Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 1/2018

Open Access 19.06.2017 | Original Contribution

The influence of perceived parenting styles on socio-emotional development from pre-puberty into puberty

verfasst von: Min Yee Ong, Janna Eilander, Seang Mei Saw, Yuhuan Xie, Michael J. Meaney, Birit F. P. Broekman

Erschienen in: European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2018

Abstract

The relative impact of parenting on socio-emotional development of children has rarely been examined in a longitudinal context. This study examined the association between perceived parenting styles and socio-emotional functioning from childhood to adolescence. We hypothesized that optimal parenting associated with improvement in socio-emotional functioning from childhood into early adulthood, especially for those with more behavioral problems in childhood. Children between ages 7 and 9 years were recruited for the Singapore Cohort Study of Risk Factors for Myopia (SCORM). Nine years later, 700 out of 1052 subjects were followed up (67%). During childhood, parents completed the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), while young adults completed the Youth Self-Report (YSR) and Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI). Perceived optimal parental care resulted in less internalizing and externalizing problems in early adulthood in comparison to non-optimal parental care styles. Perceived optimal paternal parenting, but not maternal parenting, in interaction with childhood externalizing problems predicted externalizing symptoms in early adulthood. No significant interactions were found between perceived parenting styles and internalizing problems. In conclusion, perceived parental care associates with the quality of socio-emotional development, while optimal parenting by the father is especially important for children with more externalizing problems in childhood.

Introduction

The onset of most common mental disorders is before adulthood [1, 2]. The early onset of psychopathology implies a formidable cost for both the individual and society. The peripubertal peak in the onset of mental disorders underscores the importance of early interventions targeting at-risk children [3]. The ability to effectively identify children at risk is essential for the design and delivery of prevention programs [46]. These considerations suggest that it is critical to understand how socio-emotional functioning changes over development and to identify the course of this variation.
Emotional and behavioral dysregulation is an important predictor of mental disorders later in life [7]. However, the association between measures of emotional and behavioral problems in childhood and later psychopathology, while statistically significant, shows variation over time with evidence for a considerable portion of children exhibiting emotional or behavioral difficulties in early life, but without evidence of psychopathology at later ages [8, 9]. It is thus important to understand the sources of variation in emotions and behavior and to identify factors that moderate changes over time.
One of the most important factors that influence socio-emotional development is parenting [3, 1014]. Positive forms of parenting are beneficial for cognitive and social development, while negative parenting such as punishment and low warmth are associated with disruptive behavior in children and increased risk for psychopathology [11, 15]. These effects are apparent in studies that used self-reports of perceived parenting styles using measures such as the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) [16]. For example, PBI scores characterized by low care and high overprotection predict psychopathology such as depression, anxiety disorders, and eating disorders [1620].
Studies of the relationship between perceived parenting and psychopathology are most commonly explored with cross-sectional design and often conducted in adult samples [2123]. The design of such studies does not permit an analysis of the potential role of parenting as a moderator of the relationship between early risk and later outcomes. In addition, most studies focus on clinical samples, or on groups with low socio-emotional status [24]. Hence, these samples do not inform about the extent of perceived parenting effects on socio-emotional development in a general population.
The aim of this study is to use a longitudinal design to examine whether parenting, as measured with the PBI, is a moderator of the relation between early socio-emotional functioning and later mental health. We hypothesize that perceived optimal parenting, characterized by high warmth and low control, in interaction with behavioral and emotional profiles in middle childhood, will have a positive effect on the socio-emotional development over time and lead to less reported symptoms of psychopathology during late adolescence/early adulthood. Moreover, since the effects of intervention programs that target parental care are often greatest among children with negative temperament [25], we predicted that the effect of optimal parenting would be most apparent among children with increased emotional and behavioral problems in childhood.

Methods

Participants

The current study sample was derived from the SCORM that recruited a total of 1979 children out of 2913 Asian children (participation rate: 67.9%) from three normal stream schools in Singapore between November 1999 and May 2001. The majority of the children in Singapore attend normal stream schools, while children with intellectual disability (IQ <70) often attend ‘special’ schools [26]. Children with serious chronic medical conditions (e.g., heart disorders, cancer, and chronic eye conditions) were excluded (n = 94).

Measures of internalizing and externalizing traits

The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) 4–18 parental report and the Youth Self-Report (YSR) are both tests of emotional and behavioral difficulties for, respectively, parents of children between 4 and 18 years old (CBCL), and for adolescents between 11 and 18 years of age (YSR). The CBCL consists of 118 items and the YSR 112 items, both assessed with a three-point Likert scale, with eight subscales, two syndrome groups, and a total problem score each. The two syndrome groups are internalizing and externalizing scale scores. The internalizing scale score is a grouping of social withdrawal, somatic complaints and anxiety/depression items, whereas the externalizing score is a grouping of delinquent behavior and aggressive behavior items [27]. We used raw scores for both the CBCL and YSR [28]. The CBCL and YSR have good construct validity and acceptable test–retest reliability coefficients among the subscales. Cronbach’s α values range from .62 to .92 [27].

Measurement of parenting styles

The PBI is a widely used battery for assessing parenting behaviors [29]. It is a self-report questionnaire that asks participants retrospectively about the perceived parenting styles of both parents during the first 16 years of their life. Participants complete sections for mother and father separately. PBI measures perceived levels of care and overprotection/control. It consists of 25 items, 12 ‘care’ items and 13 ‘overprotection’ items [7]. The reliability and validity of the PBI have been found to be satisfactory [29].

