Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Neuropsychology Review 2/2014

01.06.2014 | Review

What Neuropsychology Tells us About Human Tool Use? The Four Constraints Theory (4CT): Mechanics, Space, Time, and Effort

verfasst von: François Osiurak

Erschienen in: Neuropsychology Review | Ausgabe 2/2014

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Abstract

Our understanding of human tool use comes mainly from neuropsychology, particularly from patients with apraxia or action disorganization syndrome. However, there is no integrative, theoretical framework explaining what these neuropsychological syndromes tell us about the cognitive/neural bases of human tool use. The goal of the present article is to fill this gap, by providing a theoretical framework for the study of human tool use: The Four Constraints Theory (4CT). This theory rests on two basic assumptions. First, everyday tool use activities can be formalized as multiple problem situations consisted of four distinct constraints (mechanics, space, time, and effort). Second, each of these constraints can be solved by the means of a specific process (technical reasoning, semantic reasoning, working memory, and simulation-based decision-making, respectively). Besides presenting neuropsychological evidence for 4CT, this article shall address epistemological, theoretical and methodological issues I will attempt to resolve. This article will discuss how 4CT diverges from current cognitive models about several widespread hypotheses (e.g., notion of routine, direct and automatic activation of tool knowledge, simulation-based tool knowledge).
Fußnoten
1
Some studies have assessed the apraxic patients’ ability to use several tools and objects together such as preparing instant coffee (multiple object task; e.g., De Renzi and Lucchelli 1988). These tasks are now considered as assessing the action disorganization syndrome because of the presence of several steps to achieve the goal (see below).
 
2
The notion of “space constraint” stresses the problem that tools and objects are not always here now, that is, directly present to our senses (e.g., they are not visible because they are in another room). In this way, the emphasis is put on the problem of the absence. This differs dramatically from the perspective where the space is divided in function of the individual’s capacities (e.g., reaching space). In this way, it has been shown that tool use can extend our reaching space (e.g., Farnè and Làdavas 2000; Cardinalli et al. 2009; Iriki et al. 1996). Nevertheless, even if an object is out of reach, this object can still be present to the senses, if it is visible, for example.
 
3
Several studies have revealed serious limitations on the ability of nonhuman animals to solve tool-use situations that are relatively simple for humans (e.g., Povinelli 2000; Santos et al. 2006; Visalberghi and Limongelli 1994). In line with this, it has been suggested that tool use in animals is not based on relational representations of the underlying generative mechanisms involved, but rather on “concrete” representations learned by trial-and-error (e.g., Penn et al. 2008). The notion of “basic processes” refers here to this kind of learning.
 
4
Interestingly, nonhuman, animal users fail transfer tasks (Martin-Ordas et al. 2008; see Penn et al. 2008). These findings provide support for the idea that humans are able to reinterpret the physical world in terms of unobservable, hypothetical entities, such as causal forces (Penn et al. 2008; see below).
 
5
The simulation-based knowledge is confronted with some limitations that will be discussed later.
 
6
The term technique is used here as synonymous with “mechanical knowledge”.
 
7
Semantic memory can be defined as “that system which processes, stores and retrieves information about the meaning of words, concepts and facts” (Warrington 1975). In other words, it allows us to think about things that are not here now (Tulving 1985).
 
8
Of course, this is not to deny that it is different to know that knives can generally be found in kitchens or supermarkets and to remember that we possess the appropriate knife in our kitchen or the supermarket where we used to go. This issue is at the heart of important controversies concerning the distinction between semantic and episodic memory introduced by Tulving (1972). Unfortunately, this issue is beyond the scope of the present article, so I will only refer to semantic knowledge to describe the ability to think about things that are not here now. Of particular interest here is that semantic knowledge is fundamental to categorize the tools and objects in function of different activities, from very basic (body self-care, feeding, sleeping) to “non-basic” activities (relaxing, playing the guitar, working). And, it is this categorical organization, which allows us to know and even to remember how to get the appropriate tools and objects. In other words, semantic memory is viewed here as the means to organize the search in memory.
 
9
This conception of working memory is quite consistent with current models (e.g., Baddeley 2003).
 
10
Tools can also be used to enhance cognitive skills (Jonassen 1992; see also Vygotsky 1978). For instance, abacuses and pocket calculators improve calculation efficiency, and diaries and portable phones facilitate remembering the activities to perform. These tools might suggest that cognitive effort is also assessed when people are engaged in everyday activities. Unfortunately, this issue is beyond the scope of the present article.
 
11
The simulation-based decision-making process has to be viewed as a dynamic process in that the effort associated with each option can be re-evaluated all along the activity. For instance, a user can choose to reach a ball that rolled under a sofa with the arm and then decide to get an umbrella to reach the ball with it.
 
12
As stressed above, a potential mistake might be to believe that when patients show how to use a tool in a conventional way, they necessarily access information about the usual function.
 
13
Recent evidence demonstrates that the perception of the length of rods via dynamic touch can be recalibrated after a training session with auditive feedback (Wagman and Abney 2012). Even if these results were not obtained in a tool-use situation they indicate that the perception of object properties is not specific to a particular perceptual modality but constrained by the object’s mechanical properties.
 
14
These procedures have been reported to be impaired in brain-damaged patients, particularly those with damage to superior parietal lobes (Binkofski et al. 2001). The term of tactile agnosia/apraxia have been proposed, stressing the importance of both the sensory and motor components of the disorder. Note also that, besides these stereotypical exploratory procedures, people can perceive a certain number of properties of objects (e.g., mass, length), without the benefit of vision, simply by wielding the objects. This kind of touch is referred to as dynamic touch (Turvey 1996; Wagman and Abney 2012). Interestingly, it has been shown that people are able, via dynamic touch, to determine whether an object is more suited for a given tool-use action (e.g., hammering) than for another one (e.g., poking; Wagman and Carello 2001; see also Wagman and Carello 2003).
 
