co-twin scores on zygosity is an estimate of h 2.xy. Again, this estimate only exceeds the mean scores on the outcome variable for MZ co-twins if non-additive genetic influences are in operation; as in the univariate analysis, h2.xy was fixed to equal the MZ co-twin mean for the outcome variable in these instances. These analyses were conducted using the CAST subscales as selection variables, and then using the Conners ADHD subscales as selection variables.
The results of the univariate and bivariate DeFries–Fulker analyses were then used to calculate genetic correlations between extreme CAST subscale scores and extreme Conners ADHD subscale scores, using the equation:\(\sqrt {\frac{{\left( {\beta_{xy} \times \beta_{yx} } \right)}}{{\left( {\beta_{x} \times \beta_{y} } \right)}}}\) [35]. β xy and β yx represent h 2.xy estimates, while β x and β y are \(h^{2}_{\text{g}}\) estimates for both measures.

Results

Full sample

Descriptive statistics for all measure subscales are shown in Table 1. Table 2 shows r ph estimates. The highest estimate was between Communication and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (.43), followed by Communication and Inattention (.40). RRBI displayed modest r ph with Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (.26) and Inattention (.05), while associations were weakest between Social and hyperactivity/impulsivity (.06) and Inattention (.07).
Table 2
Phenotypic correlations
Conners ADHD
CAST subscale
subscale
Conners ADHD hyperactivity/impulsivity
Conners ADHD inattention
CAST social
0.06*
0.07*
CAST communication
0.43*
0.40*
CAST RRBI
0.26*
0.05*
CAST Childhood Autism Spectrum Test, CAST social CAST social difficulties subscale, CAST communication CAST communication atypicalities subscale; CAST RRBI CAST repetitive, restricted behaviours and interests subscale, Conners ADHD Conners’ Parent Rating Scale, Conners ADHD hyperactivity/impulsivity Conners hyperactivity/impulsivity subscale, Conners Inattention Conners inattention subscale
* p < 0 .05
Twin correlations are shown in Table 3. These suggest that each CAST and Conners ADHD subscale was heritable. Cross-trait cross-twin correlations suggested that the covariance between the CAST and Conners ADHD subscales was largely influenced by genetic factors; with the exception of the cross-trait cross-twin correlations between RRBI and Inattention, MZ estimates exceeded DZ estimates. Many of the DZ estimates were also less than half the MZ estimates, indicating that D may have played a role in the covariance between some of the subscales. No MZ cross-trait cross-twin correction exceeded rph, indicating E.
Table 3
Twin correlations
Cross-twin correlations
Measure
Monozygotic twins
Dizygotic twins
Estimate
95 % CI
Estimate
95 % CI
CAST Social
0.72
0.70–0.74
0.34
0.30–0.38
CAST Communication
0.76
0.74–0.77
0.39
0.35–0.43
CAST RRBI
0.71
0.69–0.73
0.30
0.26–0.34
Conners Hyp/Imp
0.88
0.87–0.89
0.50
0.46–0.53
Conners Inattention
0.78
0.76–0.79
0.37
0.33–0.41
Cross-trait cross-twin correlations
Measures
Monozygotic twins
Dizygotic twins
Estimate
95 % CI
Measure
Estimate
CAST Social-Conners Hyp/Imp
0.12
0.07–0.16
0.08
0.07–0.16
CAST Social-Conners Inatt.
0.13
0.09–0.17
0.08
0.04–0.13
CAST comm.-Conners Hyp/Imp
0.44
0.41–0.48
0.27
0.23–0.31
CAST comm.-Conners Inatt.
0.41
0.37–0.45
0.19
0.14–0.23
CAST RRBI-Conners Hyp/Imp
0.26
0.22–0.30
0.21
0.17–0.26
CAST RRBI-Conners Inatt.
0.10
0.06–0.15
0.11
0.06–0.15
