Skip to main content
Log in

The Future is Bright? Effects of Mood on Perception of the Future

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Most people believe that the future will bring them more good things than bad, and therefore have high hopes for the future (MacLeod et al. Cogn Emot 10:69–85, 1996). However, many patients with mood disorders do not hold this positive belief about the future. At the extreme, low expectations of positive outcomes in the future can lead to feelings of hopelessness (O’Connor et al. Psychol Health Med 5:155–161, 2000). This paper aims to extend the literature on subjective probability of future events, using a mood induction paradigm to examine the effects of transient mood change on perceived likelihood of future events in a non-clinical community sample. Participants rated likelihood of future events from a standardized list and from their own lives. Ratings were made in both normal and experimentally-induced positive or negative mood. Results show that self-generated future events were perceived to be more likely than those from a standardized list, and that negative mood significantly biased perceived likelihood of other-generated future events. Participants rating standardized list events saw positive outcomes as less likely and negative outcomes as more likely in induced negative mood than they did in normal mood. Mood had no effect on ratings of self-generated events. Possible directions for future research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The terms “affect” and “mood” are used interchangeably by Watson and colleagues, though some psychologists prefer to distinguish them, with affect referring to the behavioural expression of a mood, rather than the mood itself.

References

  • Andersen, S. M., & Limpert, C. (2001). Future-event schemas: Automaticity and rumination in major depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25(3), 311–333. doi:10.1023/A:1026447600924.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, S. M., Spielman, L. A., & Bargh, J. A. (1992). Future-event schemas and certainty about the future: Automaticity in depressives' future-event predictions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 711–723. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.5.711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). BDI-II manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck depression inventory: 25 years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Weissman, A. W., Lester, D., & Trexler, L. (1979). Cognitive therapy for depression. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buehler, R., & McFarland, C. (2001). Intensity bias in affective forecasting: The role of temporal focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1480–1493. doi:10.1177/01461672012711009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, G., & Mathews, A. (1983). Cognitive processes in anxiety. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5, 51–62. doi:10.1016/0146-6402(83)90015-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. M. (1983). On the induction of depressed mood in the laboratory: Evaluation and comparison of the Velten and musical procedures. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5, 27–49. doi:10.1016/0146-6402(83)90014-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conaghan, S., & Davidson, K. M. (2002). Hopelessness and the anticipation of positive and negative future experiences in older parasuicidal adults. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41(3), 233–242. doi:10.1348/014466502760379208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is coefficient alphas? An examination of theory and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 98–104. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.78.1.98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hepburn, S. R., Barnhofer, T., & Williams, J. M. G. (2006). Effects of mood on how future events are generated and perceived. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 801–811. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.03.022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huppert, F. A., & Whittington, J. E. (2003). Evidence for the independence of positive and negative well-being: Implications for quality of life assessment. British Journal of Health Psychology, 8(1), 107–122. doi:10.1348/135910703762879246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, E. C., & O'Connor, R. C. (2003). Hopelessness and future thinking in parasuicide: The role of perfectionism. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 355–365. doi:10.1348/014466503322528900.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, E. J., & Tversky, A. (1983). Affect generalization and the perception of risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 20–31. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.45.1.20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Siemer, M. (2004). Memory accessibility, mood regulation and dysphoria: Difficulties in repairing sad mood with happy memories? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 179–188. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.113.2.179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, A. K. (1999). Prospective cognitions. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, A. K., Byrne, A., & Valentine, J. D. (1996). Affect, emotional disorder, and future-directed thinking. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 69–85. doi:10.1080/026999396380394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., & Campbell, L. (1992). Memory accessibility and probability judgments: An experimental evaluation of the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 478–486.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, A. K., & Cropley, M. L. (1995). Depressive future-thinking: The role of valence and specificity. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 35–50. doi:10.1007/BF02229675.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, A. K., & Moore, R. (2000). Positive thinking revisited: Positive cognitions, well-being and mental health. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 7(1), 1–10. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0879(200002)7:1<1::AID-CPP228>3.0.CO;2-S.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, A. K., Pankhania, B., Lee, M., & Mitchell, D. (1997). Parasuicide, depression and the anticipation of positive and negative experiences. Psychological Medicine, 27, 973–977. doi:10.1017/S003329179600459X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, A. K., Rose, G., & Williams, J. M. G. (1993). Components of hopelessness about the future in parasuicide. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 17, 441–455. doi:10.1007/BF01173056.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, A. K., & Tarbuck, A. F. (1994). Explaining why negative events will happen to oneself: Parasuicides are pessimistic because they can't see any reason not to be. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 317–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, A. K., Tata, P., Kentish, J., Carroll, F., & Hunter, E. (1997). Anxiety, depression, and explanation-based pessimism for future positive and negative events. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 4, 15–24. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0879(199703)4:1<15::AID-CPP112>3.0.CO;2-#.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, A. K., Williams, J. M. G., & Bekerian, D. A. (1991). Worry is reasonable: The role of explanations in pessimism about future personal events. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 478–486. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newby-Clark, I. R., & Ross, M. (2003). Conceiving the past and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(7), 807–818. doi:10.1177/0146167203029007001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O'Connor, R. C., Connery, H., & Cheyne, W. M. (2000). Hopelessness: The role of depression, future directed thinking and cognitive vulnerability. Psychology, Health and Medicine, 5, 155–161. doi:10.1080/713690188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pietromonaco, P., & Markus, H. (1985). The nature of negative thoughts in depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 799–807. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.48.3.799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pysczcynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and the depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reactive depression. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122–138. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.102.1.122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rusting, C. L., & DeHart, T. (2000). Retrieving positive memories to regulate negative mood: Consequences for mood-congruent memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 737–752. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. (2003). Mood as information: 20 Years later. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 296–303. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1403&4_20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R., & Lomax Cook, F. (1984). The mass media and judgment of risk: Distinguishing impact on personal and societal levels of judgement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 693–708. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.47.4.693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 6, 473–482. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(68)90028-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., & Hesse, F. W. (1998). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 557–580. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199607)26:4<557::AID-EJSP769>3.0.CO;2-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelenski, J. M., & Larsen, R. J. (2002). Predicting the future: How affect-related personality traits influence likelihood judgments of future events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(7), 1000–1010. doi:10.1177/01467202028007012.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a Wellcome Trust Prize Studentship to Silvia Hepburn. Thanks to Catherine Crane, Danielle Duggan, Melanie Fennell and Wendy Swift.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Silvia R. Hepburn.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hepburn, S.R., Barnhofer, T. & Williams, J.M.G. The Future is Bright? Effects of Mood on Perception of the Future. J Happiness Stud 10, 483–496 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-008-9102-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-008-9102-9

Keywords

Navigation