15
Proponents of a strong, embodied cognition approach assume that, as people perceive visual tool or objects, simulations of potential actions can become automatically active in preparation for action (e.g., Barsalou 1999; Pezzulo et al. 2011). As discussed above, this proposal is debatable.
 
16
All human societies develop technical equipment, which is modified and improved, a phenomenon called ratchet effect (Tomasello 1999; for further discussion on this aspect with regard to the principles of dialectic and stigmergy, see Osiurak et al. 2010).
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Agre, P. E. (1988). The dynamic structure of everyday life (Tech. Rep. No. 1085). Massachusetts Institute of Technology: Artificial Intelligence Laboratory. Agre, P. E. (1988). The dynamic structure of everyday life (Tech. Rep. No. 1085). Massachusetts Institute of Technology: Artificial Intelligence Laboratory.
Zurück zum Zitat Andersen, S. J., Yamagishi, N., & Karavia, V. (2002). Attentional processes link perception and action. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 269, 1225–1232. Andersen, S. J., Yamagishi, N., & Karavia, V. (2002). Attentional processes link perception and action. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 269, 1225–1232.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson, J. R., Bothwell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. (2004). An integrated theory of mind. Psychological Review, 111, 1036–1060.PubMed Anderson, J. R., Bothwell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. (2004). An integrated theory of mind. Psychological Review, 111, 1036–1060.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Baber, C. (2003). Cognition and tool use. London: Talyor & Francis. Baber, C. (2003). Cognition and tool use. London: Talyor & Francis.
Zurück zum Zitat Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839.PubMed Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.PubMed Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.PubMed Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bartolo, A., Daumüller, M., Della Sala, S., & Goldenberg, G. (2007). Relationship between object-related gestures and the fractionated object knowledge system. Behavioural Neurology, 18, 143–147.PubMed Bartolo, A., Daumüller, M., Della Sala, S., & Goldenberg, G. (2007). Relationship between object-related gestures and the fractionated object knowledge system. Behavioural Neurology, 18, 143–147.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Beck, B. B. (1980). Animal tool use behavior: The use and manufacture of tools by animals. New York: Garland STPM Press. Beck, B. B. (1980). Animal tool use behavior: The use and manufacture of tools by animals. New York: Garland STPM Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Beck, S. R., Apperly, I. A., Chappel, J., Guthrie, C., & Cutting, N. (2011). Making tools isn’t child’s play. Cognition, 119, 301–306.PubMed Beck, S. R., Apperly, I. A., Chappel, J., Guthrie, C., & Cutting, N. (2011). Making tools isn’t child’s play. Cognition, 119, 301–306.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. London: Pergamon. Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. London: Pergamon.
Zurück zum Zitat Besnard, J., Allain, P., Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2009). Contrôle exécutif et comportement d’utilisation d’objets. Vers une dissociation. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 1, 120–132. Besnard, J., Allain, P., Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2009). Contrôle exécutif et comportement d’utilisation d’objets. Vers une dissociation. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 1, 120–132.
Zurück zum Zitat Besnard, J., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Osiurak, F., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., et al. (2010). Utilization Behaviour: Clinical and theoretical approaches. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 1–10. Besnard, J., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Osiurak, F., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., et al. (2010). Utilization Behaviour: Clinical and theoretical approaches. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 1–10.
Zurück zum Zitat Besnard, J., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2011). A contribution to the study of environmental dependency phenomena: The social hypothesis. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3278–3294. Besnard, J., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2011). A contribution to the study of environmental dependency phenomena: The social hypothesis. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3278–3294.
Zurück zum Zitat Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1076–1096.PubMed Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1076–1096.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Binkofski, F., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2013). Two action systems in the human brain. Brain and Language, 127, 222–229.PubMed Binkofski, F., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2013). Two action systems in the human brain. Brain and Language, 127, 222–229.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Binkofski, F., Kunesch, E., Classen, J., Seitz, R. J., & Freund, H.-J. (2001). Tactile apraxia. Unimodal disorder of tactile object exploration associated with parietal lesions. Brain, 124, 132–144. Binkofski, F., Kunesch, E., Classen, J., Seitz, R. J., & Freund, H.-J. (2001). Tactile apraxia. Unimodal disorder of tactile object exploration associated with parietal lesions. Brain, 124, 132–144.
Zurück zum Zitat Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2002). Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 237–242.PubMed Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2002). Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 237–242.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1981). Sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees: A preliminary report. Journal of Human Evolution, 10, 585–593. Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1981). Sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees: A preliminary report. Journal of Human Evolution, 10, 585–593.
Zurück zum Zitat Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1984). Possible causes of sex difference in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 13, 415–440. Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1984). Possible causes of sex difference in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 13, 415–440.
Zurück zum Zitat Boesch-Achermann, H., & Boesch, C. (1993). Tool use in wild chimpanzees: New light from dark forests. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 18–21. Boesch-Achermann, H., & Boesch, C. (1993). Tool use in wild chimpanzees: New light from dark forests. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 18–21.