CAST Childhood Autism Spectrum Tes, CAST social CAST social difficulties subscale, CAST communication/CAST comm. CAST communication atypicalities subscale, CAST RRBI CAST repetitive, restricted behaviours and interests subscale, Conners hyp/imp Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (Conners ADHD) hyperactivity/impulsivity subscale, Conners inattention/Conners inatt. Conners ADHD inattention subscale
Fit statistics for the twin models tested are given in Table 4. A saturated model that equated twin correlations across sexes fitted better than one that allowed them to differ by sex, as indicated by a considerably lower BIC estimate for the constrained saturated model (−157,379.80) than for the saturated model with sex differences (−156,485.70). ACE and ADE models were thus tested with equivalent estimates in both sexes. Models included all three CAST and both Conners ADHD subscales. Relative to the saturated model, an ADE correlated factors solution had most negative BIC value (−157,928.84). In dropping D from the model, BIC (−158,004.70) reduced further, hence an AE model was chosen as the best fitting model. Parameter estimates from the AE model are all shown in Table 5. All three CAST subscales were heritable, with A explaining 69–72 % of the variance in each. The same was true for both Conners ADHD subscales (A = 75–86 %). The aetiological correlations between the measure subscales suggested that the strongest degree of genetic overlap was between Communication and both Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (r A = .50) and Inattention (r A = .48). Genetic correlations between the CAST communication difficulties subscale and traits of ADHD were significantly stronger than all other genetic correlations; there was moderate A overlap between RRBI and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (r A = .31). However, rA was low between all other subscales (r A = .08–.14). Nonshared environmental overlap between the three CAST subscales and Conners ADHD subscales was low. The highest rE estimates were between Communication and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (r E = 22) and Inattention (r E = .19). This was again significantly higher than the estimates seen between other subscales. RRBI displayed greater E overlap with hyperactivity/impulsivity (r E = .11) than Inattention (r E = −.03), while rE between Social and Conners ADHD subscales was low and largely non-significant.
Table 4
Twin model fit statistics
Model
−2LL
df
Parameters
BIC
Δχ 2
Δdf
p
AIC
Saturated
66,869.83
27,116
130
−157,379.84
ACE
67,001.24
27,196
50
−157,910.04
131.41
80
<0.001
−28.59
ADE
66,982.44
27,196
50
−157,928.84
112.61
80
<0.05
−47.39
Submodels
Model
−2LL
df
Parameters
BIC
Comparative fit with saturated model
Comparative fit with ADE model
Δχ2
Δdf
p
AIC
Δχ2
Δdf
p
AIC
AEa
67,030.60
27,211
35
−158,004.70
160.77
95
<0.001
−29.23
48.17
15
<0.001
18.17
E
73,177.18
27,226
20
−151,982.20
6,307.35
110
<0.001
6,087.35
6,194.74
30
<0.001
6,134.74
All submodels are nested within the ADE model, which was a better fit relative to the saturated model than the ACE model
−2LL fit statistic, BIC Bayesian Information Criteria, AIC Akaike’s Information Criteria, Δχ 2 change in −2LL between two models, Chi square distributed
aIndicates best fitting model; p values are corrected for multiple comparisons using the Bonferoni correction
Table 5
Estimates derived from the best fitting correlated factors solution
 