Measurements of demographic and socioeconomic data

Parents completed a questionnaire, in English or Chinese, on demographic information inclusive socioeconomic indicators at the baseline visit. Ethnicity was determined based on the father’s reported ethnicity [30]. Socioeconomic status was assessed on the basis of maternal education, a frequently used index in studies with children.

Procedures

Before commencing study, all parents provided written informed consent while children provided written assent. At baseline, the children were in grades one to three (7–9 years old). At that time the children were assessed with an IQ test (Raven Standard Progressive Matrices), parents provided information on demographics and filled out the English version of the CBCL. Of the initial SCORM sample of 1979 children, only 1336 completed the CBCL, because one of the three schools did not participate in completing the CBCL. Subsequently, participants were followed up 9 years after baseline. During follow-up, between the ages of 16 and 21, a package consisting of a consent form on the follow-up study and a set of self-report questionnaires including YSR and PBI in English were mailed to 1052 participants. After giving consent, the participants were to complete and mail back the set of questionnaires to the study team. Seven hundred of the 1052 participants gave consent for the follow-up study (response rate 66.5%). Among those 700 participants, 460 had complete data on the CBCL, YSR and PBI; hence, they were included for analyses in this study. The Singapore Eye Research Institute Ethics Committee approved the initial and follow-up study and the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki were observed.

Statistics

To appreciate parenting effects, we were especially interested in those children with discrepancies between their CBCL and YSR scores. This includes one group with high CBCL score at age 7–9 years, but low YSR score at young adulthood, and another group with an initial low score on the CBCL when young, but high YSR score at later stage in life.
CBCL and YSR raw scores were used and adjusted for gender and age. Differences in ages, gender, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and IQ between the included and excluded samples were analyzed by using parametric tests if normality and homogeneity assumptions were satisfied; otherwise, the non-parametric Mann–Whitney U test was used. Categorical data were analyzed using the Chi-squared test.
The association between scores on the CBCL and YSR in interaction with parenting styles was analyzed as categorical variables in general linear models, where CBCL and YSR were divided into four groups: low scores, low average scores, high average scores and high scores. For parenting styles, comparisons were made between optimal versus “non-optimal parenting” (which groups affectionate constraint, affectionless control, and neglectful parenting) as well as optimal versus affectionless control, neglectful, and affectionate constraint parenting separately [31].
All outliers above 3 standard deviations were removed. All statistical procedures were performed with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) 16.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and p values were two-tailed and considered statistically significant when the values were below .05.

Results

Of the 460 participants with complete data on the CBCL, YSR and PBI, 4 were older than 21 years and 8 with missing age; hence, they were excluded for analyses in this study. Based on scatter plots, three outliers (i.e., >3 SD away) were removed. Thus, data from 445 participants with complete data (192 boys and 253 girls) were included in our final analyses.
The age ranged from 6 to 10 years with a mean of 7.8 years (SD = 0.83) at the time of baseline (CBCL) and 16 to 21 years with a mean of 18.4 years (SD = 1.32) at the second assessment that included the YSR and PBI. The mean score for internalizing problems on the CBCL raw score was 7.68 (SD = 7.53, range 0–58) and 7.71(SD = 7.06, range 0–65) for externalizing problems. The mean score for internalizing problems on the YSR raw score was 13.0 (SD = 8.4, range 0–48) and 10.5 (SD = 6.8, range 0–33) for externalizing problems. There was a high correlation between the CBCL and YSR total raw scores as well as sub-scores (p < .001). No main effects on maternal education or IQ were found. Hence, these variables were not included in subsequent analyses.
Comparison on group characteristics were conducted for those included for analysis (n = 445) and those excluded from analysis (n = 255) in the group of 700 children who were followed up with questionnaires returned. Chi-squared analysis was conducted to compare group differences in gender, race, income, and housing, while non-parametric Mann–Whitney U test was conducted to compare group differences in age and IQ scores, as they were not normally distributed in our sample (with more children having higher IQs and more children being 7 instead of 8 or 9 years old at baseline). No significant difference was found between the groups (see Table 1). We also explored if there were differences in PBI and CBCL between the two groups using Chi-square analysis for PBI groups and independent t test for CBCL. Likewise, no significant difference was found between the groups for PBI and CBCL (see Tables 2, 3 for PBI and CBCL analysis, respectively).
Table 1
Comparison on demographic variables of children included for analysis (N = 445) and excluded from analysis (N = 255) from the group with returned questionnaires at follow-up visit
 
Remained in follow-up visit (N = 700)
Included for analysis (N = 445)
Excluded for analysis (N = 255)
X (p value)
Gender
  