Zurück zum Zitat Borghi, A. M., & Riggio, L. (2009). Sentence comprehension and simulation of object temporary, canonical and stable affordances. Brain Research, 1253, 117–128.PubMed Borghi, A. M., & Riggio, L. (2009). Sentence comprehension and simulation of object temporary, canonical and stable affordances. Brain Research, 1253, 117–128.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Botvinick, M. M., & Plaut, D. C. (2004). Doing without schema hierarchies: A recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. Psychological Review, 111, 395–429.PubMed Botvinick, M. M., & Plaut, D. C. (2004). Doing without schema hierarchies: A recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. Psychological Review, 111, 395–429.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Botvinick, M. M., & Rosen, Z. (2009). Anticipation of cognitive demand during decision-making. Psychological Research, 73, 835–842.PubMedCentralPubMed Botvinick, M. M., & Rosen, Z. (2009). Anticipation of cognitive demand during decision-making. Psychological Research, 73, 835–842.PubMedCentralPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). When objects lose their meaning: What happens to their use? Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neurosciences, 2, 236–251. Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). When objects lose their meaning: What happens to their use? Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neurosciences, 2, 236–251.
Zurück zum Zitat Brazzelli, M., Colombo, N., Della Sala, S., & Spinnler, H. (1994). Spared and impaired cognitive abilities after bilateral frontal damage. Cortex, 30, 27–51.PubMed Brazzelli, M., Colombo, N., Della Sala, S., & Spinnler, H. (1994). Spared and impaired cognitive abilities after bilateral frontal damage. Cortex, 30, 27–51.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E. J. (2010). Grasping beer mugs: On the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 341–358.PubMed Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E. J. (2010). Grasping beer mugs: On the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 341–358.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Buxbaum, L. J. (2001). Ideomotor Apraxia: A call to action. Neurocase, 7, 445–448.PubMed Buxbaum, L. J. (2001). Ideomotor Apraxia: A call to action. Neurocase, 7, 445–448.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Buxbaum, L. J., & Kalénine, S. (2010). Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 201–218.PubMed Buxbaum, L. J., & Kalénine, S. (2010). Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 201–218.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., & Carew, T. G. (1997). The role of memory in object use. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 219–254. Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., & Carew, T. G. (1997). The role of memory in object use. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 219–254.
Zurück zum Zitat Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., & Montgomery, M. W. (1998). Ideational apraxia and naturalistic action. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 617–643.PubMed Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., & Montgomery, M. W. (1998). Ideational apraxia and naturalistic action. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 617–643.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Buxbaum, L. J., Kyle, K. M., & Menon, R. (2005). On beyond mirror neurons: Internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 226–239.PubMed Buxbaum, L. J., Kyle, K. M., & Menon, R. (2005). On beyond mirror neurons: Internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 226–239.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cardinalli, L., Frassinetti, F., Brozzoli, C., Urquizar, C., Roy, A. C., & Farnè, A. (2009). Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Current Biology, 19, 478–479. Cardinalli, L., Frassinetti, F., Brozzoli, C., Urquizar, C., Roy, A. C., & Farnè, A. (2009). Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Current Biology, 19, 478–479.
Zurück zum Zitat Chappell, J., & Kacelnik, A. (2002). Tool selectivity in a non-primate species, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Animal Cognition, 5, 71–78.PubMed Chappell, J., & Kacelnik, A. (2002). Tool selectivity in a non-primate species, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Animal Cognition, 5, 71–78.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: Grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 853–861.PubMed Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: Grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 853–861.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Clark, M. A., Merians, A. S., Kothari, A., Poizner, H., Macauley, B., Rothi, L. J. G., et al. (1994). Spatial planning deficits in limb apraxia. Brain, 117, 1093–1106.PubMed Clark, M. A., Merians, A. S., Kothari, A., Poizner, H., Macauley, B., Rothi, L. J. G., et al. (1994). Spatial planning deficits in limb apraxia. Brain, 117, 1093–1106.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cooper, R. P. (2002). Order and disorder in everyday action: The roles of contention scheduling and supervisory attentional system. Neurocase, 8, 61–79.PubMed Cooper, R. P. (2002). Order and disorder in everyday action: The roles of contention scheduling and supervisory attentional system. Neurocase, 8, 61–79.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 297–338.PubMed Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 297–338.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (2006). Hierarchical schemas and goals in the control of sequential behavior. Psychological Review, 113, 887–916.PubMed Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (2006). Hierarchical schemas and goals in the control of sequential behavior. Psychological Review, 113, 887–916.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cooper, R. P., Schwartz, M. F., Yule, P. G., & Shallice, T. (2005). The simulation of action disorganisation in complex activities of daily living. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 959–1004.PubMed Cooper, R. P., Schwartz, M. F., Yule, P. G., & Shallice, T. (2005). The simulation of action disorganisation in complex activities of daily living. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 959–1004.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Cubelli, R., Marchetti, C., Boscolo, G., & Della Sala, S. (2000). Cognition in action: Testing a model of limb apraxia. Brain and Cognition, 44, 144–165.PubMed Cubelli, R., Marchetti, C., Boscolo, G., & Della Sala, S. (2000). Cognition in action: Testing a model of limb apraxia. Brain and Cognition, 44, 144–165.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Daprati, E., & Sirigu, A. (2006). How we interact with objects: Learning from brain lesions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 265–270.PubMed Daprati, E., & Sirigu, A. (2006). How we interact with objects: Learning from brain lesions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 265–270.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat De Renzi, E. (1989). Apraxia. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 245–263). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers. De Renzi, E. (1989). Apraxia. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 245–263). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
Zurück zum Zitat De Renzi, E., & Lucchelli, F. (1988). Ideational apraxia. Brain, 111, 1173–1185.PubMed De Renzi, E., & Lucchelli, F. (1988). Ideational apraxia. Brain, 111, 1173–1185.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat De Renzi, E., Cavalleri, F., & Facchini, S. (1996). Imitation and utilisation behaviour. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 61, 396–400.PubMedCentralPubMed De Renzi, E., Cavalleri, F., & Facchini, S. (1996). Imitation and utilisation behaviour. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 61, 396–400.PubMedCentralPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Prablanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 34, 35–42.PubMed Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Prablanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 34, 35–42.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Duncan, J. (1986). Disorganisation of behaviour after frontal lobe damage. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 271–290. Duncan, J. (1986). Disorganisation of behaviour after frontal lobe damage. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 271–290.