A
E
Estimate
95 % CI
Estimate
95 % CI
Variance Components Estimates
 Social
0.72
0.68–0.75
0.28
0.27–0.30
 Comm.
0.71
0.69–0.73
0.29
0.27–0.30
 RRBI
0.69
0.66–0.70
0.31
0.30–0.33
 H/I
0.86
0.84–0.87
0.14
0.12–0.15
 Inatt.
0.75
0.74–0.76
0.25
0.22–0.26
 
r A
r E
Aetiological Correlations
    
 Social–H/I
0.08
0.01 to 0.09
−0.02
0.09 to 0.05
 Socia–Inatt.
0.08
0.01 to 0.10
0.03
0.01 to 0.06
 Comm.–H/I
0.50
0.44 to 0.56
0.22
0.21 to 0.24
 Comm.–Inatt.
0.48
0.41 to 0.49
0.19
0.17 to 0.20
 RRBI–H/I
0.31
0.22 to 0.34
0.11
0.01 to 0.12
 RRBI–Inatt.
0.14
0.09 to 0.18
0.03
0.05 to 0.01
Bivariate heritability and nonshared environment
 
Conners ADHD hyperactivity/impulsivity
Conners ADHD Inattention
Bivariate heritability %
Bivariate nonshared environment %
Bivariate heritability %
Bivariate nonshared environment %
CAST social
100
0
86
14
CAST communication
91
9
88
12
CAST RRBI
92
8
80
20
All italicised estimates were not significant, as indicated by confidence intervals overlapping with zero
CAST Childhood Autism Spectrum Test, Social social difficulties subscale of the CAST, Communication/Comm. communication atypicalities subscale of the CAST, RRBI repetitive, restricted behaviours and interests subscale of the CAST, Conners ADHD Conners’ Parent Rating Scale, Hyp/imp hyperactivity/impulsivity subscale of conners adhd,Inatt inattention subscale of Conners ADHD, A additive genetic influences,E nonshared environmental influences, r A additive genetic correlation, r E nonshared environmental correlation
The majority of rph between the CAST and Conners ADHD subscales could be explained by A (see Table 5). Bivariate heritability was estimated as at least 80 % got each r ph estimate, with estimates of bivariate heritability peaking at 100 %. E, by contrast, explained no greater than 20 % of each r ph estimate. These results did not significantly change when individuals with ASC were removed from the analyses (contact the first author for further details).

Extremes analysis

Results of the DeFries–Fulker analysis are provided in full in the supplementary materials. Descriptive statistics for the CAST and Conners ADHD subscales in the 5 % extreme groups, along with the number of probands in each group, are given in Table 1.
Table S1 shows the phenotypic group correlations, which broadly mirrored the pattern of r ph estimates in the full sample. Communication displayed stronger associations with both Conners ADHD subscales (53–60) than RRBI (24–40) and Social (24–34). In the univariate analyses, DZ co-twin means were closer to 0 than MZ co-twin means, indicating \(h^{2}_{\text{g}}\) for each subscale (see Table S2). Regression analyses confirmed this; \(h^{2}_{\text{g}}\) estimates, which are shown in Table S3, ranged from .69–.73 for the CAST subscales, and from .70–.85 for the Conners ADHD subscales. In the bivariate analyses, all DZ co-twin means on the outcome measure regressed toward 0 to a greater extent than MZ co-twin means, suggesting h 2.xy (see Table S2). Further, bivariate heritability estimates, given in Table S3, indicated that phenotypic group correlations between the CAST and Conners ADHD subscales were almost entirely explained by genetic factors.
From the \(h^{2}_{\text{g}}\) and h 2.xy estimates derived from the DeFries-Fulker analyses, genetic correlations between extreme scores on the CAST and Conners ADHD subscales were calculated. Generally, these were higher than r A estimates derived in the full sample. The highest estimate was between communication and inattention (.65); communication also displayed considerable genetic overlap with hyperactivity/impulsivity (.60). RRBI displayed moderate genetic overlap with both Conners ADHD subscales (.35–.36). Genetic overlap between Social and the two Conners ADHD subscales was weak (.21) to moderate (.45).