0.643 (.423)
 Boys
192
118
 
 Girls
253
137
 
Race
  
6.09 (.193)
 Chinese
347
198
 
 Malay
65
40
 
 Indian
30
13
 
 Others
2
0
 
 Missing
1
4
 
Income
  
5.94 (.051)
 $2000 or less/mth
121
89
 
 >$20,000–$50,000/mth
212
115
 
 >$5000/mth
106
46
 
 Missing
6
5
 
Housing
  
4.77 (.190)
 1–3 Room
49
41
 
 4 Room and above
345
182
 
 Private housing
25
18
 
 Others
26
13
 
 Missing
0
1
 
Father education level
  
4.14 (.388)
 No formal education
11
9
 
 Elementary school
100
66
 
 High school
197
116
 
 Pre-university/diploma
76
40
 
 University
61
24
 
Mother education level
  
6.92 (.140)
 No formal education
13
10
 
 Elementary school
108
77
 
 High school
228
115
 
 Pre-university/diploma
69
40
 
 University
37
13
 
   
U (p value)
Age (at first visit)
Mean (SD)
7.80 (0.83)
7.92 (0.88)
52,919.5 (.113)
IQ (at first visit)
Mean (SD)
116.08 (10.7)
114.25 (11.66)
38,713 (.068)
Table 2
Comparison on PBI score distribution according to bonding type, means and standard deviation on perceived level of care and control by parents in children included for analysis (N = 445) and excluded from analysis (N = 255)
Remained in follow-up visit (N = 700)
 
Included for analysis (N = 445)
Excluded for analysis (N = 255)
X (p value)
Maternal
 Bonding type (%)
  Optimal
30.8
22.0
 
  Neglectful
18.0
14.5
 
  Affectionate constraint
17.8
11.0
 
  Affectionless Control
28.8
24.7
 
  Missing
4.7
27.8
 
Maternal care [Mean (SD)]
26.20 (6.16)
26.33 (6.30)
28.6 (.486)
Maternal control [Mean (SD)]
13.30 (6.32)
13.56 (6.90)
32.0 (.466)
Paternal
 Bonding type (%)
  Optimal
27.2
18.4
 
  Neglectful
20.0
12.9
 
  Affectionate constraint
17.3
10.2
 
  Affectionless control
27.0
23.1
 
  Missing
8.5
35.3
 
Paternal care [Mean (SD)]
23.16 (6.73)
22.66 (7.33)
38.5 (.235)
Paternal control [Mean (SD)]
11.84 (6.35)
12.43 (6.70)
35.0 (.284)
Table 3
Comparison on CBCL score [means (SD)] between groups of children included for analysis (N = 445) and excluded from analysis (N = 255)
Remained in follow-up visit (N = 700)
CBCL Raw Score
Included for analysis (N = 445)
Excluded for analysis (N = 255)
t (p value)
Internalizing
7.68 (7.53)
7.30 (6.77)
−.444 (.657)
Externalizing
7.71 (7.06)
7.12 (6.03)
−.742 (.458)
Total
27.24 (22.46)
25.72 (20.01)
−.604 (.546)

Perceived parenting styles in Singapore youths

The scores of the PBI showed that the majority of mothers and fathers were perceived by the participants to demonstrate optimal parenting styles, followed by affectionless controlled parenting styles. Using paired sample t test; significant differences on the PBI subscales “Care” and “Control” were found between Singaporean mothers and Singaporean fathers (see Table 2). Singaporean mothers scored higher on both care [t(399) = 9.81, p < .001] and control [t(400) = 5.44, p < .001] when compared with fathers.

Effects of parenting styles on YSR

General linear model of CBCL categorical scores based on quartiles (low scores/below average/above average and high scores) in interaction with parenting styles on YSR were analyzed. The main effects showed that scores for participants reporting optimal maternal care and paternal care were significantly lower than for participants reporting “non-optimal care” (combination of all not-optimal parenting styles, namely “affectionless control”, “neglectful”, and “affectionate constraint”) on the YSR internalizing, externalizing and total score (see Table 4), When comparing optimal parenting with “affectionless control”, “neglectful”, and “affectionate constraint” parenting separately, all main effects were significant for all three YSR scales except for paternal affectionate constraint parenting.
Table 4
Main effects of perceived optimal parenting (F value) with other parenting style on YSR internalizing, externalizing and total score
 
Mother
Father
YSR internalizing
22.7***
15.0***
 Optimal
(n = 131)
(n = 117)
 Non-optimal
(n = 272)
(n = 265)
 
24.1***
16.9***
 Optimal
(n = 131)
(n = 117)
 Affectionless control
(n = 121)
(n = 112)
 
1.94
0.53
 Optimal
(n = 131)
(n = 117)
 Affectionate constraint
(n = 78)
(n = 72)
 
16.7***
11.6**
 Optimal
(n = 131)
(n = 117)
 Neglectful
(n = 73)
(n = 81)
YSR externalizing
17.1***
14.4***
 Optimal
(n = 134)
(n = 117)
 Non-optimal
(n = 276)
(n = 275)
 
13.9***
12.6***
 Optimal
(n = 134)
(n = 117)
 Affectionless control
(n = 123)
(n = 114)
 
2.60
4.10*
 Optimal
(n = 134)
(n = 117)
 Affectionate constraint
(n = 74)
(n = 74)
 
16.8***
10.9**
 Optimal
(n = 134)
(n = 117)
 Neglectful
(n = 79)
(n = 87)
YSR total
23.6***
15.8***
 Optimal
(n = 125)
(n = 109)
 Non-optimal
(n = 247)
(n = 241)
 
19.40***
18.2***
 Optimal
(n = 125)
(n = 109)
 Affectionless control
(n = 108)
(n = 98)
 
5.82*
3.42
 Optimal
(n = 125)
(n = 109)
 Affectionate constraint
(n = 72)
(n = 67)
 
17.6***
9.60**
 Optimal
(n = 125)
(n = 109)
 Neglectful
(n = 67)
(n = 76)
*** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05

