Skip to main content
Log in

Ruminations and Flow: Why Do People with a More Harmonious Passion Experience Higher Well-Being?

  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research shows that harmonious and obsessive passion are positively and negatively linked to well-being respectively (e.g., Vallerand et al. in J Personal 75:505–534, 2007; Psychol Sport Exerc 9:373–392, 2008). The present study investigated the psychological mechanisms underlying the different impact of the two types of passion on well-being. A theoretical model involving passion, ruminations, flow experiences, and well-being was tested. Results showed that the more people have a harmonious passion, the more they tend to experience flow in their favorite activity, which in turn predicts higher well-being. Obsessive passion did not seem to be systematically linked to flow in the favorite activity. In contrast, the more people have an obsessive passion, the more they tend to ruminate about their passionate activity while engaging in another activity, which did not seem to be systematically the case for people with a harmonious passion. These ruminations are negatively related to flow experiences in the other activity, which are positively associated with well-being. Flow and ruminations thus seem to contribute to the understanding of the link between passion and well-being.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Research shows that passion can be distinguished from other motivational constructs such as intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. From a theoretical perspective (Vallerand 2010), passionate activities differ from intrinsically motivated ones in that they are deeply valued by the individual and they are internalized into the person’s identity. This is not necessarily the case for intrinsically motivated activities. The concept of passion (both harmonious and obsessive) also differs from extrinsic forms of motivation because, contrary to extrinsic motivation (which means to engage in an activity to obtain something outside of the activity), passion implies a strong liking for the activity. Because passion entails both an identification to and a strong liking for an activity, it should have a more profound impact on people’s lives than motivation or goals. Empirical evidence confirms that when motivation and passion are assessed toward a given activity, harmonious and obsessive passion, respectively predict positive and negative affects above and beyond what is predicted by intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (Vallerand et al. 2003, Study 2).

  2. The χ 2M tests for differences between the estimated and observed correlation matrices, such that a non-significant p value supports the adequacy of the proposed model. The NC, which is the ratio of the chi-square statistic on its degrees of freedom, takes into account the sample size and is thus usually a better fit index than the χ 2M . Values smaller than 2.0 for this index indicate a good fit (Tabachnick and Fidell 2007). The CFI, NFI, and NNFI are comparative or incremental fit indices that assess fit relative to other models (Kline 2005). Values greater than .95 on the CFI, the NFI and the NNFI are indicative of a good-fitting model (Tabachnick and Fidell 2007). The NNFI is adjusted for model complexity and can yield values greater than 1.00. The RMSEA is a parsimony-adjusted index which corrects for model complexity (Kline 2005). Browne and Cudeck (1993) suggest that RMSEAs less than .05 are indicative of a “close fit” and that values up to .08 represent reasonable errors of approximation. Finally, SRMR is a measure of the mean absolute correlation residual (i.e., the mean difference between observed and predicted covariances) and should be less than .10 to indicate a reasonable fit (Kline 2005).

  3. Specifically, correlations between passion and the other variables are expected to differ from the relations found using SEM because SEM controls for the common variance typically found between the two types of passion (e.g., Philippe et al. 2010; Vallerand et al. 2003). By controlling for this positive correlation, relations between each type of passion and the dependent variable can be investigated independently from the effect of the other type of passion. The two types of passion are expected to correlate positively because as types of passion they share common elements. Indeed, both types of passion refer to a unique relationship with an activity that a person has internalized in his or her identity. In addition, whether more obsessively or harmoniously passionate, people equally report liking their activity, finding it important, and devoting time to it (for a similar analytical strategy, see Carbonneau et al. 2010; Mageau et al. 2005; Philippe et al. 2009a, c; Rousseau and Vallerand 2008). The correlation between harmonious and obsessive passion observed in the present study is similar to the one found in previous research (e.g., Philippe et al. 2010; Vallerand et al. 2003).