Zurück zum Zitat Farnè, A., & Làdavas, E. (2000). Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use. NeuroReport, 11, 1645–1649.PubMed Farnè, A., & Làdavas, E. (2000). Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use. NeuroReport, 11, 1645–1649.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Forde, E. M. E., & Humphreys, G. W. (2000). The role of semantic knowledge and working memory in everyday tasks. Brain and Cognition, 44, 214–252.PubMed Forde, E. M. E., & Humphreys, G. W. (2000). The role of semantic knowledge and working memory in everyday tasks. Brain and Cognition, 44, 214–252.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Frey, S. H. (2008). Tool use, communicative gesture and cerebral asymmetries in the modern human brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 363, 1951–1957. Frey, S. H. (2008). Tool use, communicative gesture and cerebral asymmetries in the modern human brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 363, 1951–1957.
Zurück zum Zitat Garcea, F. E., & Mahon, B. Z. (2012). What is a tool concept? Dissociating manipulation knowledge from function knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 40, 1303–1313. Garcea, F. E., & Mahon, B. Z. (2012). What is a tool concept? Dissociating manipulation knowledge from function knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 40, 1303–1313.
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin Co. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin Co.
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson, K. R. (1993). Generative interplay between technical capacities, social relations, imitation and cognition. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution (pp. 251–269). New York: Cambridge University Press. Gibson, K. R. (1993). Generative interplay between technical capacities, social relations, imitation and cognition. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution (pp. 251–269). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Giovannetti, T., Britnell, P., Brennan, L., Siderowl, A., Grossman, M., Libon, D. J., et al. (2012). Everyday action impairment in Parkinson’s disease dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18, 787–798.PubMedCentralPubMed Giovannetti, T., Britnell, P., Brennan, L., Siderowl, A., Grossman, M., Libon, D. J., et al. (2012). Everyday action impairment in Parkinson’s disease dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18, 787–798.PubMedCentralPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Goldenberg, G. (1995). Imitating gestures and manipulating a mannikin: The representation of the human body in ideomotor apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 33, 63–72.PubMed Goldenberg, G. (1995). Imitating gestures and manipulating a mannikin: The representation of the human body in ideomotor apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 33, 63–72.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Goldenberg, G. (1999). Matching and imitation of hand and finger postures in patients with damage in left or right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 37, 559–566.PubMed Goldenberg, G. (1999). Matching and imitation of hand and finger postures in patients with damage in left or right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 37, 559–566.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Goldenberg, G. (2013a). Apraxia: The cognitive side of motor control. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Goldenberg, G. (2013a). Apraxia: The cognitive side of motor control. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldenberg, G. (2013b). Apraxia in left-handers. Brain, 136, 2592–2601.PubMed Goldenberg, G. (2013b). Apraxia in left-handers. Brain, 136, 2592–2601.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Goldenberg, G., & Hagmann, S. (1998). Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 36, 581–589.PubMed Goldenberg, G., & Hagmann, S. (1998). Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 36, 581–589.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Goldenberg, G., & Spatt, J. (2009). The neural basis of tool use. Brain, 132, 1645–1655.PubMed Goldenberg, G., & Spatt, J. (2009). The neural basis of tool use. Brain, 132, 1645–1655.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Goldenberg, G., Hartmann, K., & Schlott, I. (2003). Defective pantomime of object use in left brain damage: Apraxia or asymbolia? Neuropsychologia, 41, 1565–1573.PubMed Goldenberg, G., Hartmann, K., & Schlott, I. (2003). Defective pantomime of object use in left brain damage: Apraxia or asymbolia? Neuropsychologia, 41, 1565–1573.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Goldenberg, G., Hentze, S., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2004). The effect of tactile feedback on pantomime of tool use in apraxia. Neurology, 63, 1863–1867.PubMed Goldenberg, G., Hentze, S., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2004). The effect of tactile feedback on pantomime of tool use in apraxia. Neurology, 63, 1863–1867.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Goldenberg, G., Hartmann-Schmid, K., Sürer, F., Daumüller, M., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2007). The impact of dysexecutive syndrome on use of tools and technical devices. Cortex, 43, 424–435.PubMed Goldenberg, G., Hartmann-Schmid, K., Sürer, F., Daumüller, M., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2007). The impact of dysexecutive syndrome on use of tools and technical devices. Cortex, 43, 424–435.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gredlein, J. M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Sex differences in young children’s use of tools in a problem-solving task. Human Nature, 16, 211–232. Gredlein, J. M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Sex differences in young children’s use of tools in a problem-solving task. Human Nature, 16, 211–232.
Zurück zum Zitat Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideomotor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMed Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideomotor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gruber, T., Clay, Z., & Zuberbühler, K. (2010). A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: Evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage. Animal Behaviour, 80, 1023–1033. Gruber, T., Clay, Z., & Zuberbühler, K. (2010). A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: Evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage. Animal Behaviour, 80, 1023–1033.