Interactions between CBCL and perceived parenting styles

No significant interaction was found between internalizing traits of childhood (CBCL) and parenting styles (of mother or father) on internalizing traits in adolescence/young adulthood (YSR). However, significant interactions were found between paternal parenting styles and externalizing traits in childhood (CBCL) and externalizing traits in adolescence/young adulthood (YSR). More specifically, a significant interaction was found between optimal parenting of the father and externalizing traits in childhood (CBCL) and externalizing traits in adolescence/young adulthood (YSR), F(3, 374) = 4.08, p = .007 (See Fig. 1). Also, significant interactions were found between paternal optimal parenting versus paternal affectionless control parenting [F(3, 213) = 2.87, p = .037] (See Fig. 2), and versus parental neglectful parenting and CBCL externalizing traits on YSR externalizing traits [F(3, 186) = 3.50, p = .017] (See Fig. 3), but only not versus affectionate constraint [F(3, 173) = 1.79, p = .15] (see Fig. 4) and externalizing traits in childhood on externalizing traits in young adulthood. In other words, and as shown in the figures, optimal care of the father lowers the risk of developing externalizing problems in youth for those children who had already higher externalizing scores in their childhood. Interestingly no interaction was found between any of the maternal parenting styles with internalizing or externalizing traits of the child (see Table 5).
Table 5
Interactions of perceived parenting style by fathers and mothers (optimal versus other parenting style) and CBCL internalizing, externalizing and total score on YSR internalizing, externalizing and total score [F(p value)]
 
YSR
Internalizing
Externalizing
Total
Opt vs. non-opt
 PBI father
  CBCL int
1.17 (.32)
  CBCL ext
4.08** (.007)
  CBCL total
1.63 (.18)
 PBI mother
  CBCL int
0.80 (.50)
  CBCL ext
0.91 (.44)
  CBCL total
1.05 (.37)
Opt vs. affectionless control
 PBI father
  CBCL int
1.21 (.31)
  CBCL ext
2.87* (.037)
  CBCL total
1.29 (.28)
 PBI mother
  CBCL int
1.71 (.16)
  CBCL ext
1.62 (.19)
  CBCL total
2.11 (.10)
Opt vs. affectionate constraint
 PBI father
  CBCL int
0.98 (.40)
  CBCL ext
1.79 (.15)
  CBCL total
0.92 (.43)
 PBI mother
  CBCL int
1.14 (.34)
  CBCL ext
0.50 (.68)
  CBCL total
0.60 (.62)
Opt vs. neglectful
 PBI father
  CBCL int
1.22 (.30)
  CBCL ext
3.50* (.017)
  CBCL total
1.56 (.20)
 PBI mother
  CBCL int
1.10 (.35)
  CBCL ext
0.66 (.58)
  CBCL total
0.75 (.53)
*** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05