  4. Because past research shows that men and women often react differently in the school context (Cai 2005; Else-Quest et al. 2010; Ewert 2010; Fan 2011; Tison et al. 2011), we replicated our final model while controlling for the impact of gender on all endogenous variables. Results showed that gender was not significantly linked to the model’s variables and that all significant relations were observed above and beyond any gender differences. For the sake of parsimony, the model is presented without controlling for gender.

References

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychology research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588–606. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M., & Wu, E. J. W. (2004). EQS 6.1 for Windows. Multivariate Software, Inc.

  • Blais, M. R., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Brière, N. M. (1989). L’échelle de satisfaction de vie: Validation canadienne-française du ‘‘Satisfaction With Life Scale’’ [French-Canadian Validation of the Satisfaction With Life Scale]. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Sciences, 21, 210–223. doi:10.1037/h0079854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloch, C. (2002). Moods and quality of life. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3, 101–128. doi:10.1023/A:1019647818216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonneville-Roussy, A., Lavigne, G. L., & Vallerand, R. J. (2011). When passion leads to excellence: The case of musicians. Psychology of Music, 39, 123–138. doi:10.1177/0305735609352441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. Bollen & R. Stine (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cai, X. (2005). Stability of externalizing problem behaviors with onset in early childhood: A meta-analytic review. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, United States.

  • Cantor, N., & Sanderson, C. A. (1999). Life task participation and well-being: The importance of taking part in daily life. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundation of hedonic psychology (pp. 230–243). New York: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carbonneau, N., Vallerand, R. J., & Massicotte, S. (2010). Is the practice of Yoga associated with positive outcomes? The role of passion. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 452–465. doi:10.1080/17439760.2010.534107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S. (Ed.). (1996). Goal engagement and the human experience (Vol. 9). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, S. G., & Haworth, J. T. (1994). ‘‘Flow’’ experience in the daily lives of sixth form college students. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 511–523. Retrieved from http://pao.chadwyck.co.uk/PDF/1281966966867.pdf.

  • Conway, M., Csank, P. A. R., Holm, S. L., & Blake, C. K. (2000). On assessing individual differences in rumination on sadness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 75, 404–425. doi:10.1207/S15327752JPA7503_04.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1982). Toward a psychology of optimal experience. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (pp. 13–36). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The flow experience and its significance for human psychology. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 15–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow: The psychology of engagement with everyday life. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). If we are so rich, why aren’t we happy? American Psychologist, 54, 821–827. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.10.821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delle Fave, A., & Massimini, F. (2004). Bringing subjectivity into focus: Optimal experiences, life themes and person-centered rehabilitation. In P. A. Linley & S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive psychology in practice (pp. 581–597). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research, 31, 103–157. doi:10.1007/BF01207052.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–76. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberger, R., Jones, J. R., Stinglhamber, F., Shanock, L., & Randall, A. T. (2005). Flow experiences at work: For high need achievers alone? Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 755–775. doi:10.1002/job.337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-national patterns of gender differences in mathematics: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 103–127. doi:10.1037/a0018053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ewert, S. (2010). Male and female pathways through four-year colleges: Disruption and sex stratification in higher education. American Educational Research Journal, 47, 744–773. doi:10.3102/0002831210374351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, W. (2011). Social influences, school motivation and gender differences: An application of the expectancy-value theory. Educational Psychology, 31, 157–175. doi:10.1080/01443410.2010.536525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forest, J., Sarrazin, C., Morin, E. M., Brunet, L., Savoie, A., & Mageau, G. A. (2008). La passion harmonieuse et la passion obsessive comme variables prévisionnelles de la santé psychologique et de l’état psychologique optimal de flow au travail: Résultats d’une étude empirique réalisée auprès de 118 travailleurs québécois (Harmonious and obsessive passion as predictors of mental health and the optimal psychological state of flow at work: Results from an empirical study with 118 Quebec workers). In A. Balikdjian, C. Lemoine, N. Kridis, & P. Salengros (Eds.), Actes du colloque du 14ème congrès de l’Association Internationale de Psychologie du Travail de Langue Française (pp. 43–54). Paris: L’Harmattan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, P. A., Tix, A. P., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Testing moderator and mediator effects in counseling psychology research. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 115–134. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.51.1.115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Han, S. (1988). The relationship between life satisfaction and flow in elderly Korean immigrants. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 138–149). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haworth, J. T. (1997). Work, leisure, and well-being. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haworth, J. T., & Evans, S. (1995). Challenge, skill, and positive subjective states in the daily life of a sample of YTS students. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 68, 109–121. Retrieved from http://content.ebscohost.com/pdf17_20/pdf/1995/OOP/01Jun95/9510086532.pdf.