Zurück zum Zitat Hartmann, K., Goldenberg, G., Daumüller, M., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2005). It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: The neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices. Neuropsychologia, 43, 625–627.PubMed Hartmann, K., Goldenberg, G., Daumüller, M., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2005). It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: The neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices. Neuropsychologia, 43, 625–627.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J., & Valenstein, E. (1982). Two forms of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology, 32, 342–346.PubMed Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J., & Valenstein, E. (1982). Two forms of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology, 32, 342–346.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J. G., Mack, L., Feinberg, T., & Watson, R. T. (1986). Apraxia after superior parietal lesions. Cortex, 32, 141–150. Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J. G., Mack, L., Feinberg, T., & Watson, R. T. (1986). Apraxia after superior parietal lesions. Cortex, 32, 141–150.
Zurück zum Zitat Hermsdörfer, J., Hentze, S., & Goldenberg, G. (2006). Spatial and kinematic features of apraxic movement depend on the mode of execution. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1642–1652.PubMed Hermsdörfer, J., Hentze, S., & Goldenberg, G. (2006). Spatial and kinematic features of apraxic movement depend on the mode of execution. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1642–1652.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hodges, J. R., Spatt, J., & Patterson, K. (1999). “What” and “how”: Evidence for the dissociation of object knowledge and mechanical problem-solving skills in the human brain. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 9444–9448. Hodges, J. R., Spatt, J., & Patterson, K. (1999). “What” and “how”: Evidence for the dissociation of object knowledge and mechanical problem-solving skills in the human brain. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 9444–9448.
Zurück zum Zitat Hodges, J. R., Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Spatt, J. (2000). The role of knowledge in object use: Evidence from semantic dementia. Brain, 123, 1913–1925.PubMed Hodges, J. R., Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Spatt, J. (2000). The role of knowledge in object use: Evidence from semantic dementia. Brain, 123, 1913–1925.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.PubMed Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Humphreys, G. W., & Forde, E. M. E. (1998). Disordered action schema and action disorganisation syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 771–811. Humphreys, G. W., & Forde, E. M. E. (1998). Disordered action schema and action disorganisation syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 771–811.
Zurück zum Zitat Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport, 7, 2325–2330.PubMed Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport, 7, 2325–2330.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Jacobs, D. H., Adair, J. C., Williamson, D. J. G., Na, D. L., Gold, M., Foundas, A. L., et al. (1999). Apraxia and motor-skill acquisition in Alzheimer disease are dissociable. Neuropsychologia, 37, 875–880.PubMed Jacobs, D. H., Adair, J. C., Williamson, D. J. G., Na, D. L., Gold, M., Foundas, A. L., et al. (1999). Apraxia and motor-skill acquisition in Alzheimer disease are dissociable. Neuropsychologia, 37, 875–880.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat James, W. (2007). The principles of psychology. New York: Cosimo Classics (Original work published 1890) James, W. (2007). The principles of psychology. New York: Cosimo Classics (Original work published 1890)
Zurück zum Zitat Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2012). On the persistence of tool- based compatibility effects. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 220, 16–22. Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2012). On the persistence of tool- based compatibility effects. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 220, 16–22.
Zurück zum Zitat Jarry, C., Osiurak, F., Delafuys, D., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2013). Apraxia of tool use: More évidence for the technical reasoning hypothesis. Cortex, 49, 2322–2333.PubMed Jarry, C., Osiurak, F., Delafuys, D., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2013). Apraxia of tool use: More évidence for the technical reasoning hypothesis. Cortex, 49, 2322–2333.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Jax, S. A., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2010). Response interference between functional and structural actions linked to the same familiar object. Cognition, 115, 350–355.PubMedCentralPubMed Jax, S. A., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2010). Response interference between functional and structural actions linked to the same familiar object. Cognition, 115, 350–355.PubMedCentralPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, 103–109. Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, 103–109.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson, S. L. (1981). Effect of training device on retention and transfer of a procedural task. Human Factors, 23, 257–272. Johnson, S. L. (1981). Effect of training device on retention and transfer of a procedural task. Human Factors, 23, 257–272.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson-Frey, S. H. (2007). What puts the how in where? Tool use and the divided visual streams hypothesis. Cortex, 43, 368–375. Johnson-Frey, S. H. (2007). What puts the how in where? Tool use and the divided visual streams hypothesis. Cortex, 43, 368–375.
Zurück zum Zitat Jonassen, D. H. (1992). What are cognitive tools? In P. A. M. Kommers, D. H. Jonassen, J. T. Mayes, & A. Ferreira (Eds.), Cognitive Tools for Learning (NATO ASI Series, pp. 1–6). Jonassen, D. H. (1992). What are cognitive tools? In P. A. M. Kommers, D. H. Jonassen, J. T. Mayes, & A. Ferreira (Eds.), Cognitive Tools for Learning (NATO ASI Series, pp. 1–6).
Zurück zum Zitat Klatzky, R. L., & Lederman, S. J. (2002). Touch. In A. F. Healy & R. W. Proctor (Eds.), Experimental Psychology (pp. 147–176). New York: Wiley. Klatzky, R. L., & Lederman, S. J. (2002). Touch. In A. F. Healy & R. W. Proctor (Eds.), Experimental Psychology (pp. 147–176). New York: Wiley.
Zurück zum Zitat Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 665–682. Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 665–682.
Zurück zum Zitat Kunde, W., Müsseler, J., & Heuer, H. (2007). Spatial compatibility effects with tool use. Human Factors, 49, 661–670.PubMed Kunde, W., Müsseler, J., & Heuer, H. (2007). Spatial compatibility effects with tool use. Human Factors, 49, 661–670.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Latash, M. L., Scholz, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2007). Toward a new theory of motor synergies. Motor Control, 11, 267–308. Latash, M. L., Scholz, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2007). Toward a new theory of motor synergies. Motor Control, 11, 267–308.
Zurück zum Zitat Lauro-Grotto, R., Piccini, C., & Shallice, T. (1997). Modality-specific operations in semantic dementia. Cortex, 33, 593–622.PubMed Lauro-Grotto, R., Piccini, C., & Shallice, T. (1997). Modality-specific operations in semantic dementia. Cortex, 33, 593–622.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Leakey, R. (1980). The making of Mankind. London: Book Club Associates. Leakey, R. (1980). The making of Mankind. London: Book Club Associates.