Discussion

Our data show a high correlation between behavioral and emotional traits in childhood and those in young adulthood, suggesting a general consistency of behavior and emotional profiles/traits over time. This is especially interesting, since parents completed the CBCL of children at 7–9 years of age, while the young adult participants completed the YSR at a mean age of 18 years. This finding also suggests a high inter-rater reliability of these screening instruments over time in our cohort. Second, our data show strong main effects of perceived maternal care and paternal care, suggesting that both are influential for socio-emotional development. Third, although we found evidence of a differential effect of parental care as a function of socio-emotional status in childhood, as reflected in the CBCL data, this effect was only unique to externalizing scores in the YSR. Thus, we report a greater effect of paternal care on externalizing scores among youth with higher externalizing scores in childhood. This finding is consistent with previous reports of increased susceptibility to the effects of parenting among young children with more difficult childhood temperament [25].
Interestingly, while we found the main effects of both perceived maternal and paternal care on socio-emotional function as reflected in the YSR, the interaction effect between externalizing scores in childhood and care on externalizing scores on the YSR was apparent only for paternal care (Table 5; Fig. 1). This interaction effect was significant for optimal versus non-optimal parenting and specifically for affectionless control and neglectful parenting. These findings suggest that optimal parenting by the father is especially important for children who score high on externalizing problems in childhood, although optimal parenting of the mother also seems to decrease externalizing problems over time. For children with elevated externalized traits, optimal parenting by the father seems to be a protective factor; hence, highly reactive or negative children are more susceptible to positive parenting by the father than their less reactive peers. This finding is in concordance with previous findings of increased susceptibility to parenting styles of children with difficult behavioral and emotional traits [25, 32].
Our findings are in agreement with previous studies, which suggested differential effects of caretaking by mothers or fathers. Thus, associations of ADHD with comorbid externalizing problems were negatively associated with care of the father [14]. A study by Narusyte and colleagues [33] found that critical remarks of father, but not mother, were associated with externalizing behavior of adolescents. Other studies also suggest the importance of the paternal role with increasing age of the child. For example, in a sample of 1364 children of ten different Western geographic areas, maternal support for child autonomy in boys was mediated by higher self-reliance at grade one, while the paternal support for child autonomy was associated with increase in self-reliance in boys for a longer period, from grades one to three [34]. In addition, having a caring father has been associated with lower risk of certain psychopathologies, such as posttraumatic stress disorder [35].
In contrast to previous studies, we did not find any interaction effect of perceived parenting styles and socio-emotional status in childhood on later internalizing problems [36]. This inconsistency in findings might be explained by the differences in the study population, study design, stability of internalizing traits in our population, or differences between parenting practices in different cultures. Although some parenting practices (e.g., nurturing and protecting offspring [37]) and parent–child interaction therapies are applicable across cultures [38], a discrepancy has been described between parenting styles in Asia in comparison to European-American cultures [39], with parenting styles in Asia being more controlling than in Europe and America. In contrast to the existing literature, a higher percentage of Singaporean mothers and fathers in our study displayed affectionless control as well as neglectful parenting styles, but a lower percentage displayed affectionate control parenting styles when compared to other non-clinical samples from Europe [40]. Despite the differences in parenting styles, we only found interaction effects between paternal parenting styles, but not between controlling or neglectful parenting styles of the mother with socio-emotional status in childhood on mental health in adolescents when assessed across the entire sample. A possible explanation can be gleaned from previous studies that showed differential effects of maternal parenting styles in Asian cultures as compared to westernized cultures. There are a number of reports showing that controlling styles of mothers associate with positive outcomes in Asian participants. For example, Li and colleagues [41] found that perceived maternal authoritarian parenting styles were related to positive socio-emotional development in Chinese sample, but not in the European young adults.
A major strength of this study is the prospective study design with a reasonable sample size for a longitudinal study conducted over more than 10 years. Additionally, internalizing and externalizing problems were explored over the full range of the spectrum, with most children being in the normal range. Our study also has limitations. First, the mean score on the YSR externalizing scale is in the lower range; hence, most children do not have serious externalizing problems. As development trajectory studies show that a reduction of externalizing problems is expected for both sexes from childhood to adolescence [3], this raises the question on how much of this improvement in externalizing symptoms can be attributed to the parenting style of the father. Although in our study the overall externalizing symptoms indeed decreased over time, we still found an interaction effect with optimal parenting of father suggesting that a perceived optimal parenting style of father further reduces externalizing symptoms from childhood to early adulthood. Second, the data are based on subjective information. However, we found a high correlation between CBCL, filled out by parents, and YSR, filled out by the adolescents, which suggests high inter-rater reliability. Moreover, in previous studies, child reports of parenting have been widely used and found to be consistent with parents’ and observers’ reports [42, 43]. Furthermore, subjective experiences are thought to have a greater impact on physiology and behavior response than objective measurements [44]. Third, in our study we have investigated the parenting styles of mother and father separately, while in “real life” both parenting styles would have interacted and created an impact on the child. Also, a one- or two-parent household may influence the effect of parenting styles on the child. Although it may be difficult to collect this information over a longer period, as this may change over time, it is a limitation that we lack this information in our study. Another drawback is that our findings may not be representative of the whole Singaporean population, as the socioeconomic status in our sample was slightly higher as compared to the national sample and the children had generally a higher IQ. Although we controlled for socioeconomic status, our data may not be representative of parents and children with a very low socioeconomic status.