  • Haworth, J. T., & Hill, S. (1992). Work, leisure and psychological well-being in a sample of young adults. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 2, 147–160. doi:10.1002/casp.2450020210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgins, H. S., & Knee, C. R. (2002). The integrating self and conscious experience. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 87–100). Rochester, NY: The University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in the study of mediators and moderators: Examples from the child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 599–610. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.65.4.599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, S. A., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: The Flow State Scale. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 18, 17–35. Retrieved from http://journals.humankinetics.com/jsep-back-issues/JSEPVolume18Issue1March/DevelopmentandValidationofaScaletoMeasureOptimalExperienceTheFlowStateScale.

  • Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010) A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932. doi:10.1126/science.1192439.

  • Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences to commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 223–231. doi:10.1037/h0076171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the incentives in people’s lives. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubovy, M. (1999). On the pleasures of the mind. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 134–154). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Tkach, C. (2004). The consequences of dysphoric rumination. In C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination: Nature, theory, and treatment (pp. 21–41). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mageau, G. A., Carpentier, J., & Vallerand, R. J. (in press). The role of self-esteem contingencies in the distinction between obsessive and harmonious passion. European Journal of Social Psychology.

  • Mageau, G. A., & Vallerand, R. J. (2007). The moderating effect of passion on the relation between activity engagement and positive affect. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 312–321. doi:10.1007/s11031-007-9071-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mageau, G. A., Vallerand, R. J., Charest, J., Salvy, S., Lacaille, N., Bouffard, T., et al. (2009). On the development of harmonious and obsessive passion: The role of autonomy support, activity valuation, and identity processes. Journal of Personality, 77, 601–645. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00559.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mageau, G. A., Vallerand, R. J., Rousseau, F. L., Ratelle, C. F., & Provencher, P. J. (2005). Passion and gambling: Investigating the divergent affective and cognitive consequences of gambling. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 100–118. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02095.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1989). Toward a motivational and structural theory of ruminative thought. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 306–326). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In R. S. Wyer Jr (Ed.), Advances in Social Cognition (Vol. 9, pp. 1–47). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ministère de l’Éducation, du Loisir et du Sport du Québec. (2007). Poursuite des études et obtention d’un diplôme chez les bénéficiaires du Programme de prêts et bourses. Report prepared by la Direction de la recherche, des statistiques et des indicateurs (DRSI) et le secteur de l’Aide financière aux études (AFE), Ministère de l’Éducation, du Loisir et du Sport. http://www.mels.gouv.qc.ca/stat/recherche/doc07/PoursuiteEtudesObtDipVolet3.pdf [Consulted on March 7, 2011].

  • Mundell, C. E. (2000). The role of perceived skill, perceived challenge, and flow in the experience of positive and negative affect. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 61, 2802.

  • Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). The concept of flow. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 89–105). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philippe, F. L., Vallerand, R. J., Andrianarisoa, J., & Brunel, P. (2009a). Passion in referees: Examining their affective and cognitive experiences in sport situations. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 31, 77–96. Retrieved from http://journals.humankinetics.com/JSEP.