Zurück zum Zitat Lederman, S. J., & Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 342–368.PubMed Lederman, S. J., & Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 342–368.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1971). L’Homme et la matière. Paris: Albin Michel. Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1971). L’Homme et la matière. Paris: Albin Michel.
Zurück zum Zitat Lesourd, M., Le Gall, D., Baumard, J., Croisile, B., Jarry, C., & Osiurak, F. (2013). Apraxia and Alzheimer’s disease: Review and perspectives. Neuropsychology Review, 23, 234–256.PubMed Lesourd, M., Le Gall, D., Baumard, J., Croisile, B., Jarry, C., & Osiurak, F. (2013). Apraxia and Alzheimer’s disease: Review and perspectives. Neuropsychology Review, 23, 234–256.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Lhermitte, F. (1983). Utilisation behaviour and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106, 237–255.PubMed Lhermitte, F. (1983). Utilisation behaviour and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106, 237–255.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Lhermitte, F., Pillon, B., & Serdaru, M. (1986). Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: Imitation and utilization behavior: A neuropsychological study of 75 patients. Annals of Neurology, 19, 326–334.PubMed Lhermitte, F., Pillon, B., & Serdaru, M. (1986). Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: Imitation and utilization behavior: A neuropsychological study of 75 patients. Annals of Neurology, 19, 326–334.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Limongelli, L., Boysen, S. T., & Visalberghi, E. (1995). Comprehension of cause-effect relations in a tool-using task by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109, 18–26.PubMed Limongelli, L., Boysen, S. T., & Visalberghi, E. (1995). Comprehension of cause-effect relations in a tool-using task by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109, 18–26.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Lonsdorf, E. V. (2005). Sex differences in the development of termite-fishing skills in the wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Animal Behaviour, 70, 673–683. Lonsdorf, E. V. (2005). Sex differences in the development of termite-fishing skills in the wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Animal Behaviour, 70, 673–683.
Zurück zum Zitat Macuga, K. L., Papailiou, A. P., & Frey, S. H. (2012). Motor imagery of tool use: Relationship to actual use and adherence to Fitts’ law across tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 169–174.PubMedCentralPubMed Macuga, K. L., Papailiou, A. P., & Frey, S. H. (2012). Motor imagery of tool use: Relationship to actual use and adherence to Fitts’ law across tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 169–174.PubMedCentralPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology, 102, 59–70.PubMed Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology, 102, 59–70.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2009). Concepts and categories: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 27–51.PubMedCentralPubMed Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2009). Concepts and categories: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 27–51.PubMedCentralPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Maravita, A., & Iriki, A. (2004). Tools for the body (schema). Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 79–86.PubMed Maravita, A., & Iriki, A. (2004). Tools for the body (schema). Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 79–86.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Martin-Ordas, G., Call, J., & Colmenares, F. (2008). Tubes, tables and traps: Great apes solve two functionally equivalent trap tasks but show no evidence of transfer across tasks. Animal Cognition, 11, 423–430.PubMedCentralPubMed Martin-Ordas, G., Call, J., & Colmenares, F. (2008). Tubes, tables and traps: Great apes solve two functionally equivalent trap tasks but show no evidence of transfer across tasks. Animal Cognition, 11, 423–430.PubMedCentralPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Massen, C. (2013). Cognitive representations of tool-use interactions. New Ideas in Psychology, 31, 239–246. Massen, C. (2013). Cognitive representations of tool-use interactions. New Ideas in Psychology, 31, 239–246.
Zurück zum Zitat Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2007). Programming tool-use actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 692–704.PubMed Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2007). Programming tool-use actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 692–704.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2009). Movements, actions and tool-use actions: An ideomotor approach to imitation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2349–2358. Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2009). Movements, actions and tool-use actions: An ideomotor approach to imitation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2349–2358.
Zurück zum Zitat McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee material culture: Implications for human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee material culture: Implications for human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2006). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2006). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Morady, K., & Humphreys, G. (2011). Multiple tasks demands in action disorganization syndrome. Neurocase, 17, 461–472.PubMed Morady, K., & Humphreys, G. (2011). Multiple tasks demands in action disorganization syndrome. Neurocase, 17, 461–472.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Morgado, N., Gentaz, E., Guinet, E., Osiurak, F., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2013). Within reach but no so reachable. Obstacles matter in visual perception of reaching distances. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 462–467. Morgado, N., Gentaz, E., Guinet, E., Osiurak, F., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2013). Within reach but no so reachable. Obstacles matter in visual perception of reaching distances. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 462–467.
Zurück zum Zitat Mounoud, P. (1996). A recursive transformation of central cognitive mechanisms: The shift from partial to whole representation. In A. J. Sameroff & M. M. Haith (Eds.), The five to seven year shift: the age of reason and responsibility (pp. 85–110). Chicago: Chicago University Press. Mounoud, P. (1996). A recursive transformation of central cognitive mechanisms: The shift from partial to whole representation. In A. J. Sameroff & M. M. Haith (Eds.), The five to seven year shift: the age of reason and responsibility (pp. 85–110). Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Negri, G. A., Lunardelli, A., Reverberi, C., Gigli, G. L., & Rumiati, R. I. (2007a). Degraded semantic knowledge and accurate object use. Cortex, 43, 376–388.PubMed Negri, G. A., Lunardelli, A., Reverberi, C., Gigli, G. L., & Rumiati, R. I. (2007a). Degraded semantic knowledge and accurate object use. Cortex, 43, 376–388.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Negri, G. A. L., Rumiati, R. I., Zadini, A., Ukmar, M., Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2007b). What is the role of motor stimulation in action and object recognition? Evidence from apraxia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 795–816.PubMed Negri, G. A. L., Rumiati, R. I., Zadini, A., Ukmar, M., Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2007b). What is the role of motor stimulation in action and object recognition? Evidence from apraxia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 795–816.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Nonaka, T. (2013). Motor variability but functional specificity: The case of a C4 tetraplegic mouth calligrapher. Ecological Psychology, 25, 131–154. Nonaka, T. (2013). Motor variability but functional specificity: The case of a C4 tetraplegic mouth calligrapher. Ecological Psychology, 25, 131–154.