Conclusion

Our results suggest that optimal parenting of both parents is essential in social emotional development and an optimal parenting style of the father may moderate externalizing symptoms from pre-puberty to puberty. Future studies should replicate and further explore the role of parenting over an extended time period from childhood into young adulthood. Additionally, it will be important to explore the maternal and paternal parenting styles together. Previous studies found low levels of similarity between maternal and paternal parenting styles. Studies also showed that dissimilarity between parenting styles is an important factor in the development of socio-emotional problems in children and that more similarity in some parenting styles between parents is associated with more compliant behavior in children [4548]. Understanding the influence of different parenting styles on the child will have long-term benefits, as early intervention programs can help to improve parenting styles and as such may improve health outcomes for children.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Medical Research Council, NMRC/0975/2005 Singapore. All authors declare that the study sponsor had no role in the design or conduct of the study; in the collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of data; or in the preparation, review, or approval of the article. We thank the project coordinator, Ms. Su Shan Yeow, and other study staffs for their additional contributions.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Psychiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der psychiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Persson-Blennow I, Binett B, McNeil TF (1988) Offspring of women with nonorganic psychosis: antecedents of anxious attachment to the mother at one year of age. Acta Psychiatr Scand 78:66–71CrossRefPubMed Persson-Blennow I, Binett B, McNeil TF (1988) Offspring of women with nonorganic psychosis: antecedents of anxious attachment to the mother at one year of age. Acta Psychiatr Scand 78:66–71CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Rutter M (2010) Child and adolescent psychiatry: past scientific achievements and challenges for the future. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 19:689–703CrossRefPubMed Rutter M (2010) Child and adolescent psychiatry: past scientific achievements and challenges for the future. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 19:689–703CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Esbjørn BH, Pedersen SH, Daniel SI, Hald HH, Holm JM, Steele H (2013) Anxiety levels in clinically referred children and their parents: examining the unique influence of self-reported attachment styles and interview-based reflective functioning in mothers and fathers. Br J Clin Psychol 52:394–407CrossRefPubMed Esbjørn BH, Pedersen SH, Daniel SI, Hald HH, Holm JM, Steele H (2013) Anxiety levels in clinically referred children and their parents: examining the unique influence of self-reported attachment styles and interview-based reflective functioning in mothers and fathers. Br J Clin Psychol 52:394–407CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Canetti L, Kanyas K, Lerer B, Latzer Y, Bachar E (2008) Anorexia nervosa and parental bonding: the contribution of parent–grandparent relationships to eating disorder psychopathology. J Clin Psychol 64:703–716CrossRefPubMed Canetti L, Kanyas K, Lerer B, Latzer Y, Bachar E (2008) Anorexia nervosa and parental bonding: the contribution of parent–grandparent relationships to eating disorder psychopathology. J Clin Psychol 64:703–716CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Fuertes M, Santos PLD, Beeghly M, Tronick E (2006) More than maternal sensitivity shapes attachment. Ann NY Acad Sci 1094:292–296CrossRefPubMed Fuertes M, Santos PLD, Beeghly M, Tronick E (2006) More than maternal sensitivity shapes attachment. Ann NY Acad Sci 1094:292–296CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Tharner A, Herba CM, Luijk MP et al (2011) Subcortical structures and the neurobiology of infant attachment disorganization: a longitudinal ultrasound imaging study. Soc Neurosci 6:336–347CrossRefPubMed Tharner A, Herba CM, Luijk MP et al (2011) Subcortical structures and the neurobiology of infant attachment disorganization: a longitudinal ultrasound imaging study. Soc Neurosci 6:336–347CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Munson JA, McMahon RJ, Spieker SJ (2001) Structure and variability in the developmental trajectory of children’s externalizing problems: impact of infant attachment, maternal depressive symptomatology, and child sex. Dev Psychopathol 13:277–296CrossRefPubMed Munson JA, McMahon RJ, Spieker SJ (2001) Structure and variability in the developmental trajectory of children’s externalizing problems: impact of infant attachment, maternal depressive symptomatology, and child sex. Dev Psychopathol 13:277–296CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Côté S, Tremblay RE, Nagin D, Zoccolillo M, Vitaro F (2002) The development of impulsivity, fearfulness, and helpfulness during childhood: patterns of consistency and change in the trajectories of boys and girls. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 43:609–618CrossRefPubMed Côté S, Tremblay RE, Nagin D, Zoccolillo M, Vitaro F (2002) The development of impulsivity, fearfulness, and helpfulness during childhood: patterns of consistency and change in the trajectories of boys and girls. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 43:609–618CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Galéra C, Côté SM, Bouvard MP et al (2011) Early risk factors for hyperactivity impulsivity and inattention trajectories from age 17 months to 8 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68:1267–1275CrossRefPubMed Galéra C, Côté SM, Bouvard MP et al (2011) Early risk factors for hyperactivity impulsivity and inattention trajectories from age 17 months to 8 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68:1267–1275CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Bates JE, Pettit GS, Dodge KA, Ridge B (1998) Interaction of temperamental resistance to control and restrictive parenting in the development of externalizing behavior. Dev Psychol 34:982CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bates JE, Pettit GS, Dodge KA, Ridge B (1998) Interaction of temperamental resistance to control and restrictive parenting in the development of externalizing behavior. Dev Psychol 34:982CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Kiff CJ, Lengua LJ, Zalewski M (2011) Nature and nurturing: parenting in the context of child temperament. Clin Child Fam Psychol 14:251–301CrossRef Kiff CJ, Lengua LJ, Zalewski M (2011) Nature and nurturing: parenting in the context of child temperament. Clin Child Fam Psychol 14:251–301CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Kochanska G, Aksan N, Joy ME (2007) Children’s fearfulness as a moderator of parenting in early socialization: two longitudinal studies. Dev Psychol 43:222–237CrossRefPubMed Kochanska G, Aksan N, Joy ME (2007) Children’s fearfulness as a moderator of parenting in early socialization: two longitudinal studies. Dev Psychol 43:222–237CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Reitz E, Deković M, Meijer AM (2006) Relations between parenting and externalizing and internalizing problem behaviour in early adolescence: child behaviour as moderator and predictor. J Adolesc 29:419–436CrossRefPubMed Reitz E, Deković M, Meijer AM (2006) Relations between parenting and externalizing and internalizing problem behaviour in early adolescence: child behaviour as moderator and predictor. J Adolesc 29:419–436CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Skotheim S, Braarud HC, Høie K et al (2013) Subclinical levels of maternal depression and infant sensitivity to social contingency. Infant Behav Dev 36:419–426CrossRefPubMed Skotheim S, Braarud HC, Høie K et al (2013) Subclinical levels of maternal depression and infant sensitivity to social contingency. Infant Behav Dev 36:419–426CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Maccoby EE (2001) Parenting and its effects on children: on reading and misreading behavior genetics. Sci Ment Health Personal Personal Disord 51:201 Maccoby EE (2001) Parenting and its effects on children: on reading and misreading behavior genetics. Sci Ment Health Personal Personal Disord 51:201