  • Philippe, F. L., Vallerand, R. J., Houlfort, N., Lavigne, G., & Donahue, E. G. (2010). Passion for an activity and quality of interpersonal relationships: The mediating role of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 917–932. doi:10.1037/a0018017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Philippe, F. L., Vallerand, R. J., & Lavigne, G. (2009b). Passion makes a difference in people’s lives: A look at well-being in passionate and non-passionate individuals. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 1, 3–22. doi:10.1111/j.1758-0854.2008.01003.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Philippe, F. L., Vallerand, R. J., Richer, I., Vallières, E., & Bergeron, J. (2009c). Passion for driving and aggressive driving behavior: A look at their relationship. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39, 3020–3043. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00559.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratelle, C. F., Mageau, G. A., Vallerand, R. J., & Provencher, P. (2011). The role of absolute and relative importance in the development of passion. (in preparation).

  • Ratelle, C. F., Vallerand, R. J., Mageau, G. A., Rousseau, F. L., & Provencher, P. (2004). When passion leads to problematic outcomes: A look at gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 20, 105–119. doi:1050-5350/04/0600-0105/0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rip, B., Fortin, S., & Vallerand, R. J. (2006). The relationship between passion and injury in dance students. Journal of Dance Medicine & Science, 10, 14–20. Retrieved from http://www.er.uqam.ca/nobel/r26710/LRCS/papers/145.pdf.

  • Rousseau, F. L., & Vallerand, R. J. (2003). Le rôle de la passion dans le bien-être subjectif des aînés [The role of passion in the subjective well-being of elderly individuals]. Revue Québécoise de Psychologie, 24, 197–211. Retrieved from http://www.er.uqam.ca/nobel/r26710/LRCS/papers/121.pdf.

  • Rousseau, F. L., & Vallerand, R. J. (2008). An examination of the relationship between passion and subjective well-being in older adults. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 66, 195–211. doi:10.2190/AG.66.3.b.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlenker, B. R. (1985). Identity and self-identification. In B. R. Schlenker (Ed.), The self and social life (pp. 65–99). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Séguin-Lévesque, C., Laliberté, M.-L. N., Pelletier, L. G., Blanchard, C., & Vallerand, R. J. (2003). Harmonious and obsessive passion for the Internet: Their associations with the couple’s relationship. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 197–221. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb02079.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55, 5–14. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.55.1.5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, J. P., & Fullagar, C. J. (2009). Facilitators and outcomes of student engagement in a college setting. The Journal of Psychology, 143, 5–27. doi:10.3200/JRLP.143.1.5-27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). New York: Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tison, E. B., Bateman, T., & Culver, S. M. (2011). Examination of the gender-student engagement relationship at one university. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education, 36, 27–49. doi:10.1080/02602930903197875.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J. (2010). On passion for life activities: The dualistic model of passion. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J., Blanchard, C., Mageau, G. A., Koestner, R., Ratelle, C. F., Léonard, M., et al. (2003). Les passions de l’âme: On obsessive and harmonious passion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 756–767. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J., Mageau, G. A., Elliot, A. J., Dumais, A., Demers, M.-A., & Rousseau, F. L. (2008). Passion and performance attainment in sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 373–392. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2007.05.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallerand, R. J., Salvy, S. J., Mageau, G. A., Elliot, A. J., Denis, P., Grouzet, F. M. E., et al. (2007). On the role of passion in performance. Journal of Personality, 75, 505–534. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00447.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeigarnik, B. (Ed.). (1938). On finished and unfinished tasks. New York: Harcourt, Brace, & World.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was facilitated by masters and doctoral fellowships from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) to the first and second author, and from the Fonds de Recherche sur la Société et la Culture (FQRSC) to the first author. It was also funded by grants from SSHRC and the FQRSC to the third author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Joëlle Carpentier or Geneviève A. Mageau.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Carpentier, J., Mageau, G.A. & Vallerand, R.J. Ruminations and Flow: Why Do People with a More Harmonious Passion Experience Higher Well-Being?. J Happiness Stud 13, 501–518 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-011-9276-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-011-9276-4

Keywords

Navigation