Zurück zum Zitat Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88, 1–14. Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88, 1–14.
Zurück zum Zitat Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. Davidson, G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum Press. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. Davidson, G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2014). Pliers, not fingers. Tool-action effect in a motor intention paradigm. Cognition, 130, 66–73. Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2014). Pliers, not fingers. Tool-action effect in a motor intention paradigm. Cognition, 130, 66–73.
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Allain, P., Jarry, C., Richard, I., & Le Gall, D. (2008a). Object usage and object utilization. A single-case study. Neurocase, 14, 169–183. Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Allain, P., Jarry, C., Richard, I., & Le Gall, D. (2008a). Object usage and object utilization. A single-case study. Neurocase, 14, 169–183.
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Allain, P., Jarry, C., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Richard, I., et al. (2008b). Different constraints on grip selection in brain-damaged patients. Object use versus object transport. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2431–2434. Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Allain, P., Jarry, C., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Richard, I., et al. (2008b). Different constraints on grip selection in brain-damaged patients. Object use versus object transport. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2431–2434.
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Richard, I., et al. (2009). Unusual use of objects after unilateral brain damage. The technical reasoning model. Cortex, 45, 769–783. Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Richard, I., et al. (2009). Unusual use of objects after unilateral brain damage. The technical reasoning model. Cortex, 45, 769–783.
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2010). Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning. Toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. Psychological Review, 117, 517–540.PubMed Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2010). Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning. Toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. Psychological Review, 117, 517–540.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2011). Re-examining the gesture engram hypothesis. New perspectives on apraxia of tool use. Neuropsychologia, 49, 299–312. Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2011). Re-examining the gesture engram hypothesis. New perspectives on apraxia of tool use. Neuropsychologia, 49, 299–312.
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Baltenneck, N., Boudin, B., & Le Gall, D. (2012a). Make a gesture and I will tell you what you are miming. Pantomime recognition in healthy subjects. Cortex, 48, 584–592. Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Baltenneck, N., Boudin, B., & Le Gall, D. (2012a). Make a gesture and I will tell you what you are miming. Pantomime recognition in healthy subjects. Cortex, 48, 584–592.
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., Morgado, N., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2012b). Tool use and perceived distance. When unreachable becomes sponatenously reachable. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 331–339.PubMed Osiurak, F., Morgado, N., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2012b). Tool use and perceived distance. When unreachable becomes sponatenously reachable. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 331–339.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Lesourd, M., Baumard, J., & Le Gall, D. (2013a). Mechanical problem-solving in left brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1964–1972.PubMed Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Lesourd, M., Baumard, J., & Le Gall, D. (2013a). Mechanical problem-solving in left brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1964–1972.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., Roche, K., Ramone, J., & Chainay, H. (2013b). Handing a tool to someone can take more time than using it. Cognition, 128, 76–81.PubMed Osiurak, F., Roche, K., Ramone, J., & Chainay, H. (2013b). Handing a tool to someone can take more time than using it. Cognition, 128, 76–81.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., Wagner, C., Djerbi, S., & Navarro, J. (2013c). Why do we use automatic tools? The priority of control over effort. Experimental Psychology, 60, 453–468.PubMed Osiurak, F., Wagner, C., Djerbi, S., & Navarro, J. (2013c). Why do we use automatic tools? The priority of control over effort. Experimental Psychology, 60, 453–468.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Osiurak, F., Morgado, N., Vallet, G., Drot, M., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2014). Getting a tool gives wings: Underestimation of effort for tool use. Psychological Research, 78, 1–9.PubMed Osiurak, F., Morgado, N., Vallet, G., Drot, M., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2014). Getting a tool gives wings: Underestimation of effort for tool use. Psychological Research, 78, 1–9.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Clark, K., Stewart, M., Massaquoi, S., Lou, J.-S., et al. (1993). Procedural learning in Parkinson’s disease and cerebellar degeneration. Annuals of Neurology, 34, 594–602. Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Clark, K., Stewart, M., Massaquoi, S., Lou, J.-S., et al. (1993). Procedural learning in Parkinson’s disease and cerebellar degeneration. Annuals of Neurology, 34, 594–602.
Zurück zum Zitat Penfield, W., & Evans, J. (1935). The frontal lobe in man: A clinical study of maximum removals. Brain, 58, 115–133. Penfield, W., & Evans, J. (1935). The frontal lobe in man: A clinical study of maximum removals. Brain, 58, 115–133.
Zurück zum Zitat Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109–130.PubMed Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109–130.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., McRae, K., & Spivey, M. J. (2011). The mechanics of embodiment: A dialog on embodiment and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 5.PubMedCentralPubMed Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., McRae, K., & Spivey, M. J. (2011). The mechanics of embodiment: A dialog on embodiment and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 5.PubMedCentralPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., McRae, K., & Spivey, M. J. (2013). Computational grounded cognition: A new alliance between grounded cognition and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 612.PubMedCentralPubMed Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., McRae, K., & Spivey, M. J. (2013). Computational grounded cognition: A new alliance between grounded cognition and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 612.PubMedCentralPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Phillips, J. C., & Ward, R. (2002). S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: time course and specificity of response activation. Visual Cognition, 9, 540–558. Phillips, J. C., & Ward, R. (2002). S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: time course and specificity of response activation. Visual Cognition, 9, 540–558.