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Parker G (1990) The parental bonding instrument. A decade of research. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 25:281–282CrossRefPubMed Parker G (1990) The parental bonding instrument. A decade of research. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 25:281–282CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Burbach DJ, Kashani JH, Rosenberg TK (1989) Parental bonding and depressive disorders in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 30:417–429CrossRefPubMed Burbach DJ, Kashani JH, Rosenberg TK (1989) Parental bonding and depressive disorders in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 30:417–429CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Overbeek G, ten Have M, Vollebergh W, de Graaf R (2007) Parental lack of care and overprotection. Longitudinal associations with DSM-III-R disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidmiol 42:87–93CrossRef Overbeek G, ten Have M, Vollebergh W, de Graaf R (2007) Parental lack of care and overprotection. Longitudinal associations with DSM-III-R disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidmiol 42:87–93CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Picardi A, Caroppo E, Fabi E, Proietti S, Di Gennaro G, Meldolesi GN, Martinotti G (2013) Attachment and parenting in adult patients with anxiety disorders. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health CP & EMH 9:157CrossRef Picardi A, Caroppo E, Fabi E, Proietti S, Di Gennaro G, Meldolesi GN, Martinotti G (2013) Attachment and parenting in adult patients with anxiety disorders. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health CP & EMH 9:157CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Sato T, Uehara T, Narita T, Sakado K, Fujii Y (2000) Parental bonding and personality in relation to a lifetime history of depression. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 54:121–130CrossRefPubMed Sato T, Uehara T, Narita T, Sakado K, Fujii Y (2000) Parental bonding and personality in relation to a lifetime history of depression. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 54:121–130CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Bornstein MH (2002) Handbook of parenting. In: Steinberg L, Silk JS, Bornstein MH (eds) Children and parenting, vol 1. Lawrence Erlbaum Associate, Mahwah, pp 3–43 Bornstein MH (2002) Handbook of parenting. In: Steinberg L, Silk JS, Bornstein MH (eds) Children and parenting, vol 1. Lawrence Erlbaum Associate, Mahwah, pp 3–43
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Bornstein MH (2007) Hue categorization and color naming: cognition to language to culture. In: MacLaury RE, Paramei GV, Dedrick D (eds) Anthropology of colour: interdisciplinary multilevel modeling. John Benjamins, Amsterdam/Philadelphia, pp 3–27CrossRef Bornstein MH (2007) Hue categorization and color naming: cognition to language to culture. In: MacLaury RE, Paramei GV, Dedrick D (eds) Anthropology of colour: interdisciplinary multilevel modeling. John Benjamins, Amsterdam/Philadelphia, pp 3–27CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Brotman MA, Schmajuk M, Rich BA et al (2006) Prevalence, clinical correlates, and longitudinal course of severe mood dysregulation in children. Biol Psychiatry 60:991–997CrossRefPubMed Brotman MA, Schmajuk M, Rich BA et al (2006) Prevalence, clinical correlates, and longitudinal course of severe mood dysregulation in children. Biol Psychiatry 60:991–997CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Davidov M, Grusec JE (2006) Untangling the links of parental responsiveness to distress and warmth to child outcomes. Child Dev 77:44–58CrossRefPubMed Davidov M, Grusec JE (2006) Untangling the links of parental responsiveness to distress and warmth to child outcomes. Child Dev 77:44–58CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J (2005) Differential susceptibility to rearing influence: an evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence. In: Ellis BJ, Bjorklund DF (eds) Origins of the social mind: evolutionary psychology and child development. Guilford Press, New York Belsky J (2005) Differential susceptibility to rearing influence: an evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence. In: Ellis BJ, Bjorklund DF (eds) Origins of the social mind: evolutionary psychology and child development. Guilford Press, New York
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Broekman BFP, Chan YH, Chong YS et al (2009) The influence of birth size on intelligence in healthy children. Pediatrics 123:e1011–e1016CrossRefPubMed Broekman BFP, Chan YH, Chong YS et al (2009) The influence of birth size on intelligence in healthy children. Pediatrics 123:e1011–e1016CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C (1991) The child behavior checklist and revised child behavior profile. University Associates in Psychiatry, Burlington Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C (1991) The child behavior checklist and revised child behavior profile. University Associates in Psychiatry, Burlington
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Achenbach TM, Rescorla L (2001) Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families, Burlington Achenbach TM, Rescorla L (2001) Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families, Burlington