Discussion

We tested the degree of genetic and environmental overlap between specific autistic traits and specific traits of ADHD in the general population, including, for the first time, at the extremes of the general population. The strongest genetic associations, in both the full sample and at the extremes, were between communication difficulties characteristic of ASC and traits of ADHD, with little differentiation in the magnitude of this association across different traits of ADHD. There was also a modest degree of phenotypic and genetic overlap between repetitive routines and interests, and traits of ADHD, and least overlap was observed for autistic social difficulties. As expected, the degree of aetiological overlap between autistic traits and traits of ADHD varied across different types of autistic trait.
In contrast with our expectations, and with the two existing twin studies on this subject [20, 21], we did not find that non-social autistic traits showed the strongest association between autistic and ADHD behaviours. It is notable, however, that we did still find, in some instances, moderate genetic overlap between repetitive routines and interests and traits of ADHD, at the extremes and with hyperactivity in the full sample. Hence, our findings do support the notion that traits of ADHD do, at least, show stronger associations with non-social than social autistic traits. The key question is why communication difficulties, specifically, showed the strongest overlap with traits of ADHD.
Item overlap between the CAST Communication subscale and the Conners ADHD subscales represents one possible explanation for why traits of ADHD were so strongly linked with autistic communication difficulties relative to other autistic traits. The CAST Communication subscale is concerned with pragmatic aspects of communication, enquiring specifically about the manner in which the child interacts with other people. The Conners ADHD also asks about these aspects of communication, such as whether the child often struggles to maintain conversations due to their ADHD behaviours. Yet there is considerable evidence that these measures are valid assessments of traits of ASC [25] and ADHD [26], indicating that there can be confidence that these results represent genuine genetic overlap between communication difficulties and traits of ADHD.
From these findings one might predict that individuals with ADHD will be more likely to display communication difficulties similar to those seen in ASC. Indeed, research on the language abilities of individuals with ASC and ADHD are producing somewhat overlapping findings. While structural language skills show considerable variability in both these conditions, pragmatic language difficulties appear to feature prominently in both [36, 37]. This pattern might be expected if it is the case that the genes that are associated with communication difficulties characteristic of ASC also link with traits of ADHD.
On a theoretical level, these findings lend additional support to the fractionable autism triad hypothesis [38, 39]. This hypothesis posits that the core features of ASC are each associated with largely different causal influences, and is supported by twin studies [1217]. These findings extend this hypothesis, by suggesting that the manner in which core autistic traits relate to traits characteristic of ADHD differs across autistic trait domains. Intriguingly, these findings are similar to those reported in a twin study, also conducted on the TEDS sample, that suggested heightened aetiological overlap between communication difficulties and internalising traits [40], and also reflect those of a recent longitudinal twin study of the phenotypic associations between ASC and ADHD [8]. Thus, this study adds to increasing evidence to suggest that the fractionable autism triad hypothesis extends to how autistic traits relate to traits of other conditions.
Findings such as these have the potential to inform molecular genetic research endeavours targeted at identifying genetic variants that are shared across ASC and other conditions. This is clearly a distal goal; most molecular genetic studies have focused on ASC and ADHD as single constructs. For instance, one study reported that genetic variants, specifically calcium-signalling genes, were associated with both ASC and ADHD [41]. Could these variants be associated, in particular, with communicative aspects of ASC? Research taking a symptom-specific approach to ASC is scarce [e.g. 42]. Encouragingly, a recent paper, using data from the ALSPAC sample, tested the association between polygenic scores for ADHD and various neurodevelopmental difficulties, including autistic traits. Of note, the authors reported that while polygenic scores for ADHD did not significantly associate with social cognition difficulties, they were particularly linked with pragmatic language abilities [43], which is a similar pattern to that seen in the present study. Thus, our findings, along with these independent results, suggest that this is a potentially fruitful future direction for molecular genetic research on ASC and ADHD overlap.
These findings need to be regarded in light of the limitations of this study. Owing to the large sample size, requisite for twin model fitting to be adequately performed and for sufficient probands to be identified for extremes analysis, we were unable to conduct in-depth assessments on participants. However, this need not be viewed as a limitation. There is evidence that the heritability of traits of ASC and ADHD is constant across the general population and at the extremes [5, 6], and we did find similar, albeit slightly stronger, associations at the extremes of our sample. Furthermore, we were able to avoid referral biases that can be present in clinically ascertained samples, which can inflate co-occurrence rates between conditions. One might question whether these findings from twins generalise to singletons. However, autistic traits and traits of ADHD do not appear to be elevated in twins relative to singletons [44, 45]. Future studies should also focus on assessments of ASC and ADHD from multiple raters, rather than parents alone. The internal consistency of the three CAST subscales was relatively low, as noted previously [13, 14]. Particular items failing to contribute to the internal consistency sufficiently did not account for this, since removing any items did not result in discernable improvements in internal consistency. The modest internal consistency is, perhaps, due to the relatively small number of items that comprise each of the subscales, or heterogeneity within each subscale. An important future direction, then, is to assess autistic traits in a manner that maximises reliability, although good construct validity for the CAST subscales has been reported previously [13, 14].
This study demonstrated that the degree to which autistic traits and traits of ADHD share genetic and environmental influences with one another varies by autistic trait domain, showing for the first time that this pattern is also apparent amongst individuals in the general population displaying particularly extreme degrees of traits of ASC and ADHD. Autistic communication difficulties, in particular, were associated with similar genetic influences to traits of ADHD, more so than autistic social or non-social behaviours. These findings from a community sample suggest the importance of taking a trait-specific approach to understanding why ASC co-occurs with other conditions, rather than focusing on the ASC phenotype as a whole. These findings may lead to the prediction that communication difficulties characteristic of ASC will present with increased frequency in individuals with ADHD. Indeed, similar communication difficulties, such as pragmatic difficulties, have been reported in both ASC and ADHD [36, 37]; our findings offer a potential explanation with regard to these findings by suggesting that similar causes are associated with both traits of ADHD and communication difficulties characteristic of ASC.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge all participating families and researchers involved in TEDS for making this study possible. We thank Professor Robert Plomin for this research collaboration. TEDS is funded by a programme grant from the UK Medical Research Council [G0901245; and previously G0500079]. The first author was funded by the UK Medical Research Council and Economic and Social Research Council.

