Skip to main content
Log in

Approach, Avoidance, and the Self-Regulation of Affect and Action

  • Original paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Several literatures converge on the idea that approach and avoidance/withdrawal behaviors are managed by two partially distinct self-regulatory system. The functions of these systems also appear to be embodied in discrepancy-reducing and -enlarging feedback loops, respectively. This article describes how the feedback construct has been used to address these two classes of action and the affective experiences that relate to them. Further discussion centers on the development of measures of individual differences in approach and avoidance tendencies, and how these measures can be (and have been) used as research tools, to investigate whether other phenomena have their roots in approach or avoidance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1999). The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 839–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Development, 75, 377–394.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S. (2003). Pleasure as a sign you can attend to something else: Placing positive feelings within a general model of affect. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 241–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S. (2004). Negative affects deriving from the behavioral approach system. Emotion, 4, 3–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., Lawrence, J. W., & Scheier, M. F. (1999). Self-discrepancies and affect: Incorporating the role of feared selves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 783–792.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1999). Themes and issues in the self-regulation of behavior. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 12). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Autonomy and self-regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 284–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (in press). Self-regulatory systems: Action and affect. In J. Y. Shah & W. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation science. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., Sutton, S. K., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, and personality: Emerging conceptual integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 741–751.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, R. N. (1996). Control system dynamics. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method of clinical description and classification of personality variants: A proposal. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 573–588.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (1984). Affect, cognition, and hemispheric specialization. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotion, cognition, and behavior (pp. 320–365). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (1988). EEG measures of cerebral asymmetry: Conceptual and methodological issues. International Journal of Neuroscience, 39, 71–89.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (1995). Cerebral asymmetry, emotion, and affective style. In R. J. Davidson, & K. Hugdahl (Eds.), Brain asymmetry (pp. 361–387). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 307–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–517.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Depue, R. A., & Iacono, W. G. (1989). Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 457–492.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Depue, R. A., Krauss, S. P., & Spoont, M. R. (1987). A two-dimensional threshold model of seasonal bipolar affective disorder. In D. Magnusson & A. Öhman (Eds.), Psychopathology: An interactional perspective (pp. 95–123). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach–avoidance motivation in personality: Approach and avoidance temperaments and goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 804–818.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fowles, D. C. (1980). The three arousal model: Implications of Gray’s two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity, and psychopathy. Psychophysiology, 17, 87–104.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, 349–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1994). Emotions are functional, most of the time. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 112–126). New York: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck\(^{\prime}\)’s theory of personality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for personality (pp. 246–276). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1987). Perspectives on anxiety and impulsivity: A commentary. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 493–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 269–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1994a). Personality dimensions and emotion systems. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 329–331). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1994b). Three fundamental emotion systems. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 243–247). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1996). Ideals, oughts, and regulatory focus: Affect and motivation from distinct pains and pleasures. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 91–114). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. L., & Carver, C. S. (in press). Extreme goal setting and vulnerability to mania among undiagnosed young adults. Cognitive Therapy and Research.

  • Johnson, S. L., Turner, R. J., & Iwata, N. (2003). BIS/BAS levels and psychiatric disorder: An epidemiological study. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 25, 25–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J. (1948). Conditioned reflexes and neuron organization. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1998). Emotion, motivation, and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiololgy. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 1248–1263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levenson, R. W. (1994). Human emotion: A functional view. In P. Ekman & R. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotions: Fundamental questions (pp. 123–126). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954–969.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B., Johnson, S. L., & Carver, C. S. (1999). Exploring behavioral activation and inhibition sensitivities among college students at risk for bipolar spectrum symptomatology. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 21, 275–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B., Johnson, S. L., & Winters, R. (2001). Responsiveness to threat and incentive in bipolar disorder: Relations of the BIS/BAS scales with symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23, 133–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. (1944). Experimental studies of conflict. In J. McV. Hunt (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders (Vol. 1, pp. 431–465). New York: Ronald Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. (1941). Social learning and imitation. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogilvie, D. M. (1987). The undesired self: A neglected variable in personality research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 379–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotions: A structural theory. In P. Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 11–36). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 3–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1999). Approaching and avoiding self-determination: Comparing cybernetic and organismic paradigms of motivation. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 12, pp. 193–215). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneirla, T. C. (1959). An evolutionary and developmental theory of biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol.7, pp. 1–42). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, B. J. (1975). Individual differences as a crucible in theory construction. American Psychologist, 30, 128–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 820–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelenski, J. M., & Larsen, R. J. (1999). Susceptibility to affect: A comparison of three personality taxonomies. Journal of Personality, 67, 761–791.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. (2005). Psychobiology of personality (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Charles S. Carver.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Carver, C.S. Approach, Avoidance, and the Self-Regulation of Affect and Action. Motiv Emot 30, 105–110 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9044-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9044-7

Keywords

Navigation