Skip to main content
Log in

Nonverbal cues-based first impressions: Impression formation through exposure to static images

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Consumers often make judgments about service providers based solely on their photograph (e.g., on a billboard, on the Internet) before interacting with them face-to-face. This first impression is important for marketers to understand because it influences service provider selection and evaluation. This research explores the nature and subsequent influence of initial judgments made from static images. The results of two studies indicate that (1) judgments based on nonverbal cues can be surprisingly accurate, particularly of personality traits relevant to the domain in which the target is being judged, and (2) initial nonverbal cues-based judgments influence subsequent judgments of a target’s actions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The extant research on static images has focused on judgments of personality and character traits (e.g., honesty, intelligence, health, etc.). Our research is the first, to our knowledge, to examine the accuracy of judgments of an unknown target’s job performance made on the basis of static images.

  2. The photographs used in this research contain physical appearance cues (of which general physical attractiveness is one element) and static representations of some kinesic cues (facial expression and eye gaze).

  3. All photographs and performance measures used were obtained with permission from the targets.

  4. To ensure true thin-slice judgments, participants in each study were also asked if they had ever seen or met the target before the study. No participants indicated that they had seen or met the targets before.

  5. The reader may note that some relatively large correlations in Tables 1 and 2 are not statistically significant. This is because, following Ambady and Rosenthal (1993), targets are used as the unit of analysis rather than judges (participants). Because correlations were computed across judges, study 1A has an n of five targets (financial consultants) and study 1B an n of six targets (professors) vs an n corresponding to the number of participants. This analysis resulted in three degrees of freedom in the financial consultant study and four degrees of freedom in the professor study. The critical value for r at three degrees of freedom (at the p < 0.05 level) is a correlation of 0.878. The critical value for r at four degrees of freedom (at the p < 0.05 level) is .811.

  6. Both Cohen’s d (reported in the text) and r 2 (reported in Tables 1 and 2) are indicators of the large effect sizes found in both studies 1A and 1B.

  7. Although the paragraph was originally used by Srull and Wyer (1979) in a category accessibility study, we elected to use it here because it was designed to be ambiguous in regards to a specific personality trait. Because the target’s purported actions in the paragraph can be interpreted as either hostile or not, judgments made by participants of the target’s hostility are primarily influenced by the perceivers’ initial nonverbal cues-based impression rather than by cues in the written paragraph.

References

  • Albright, L., Kenny, D. A., & Malloy, T. E. (1988). Consensus in personality judgments at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 387–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. (2000). Toward a histology of social behavior: Judgment accuracy from thin slices of the behavioral stream. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 32 (pp. 201–271) San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Rosenthal, R. (1995). On judging and being judged accurately in zero-acquaintance situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(3), 518–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1993). Half a minute: Predicting teacher evaluations from thin slices of nonverbal behavior and physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 431–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming Impressions of personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 258–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, D. S., & Brownlow, S. (1989). Were the Physiognomists right? Personality correlates of facial babyishness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 266–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, D. S., & Finch Wero, J. L. (1993). Accuracy in face perception: A view from ecological psychology. Journal of Personality, 61, 497–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, D. S., & McArthur, L. Z. (1986). Perceiving character in faces: The impact of age-related craniofacial changes on social perception. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 3–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonoma, T. V., & Felder, L. C. (1977). Nonverbal communication in marketing: Toward a communicational analysis. Journal of Marketing Research, 14, 169–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., & Woodall, W. G. (1996). Nonverbal communication: The unspoken dialogue. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoch, S. J., & Ha, Y.-W. (1986). Consumer learning: Advertising and the ambiguity of product experience. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 221–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulbert, J., & Capon, N. (1972). Interpersonal communication in marketing: An overview. Journal of Marketing Research, 9, 27–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knapp, M. L., & Hall, J. A. (2005). Nonverbal communication in human interaction. Belmont: Wadsworth Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, S. J., & Bass, A. R. (1985). A direct experiment of halo effect. Journal of Psychology, 119, 535–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, T. W., & Summers, J. O. (2002). An initial evaluation of industrial buyers’ impressions of salespersons’ nonverbal cues. Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 22, 41–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). The halo effect: Evidence for the unconscious alteration of judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 450–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S. Jr., & Duke, M. P. (2000). Nonverbal receptivity: The diagnostic analysis of nonverbal accuracy (DANVA). In J. A. Hall & F. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement. Mahawah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunnaly, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riggio, R. E., & Friedman, H. S. (1986). Impression formation: The role of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 421–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom. New York: Hold, Rinehart, and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schul, P. L., & Lamb, C. W. (1982). Decoding nonverbal and vocal communications: A laboratory study. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 10, 154–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 656–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Srull, T. K., & Wyer R. S. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1660–1672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, D. W., Hecker, S., & Graham, J. L. (1987). It’s more than what you say: Assessing the influences of nonverbal communication in marketing. Psychology and Marketing, 4, 303–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1920). A constant error on psychological rating. Journal of Applied Psychology, 4, 25–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1963). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph, 9(2), 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35(2), 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B., & Markus, H. (1982). Affective and cognitive factors in preferences. Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 123–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zebrowitz, L. A., Hall, J. A., Murphy, N. A., & Rhodes, G. (2002). Looking smart and looking good: Facial cues to intelligence and their origins. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(2), 238–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zebrowitz, L. A., & Rhodes, G. (2004). Sensitivity to ‘bad genes’ and the anomalous face overgeneralization effect: Cue validity, cue utilization, and accuracy in judging intelligence and health. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(3), 167–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

The author thanks Raj Raghunathan and Julie Irwin for their guidance throughout the process of conducting this research and Susan Broniarczyk, Andrea Godfrey, Kelly Haws, Wayne Hoyer, and Robert Peterson for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of the paper. The paper was written while the author was a doctoral student at the University of Texas at Austin.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca Walker Naylor.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Walker Naylor, R. Nonverbal cues-based first impressions: Impression formation through exposure to static images. Market Lett 18, 165–179 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-007-9010-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-007-9010-5

Keywords

Navigation