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Psychiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der psychiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Anhänge

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Association, Washington American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Association, Washington
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Simonoff E, Pickles A, Charman T, Chandler S, Loucas T, Baird G (2008) Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 47:921–929. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f CrossRefPubMed Simonoff E, Pickles A, Charman T, Chandler S, Loucas T, Baird G (2008) Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 47:921–929. doi:10.​1097/​CHI.​0b013e318179964f​ CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Yoshida Y, Uchiyama T (2004) The clinical necessity for assessing attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD) symptoms in children with high-functioning pervasive developmental disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 13:307–314. doi:10.1007/s00787-004-0391-1 CrossRefPubMed Yoshida Y, Uchiyama T (2004) The clinical necessity for assessing attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD) symptoms in children with high-functioning pervasive developmental disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 13:307–314. doi:10.​1007/​s00787-004-0391-1 CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor MJ, Charman T, Robinson EB, Plomin R, Happé F, Asherson P, Ronald A (2013) Developmental associations between traits of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a genetically informative, longitudinal twin study. Psychol Med 43:1735–1746. doi:10.1017/S003329171200253X CrossRefPubMed Taylor MJ, Charman T, Robinson EB, Plomin R, Happé F, Asherson P, Ronald A (2013) Developmental associations between traits of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a genetically informative, longitudinal twin study. Psychol Med 43:1735–1746. doi:10.​1017/​S003329171200253​X CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Lundström S, Chang Z, Kerekes N, Gumpert CH, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Lichtenstein P, Anckarsäter H (2011) Autistic-like traits and their association with mental health problems in two nationwide twin cohorts of children and adults. Psychol Med 41:2423–2433CrossRefPubMed Lundström S, Chang Z, Kerekes N, Gumpert CH, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Lichtenstein P, Anckarsäter H (2011) Autistic-like traits and their association with mental health problems in two nationwide twin cohorts of children and adults. Psychol Med 41:2423–2433CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Ronald A, Happé F, Bolton P, Butcher LM, Price TS, Wheelwright S, Baron-Cohen S, Plomin R (2006) Genetic heterogeneity between the three components of the autism spectrum: a twin study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 45:691–699. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000215325.9d CrossRefPubMed Ronald A, Happé F, Bolton P, Butcher LM, Price TS, Wheelwright S, Baron-Cohen S, Plomin R (2006) Genetic heterogeneity between the three components of the autism spectrum: a twin study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 45:691–699. doi:10.​1097/​01.​chi.​0000215325.​9d CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat McLoughlin G, Ronald A, Kuntsi J, Asherson P, Plomin R (2007) Genetic support for the dual nature of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: substantial genetic overlap between the inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive components. J Abnorm Child Psychol 35:999–1008. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9149-9 PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed McLoughlin G, Ronald A, Kuntsi J, Asherson P, Plomin R (2007) Genetic support for the dual nature of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: substantial genetic overlap between the inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive components. J Abnorm Child Psychol 35:999–1008. doi:10.