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Parker G (1989) The parental bonding instrument: psychometric properties reviewed. Psychiatr Dev 7:317–335PubMed Parker G (1989) The parental bonding instrument: psychometric properties reviewed. Psychiatr Dev 7:317–335PubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Herz L, Gullone E (1999) The relationship between self-esteem and parenting style a cross-cultural comparison of Australian and Vietnamese Australian adolescents. J Cross Cult Psychol 30:742–761CrossRef Herz L, Gullone E (1999) The relationship between self-esteem and parenting style a cross-cultural comparison of Australian and Vietnamese Australian adolescents. J Cross Cult Psychol 30:742–761CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Easterbrooks M, Graham CA (1999) Security of attachment and parenting: homeless and low-income housed mothers and infants. Am J Orthopsychiatry 69:337–346CrossRefPubMed Easterbrooks M, Graham CA (1999) Security of attachment and parenting: homeless and low-income housed mothers and infants. Am J Orthopsychiatry 69:337–346CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Narusyte J, Neiderhiser JM, Andershed AK et al (2011) Parental criticism and externalizing behavior problems in adolescents: the role of environment and genotype–environment correlation. J Abnorm Psychol 120:365–376CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Narusyte J, Neiderhiser JM, Andershed AK et al (2011) Parental criticism and externalizing behavior problems in adolescents: the role of environment and genotype–environment correlation. J Abnorm Psychol 120:365–376CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J, Booth la Force C, Bradley RH et al (2008) Mothers’ and fathers’ support for child autonomy and early school achievement. Dev Psychol 44:895–907CrossRef Belsky J, Booth la Force C, Bradley RH et al (2008) Mothers’ and fathers’ support for child autonomy and early school achievement. Dev Psychol 44:895–907CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Lima AR, Mello MF, Andreoli SB et al (2014) The impact of healthy parenting as a protective factor for posttraumatic stress disorder in adulthood: a case-control study. PLoS One 9:e87117CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lima AR, Mello MF, Andreoli SB et al (2014) The impact of healthy parenting as a protective factor for posttraumatic stress disorder in adulthood: a case-control study. PLoS One 9:e87117CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams LR, Degnan KA, Perez-Edgar KE et al (2009) Impact of behavioral inhibition and parenting style on internalizing and externalizing problems from early childhood through adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol 37:1063–1075CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Williams LR, Degnan KA, Perez-Edgar KE et al (2009) Impact of behavioral inhibition and parenting style on internalizing and externalizing problems from early childhood through adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol 37:1063–1075CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Bornstein MH, Putnick DL (2007) Chronological age, cognitions, and practices in European American mothers: a multivariate study of parenting. Dev Psychol 43:850–864CrossRefPubMed Bornstein MH, Putnick DL (2007) Chronological age, cognitions, and practices in European American mothers: a multivariate study of parenting. Dev Psychol 43:850–864CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Leung C, Tsang S, Sin TCS, Choi S (2015) The efficacy of parent-child interaction therapy with Chinese families: randomized controlled trial. Res Social Work Pract 25:117–128CrossRef Leung C, Tsang S, Sin TCS, Choi S (2015) The efficacy of parent-child interaction therapy with Chinese families: randomized controlled trial. Res Social Work Pract 25:117–128CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Gardner F, Montgomery P, Knerr W (2015) Transporting evidence-based parenting programs for child problem behaviour (age 3–10) between countries: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 1:1–14 Gardner F, Montgomery P, Knerr W (2015) Transporting evidence-based parenting programs for child problem behaviour (age 3–10) between countries: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 1:1–14
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Avagianou PA, Zafiropoulou M (2008) Parental bonding and depression: personality as a mediating factor. Int J Adolesc Med Health 20:261–269CrossRefPubMed Avagianou PA, Zafiropoulou M (2008) Parental bonding and depression: personality as a mediating factor. Int J Adolesc Med Health 20:261–269CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Li Y, Costanzo PR, Putallaz M (2010) Maternal socialization goals, parenting styles, and social-emotional adjustment among Chinese and European American young adults: testing a mediation model. J Genet Psychol 171:330–362CrossRefPubMed Li Y, Costanzo PR, Putallaz M (2010) Maternal socialization goals, parenting styles, and social-emotional adjustment among Chinese and European American young adults: testing a mediation model. J Genet Psychol 171:330–362CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Pomerantz EM (2001) Parent x child socialization: implications for the development of depressive symptoms. J Fam Psychol 15:510–525CrossRefPubMed Pomerantz EM (2001) Parent x child socialization: implications for the development of depressive symptoms. J Fam Psychol 15:510–525CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Sessa FM, Avenevoli S, Steinberg L, Morris AS (2001) Correspondence among informants on parenting: preschool children, mothers, and observers. J Fam Psychol 15:53–68CrossRefPubMed Sessa FM, Avenevoli S, Steinberg L, Morris AS (2001) Correspondence among informants on parenting: preschool children, mothers, and observers. J Fam Psychol 15:53–68CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Erath SA, El-Sheikh M, Hinnant JB, Cummings EM (2011) Skin conductance level reactivity moderates the association between harsh parenting and growth in child externalizing behavior. Dev Psychol 47:693–706CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Erath SA, El-Sheikh M, Hinnant JB, Cummings EM (2011) Skin conductance level reactivity moderates the association between harsh parenting and growth in child externalizing behavior. Dev Psychol 47:693–706CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Caldera YM, Lindsey EW (2006) Coparenting, mother-infant interaction, and infant parent attachment relationships in two-parent families. J Fam Psychol 20:275–283CrossRefPubMed Caldera YM, Lindsey EW (2006) Coparenting, mother-infant interaction, and infant parent attachment relationships in two-parent families. J Fam Psychol 20:275–283CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Dwairy MA (2008) Parental inconsistency versus parental authoritarianism: associations with symptoms of psychological disorders. J Youth Adolesc 37:616–626CrossRef Dwairy MA (2008) Parental inconsistency versus parental authoritarianism: associations with symptoms of psychological disorders. J Youth Adolesc 37:616–626CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Dwairy M (2010) Parental inconsistency: a third cross-cultural research on parenting and psychological adjustment of children. J Child Fam Stud 19:23–29CrossRef Dwairy M (2010) Parental inconsistency: a third cross-cultural research on parenting and psychological adjustment of children. J Child Fam Stud 19:23–29CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Jaursch S, Loesel F, Beelmann A, Stemmler M (2009) Inconsistency in parenting between mothers and fathers and children’s behaviour problems. Psychol Erzieh Unterr 56:172–186 Jaursch S, Loesel F, Beelmann A, Stemmler M (2009) Inconsistency in parenting between mothers and fathers and children’s behaviour problems. Psychol Erzieh Unterr 56:172–186
Metadaten
Titel
The influence of perceived parenting styles on socio-emotional development from pre-puberty into puberty
verfasst von
Min Yee Ong
Janna Eilander
Seang Mei Saw
Yuhuan Xie
Michael J. Meaney
Birit F. P. Broekman
Publikationsdatum
19.06.2017
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2018
Print ISSN: 1018-8827
Elektronische ISSN: 1435-165X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-017-1016-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2018

European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 1/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.