​1007/​s10802-007-9149-9 PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Greven CU, Rijsdijk FV, Plomin R (2011) A twin study of ADHD symptoms in early adolescence: hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattentiveness show substantial genetic overlap but also genetic specificity. J Abnorm Child Psychol 39:265–275. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9451-9 CrossRefPubMed Greven CU, Rijsdijk FV, Plomin R (2011) A twin study of ADHD symptoms in early adolescence: hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattentiveness show substantial genetic overlap but also genetic specificity. J Abnorm Child Psychol 39:265–275. doi:10.​1007/​s10802-010-9451-9 CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat McLoughlin G, Rijsdijk F, Asherson P, Kuntsi J (2011) Parents and teachers make different contributions to a shared perspective on hyperactive-impulsive and inattentive symptoms: a multivariate analysis of parent and teacher ratings on the symptom domains of ADHD. Behav Genet 41:668–679. doi:10.1007/s10519-011-9473-2 PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed McLoughlin G, Rijsdijk F, Asherson P, Kuntsi J (2011) Parents and teachers make different contributions to a shared perspective on hyperactive-impulsive and inattentive symptoms: a multivariate analysis of parent and teacher ratings on the symptom domains of ADHD. Behav Genet 41:668–679. doi:10.​1007/​s10519-011-9473-2 PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Ronald A, Larsson H, Anckarsater H, Lichtenstein P (2014) Symptoms of autism and ADHD: a Swedish twin study examining their overlap. J Abnorm Psychol 123:440–451. doi:10.1037/a0036088 CrossRefPubMed Ronald A, Larsson H, Anckarsater H, Lichtenstein P (2014) Symptoms of autism and ADHD: a Swedish twin study examining their overlap. J Abnorm Psychol 123:440–451. doi:10.​1037/​a0036088 CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Haworth CM, Davis OS, Plomin R (2013) Twins early development study (TEDS): a genetically sensitive investigation of cognitive and behavioral development from childhood to young adulthood. Twin Res Hum Genet 16:117–125. doi:10.1017/thg.2012.91 CrossRefPubMed Haworth CM, Davis OS, Plomin R (2013) Twins early development study (TEDS): a genetically sensitive investigation of cognitive and behavioral development from childhood to young adulthood. Twin Res Hum Genet 16:117–125. doi:10.​1017/​thg.​2012.​91 CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Conners CK, Sitarenios G, Parker JDA, Epstein JN (1998) The revised Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. J Abnorm Child Psychology 26:257–268. doi:10.1023/A:1022602400621 CrossRef Conners CK, Sitarenios G, Parker JDA, Epstein JN (1998) The revised Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. J Abnorm Child Psychology 26:257–268. doi:10.​1023/​A:​1022602400621 CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Neale MC, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes HH (2003) Mx: statistical modeling. Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond Neale MC, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes HH (2003) Mx: statistical modeling. Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Markon KE, Krueger RF (2004) An empirical comparison of information-theoretic selection criteria for multivariate behavior genetic models. Behav Genet 34:593–610CrossRefPubMed Markon KE, Krueger RF (2004) An empirical comparison of information-theoretic selection criteria for multivariate behavior genetic models. Behav Genet 34:593–610CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Raftery AE (1995) Bayesian model selection in social research. Social Methodol 25:111–163CrossRef Raftery AE (1995) Bayesian model selection in social research. Social Methodol 25:111–163CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Purcell S, Eley TC, Dale PS, Oliver B, Petrill SA, Price TS, Saudino KJ, Simonoff E, Stevenson J, Taylor E, Plomin R (2001) Comorbidity between verbal and non-verbal cognitive delay in 2-year-olds: a bivariate twin analysis. Dev Sci 4:195–208CrossRef Purcell S, Eley TC, Dale PS, Oliver B, Petrill SA, Price TS, Saudino KJ, Simonoff E, Stevenson J, Taylor E, Plomin R (2001) Comorbidity between verbal and non-verbal cognitive delay in 2-year-olds: a bivariate twin analysis. Dev Sci 4:195–208CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Knopik VS, Alarcón M, DeFries JC (1997) Comorbidity of mathematics and reading deficits: evidence for a genetic etiology. Behav Genet 27:447–453CrossRefPubMed Knopik VS, Alarcón M, DeFries JC (1997) Comorbidity of mathematics and reading deficits: evidence for a genetic etiology. Behav Genet 27:447–453CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Geurts HM, Verté S, Oosterlaan J, Roeyers H, Hartman CA, Mulder EJ, Berckelaer-Onnes IA, Sergeant JA (2004) Can the Children’s Communication Checklist differentiate between children with autism, children with ADHD, and normal controls? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45:1437–1453. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00326.x CrossRefPubMed Geurts HM, Verté S, Oosterlaan J, Roeyers H, Hartman CA, Mulder EJ, Berckelaer-Onnes IA, Sergeant JA (2004) Can the Children’s Communication Checklist differentiate between children with autism, children with ADHD, and normal controls? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45:1437–1453. doi:10.​1111/​j.​1469-7610.​2004.​00326.​x CrossRefPubMed
40.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Ronald A, Butcher LM, Docherty S, Davis OS, Schalkwyk LC, Craig IW, Plomin R (2010) A genome-wide association study of social and non-social autistic-like traits in the general population using pooled DNA, 500 K SNP microarrays and both community and diagnosed autism replication samples. Behav Genet 40:31–45. doi:10.1007/s10519-009-9308-6 PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Ronald A, Butcher LM, Docherty S, Davis OS, Schalkwyk LC, Craig IW, Plomin R (2010) A genome-wide association study of social and non-social autistic-like traits in the general population using pooled DNA, 500 K SNP microarrays and both community and diagnosed autism replication samples. Behav Genet 40:31–45. doi:10.​1007/​s10519-009-9308-6 PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin J, Hamshere ML, Stergiakouli E, O’Donovan MC, Thapar A (2014) Genetic risk for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder contributes to neurodevelopmental traits in the general population. Biol Psychiatry (epub ahead of print). doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.02.013 Martin J, Hamshere ML, Stergiakouli E, O’Donovan MC, Thapar A (2014) Genetic risk for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder contributes to neurodevelopmental traits in the general population. Biol Psychiatry (epub ahead of print). doi:10.​1016/​j.​biopsych.​2014.​02.​013
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Curran S, Dworzynski K, Happé F, Ronald A, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S, Brayne C, Bolton PF (2011) No major effect of twinning on autistic traits. Autism Res 4:377–382. doi:10.1002/aur.207 CrossRefPubMed Curran S, Dworzynski K, Happé F, Ronald A, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S, Brayne C, Bolton PF (2011) No major effect of twinning on autistic traits. Autism Res 4:377–382. doi:10.​1002/​aur.​207 CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Moilanen I, Linna SL, Ebeling H, Kumpulainen K, Tamminen T, Piha J, Almqvist F (1999) Are twins’ behavioural/emotional problems different from singletons? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 8:62–67CrossRefPubMed Moilanen I, Linna SL, Ebeling H, Kumpulainen K, Tamminen T, Piha J, Almqvist F (1999) Are twins’ behavioural/emotional problems different from singletons? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 8:62–67CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Where are the strongest associations between autistic traits and traits of ADHD? evidence from a community-based twin study
verfasst von
Mark J. Taylor
Tony Charman
Angelica Ronald
Publikationsdatum
01.09.2015
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry / Ausgabe 9/2015
Print ISSN: 1018-8827
Elektronische ISSN: 1435-165X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-014-0666-0

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 9/2015

European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 9/2015 Zur Ausgabe

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.