Zurück zum Zitat Povinelli, D. J. (2000). Folk physics for apes. New York: Oxford University Press. Povinelli, D. J. (2000). Folk physics for apes. New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.
Zurück zum Zitat Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 1, 110–122. Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 1, 110–122.
Zurück zum Zitat Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in distance perception. Psychological Science, 14, 409–428. Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in distance perception. Psychological Science, 14, 409–428.
Zurück zum Zitat Reason, J. T. (1979). Actions not as planned: The price of automatization. In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (Eds.), Aspects of consciousness (pp. 67–89). London: London Academic Press. Reason, J. T. (1979). Actions not as planned: The price of automatization. In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (Eds.), Aspects of consciousness (pp. 67–89). London: London Academic Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Rieger, M. (2004). Automatic keypress activation in skilled typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 555–565.PubMed Rieger, M. (2004). Automatic keypress activation in skilled typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 555–565.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Rieger, M., & Massen, C. (2014). Tool characteristics in imagery of tool actions. Psychological Research, 78, 10–17.PubMed Rieger, M., & Massen, C. (2014). Tool characteristics in imagery of tool actions. Psychological Research, 78, 10–17.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMed Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenbaum, D. A. (2008). Reaching while walking: Reaching distance costs more than walking distance. Psychonomic Bulletin & review, 15, 1100–1104. Rosenbaum, D. A. (2008). Reaching while walking: Reaching distance costs more than walking distance. Psychonomic Bulletin & review, 15, 1100–1104.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenbaum, D. A., Marchak, F., Barnes, H. J., Vaughan, J., Slotta, J., & Jorgensen, M. (1990). Constraints for action selection: Overhand versus underhand grips. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 321–342). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Rosenbaum, D. A., Marchak, F., Barnes, H. J., Vaughan, J., Slotta, J., & Jorgensen, M. (1990). Constraints for action selection: Overhand versus underhand grips. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 321–342). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenbaum, D. A., Vaughan, J., Barnes, H. J., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1992). Time course of movement planning: Selection of handgrips for object manipulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1058–1073.PubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., Vaughan, J., Barnes, H. J., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1992). Time course of movement planning: Selection of handgrips for object manipulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1058–1073.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. J., Vaughan, J., & Jansen, C. (2001). Posture-based motion planning: Applications to grasping. Psychological Review, 108, 709–734.PubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. J., Vaughan, J., & Jansen, C. (2001). Posture-based motion planning: Applications to grasping. Psychological Review, 108, 709–734.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Rothi, L. J. G., Ochipa, C., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 443–458. Rothi, L. J. G., Ochipa, C., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 443–458.
Zurück zum Zitat Roy, E. A., & Square, P. A. (1985). Common considerations in the study of limb, verbal and oral apraxia. In E. A. Roy (Ed.), Neuropsychological studies of apraxia and related disorders (pp. 111–161). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Roy, E. A., & Square, P. A. (1985). Common considerations in the study of limb, verbal and oral apraxia. In E. A. Roy (Ed.), Neuropsychological studies of apraxia and related disorders (pp. 111–161). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Zurück zum Zitat Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L., & Hinton, G. E. (1986). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In J. L. McClelland & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (pp. 7–57). Cambridge: MIT Press. Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L., & Hinton, G. E. (1986). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In J. L. McClelland & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (pp. 7–57). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Santos, L. R., Pearson, H. M., Spaepen, G. M., Tsao, F., & Hauser, M. (2006). Probing the limits of tool competence: Experiments with two non-tool-using species (Cercopithecus aethiops and Saguinus oedipus). Animal Cognition, 9, 94–109.PubMed Santos, L. R., Pearson, H. M., Spaepen, G. M., Tsao, F., & Hauser, M. (2006). Probing the limits of tool competence: Experiments with two non-tool-using species (Cercopithecus aethiops and Saguinus oedipus). Animal Cognition, 9, 94–109.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Schwartz, M. F. (1995). Re-examining the role of executive functions in routine action production. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 769, 321–335. Schwartz, M. F. (1995). Re-examining the role of executive functions in routine action production. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 769, 321–335.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwartz, M. F., Reed, E. S., Montgomery, M. W., Palmer, C., & Mayer, N. H. (1991). The quantitative description of action disorganisation after brain damage: A case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 381–414. Schwartz, M. F., Reed, E. S., Montgomery, M. W., Palmer, C., & Mayer, N. H. (1991). The quantitative description of action disorganisation after brain damage: A case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 381–414.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwartz, M. F., Montgomery, M. W., Buxbaum, L. J., Lee, S. S., Carew, T. G., Coslett, H. B., et al. (1998). Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. Neuropsychology, 12, 13–28.PubMed Schwartz, M. F., Montgomery, M. W., Buxbaum, L. J., Lee, S. S., Carew, T. G., Coslett, H. B., et al. (1998). Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. Neuropsychology, 12, 13–28.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Schwartz, M. F., Segal, M., Veramonti, T., Ferraro, M., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2002). The naturalistic action test: A standardized assessment for everyday action impairment. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 311–339. Schwartz, M. F., Segal, M., Veramonti, T., Ferraro, M., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2002). The naturalistic action test: A standardized assessment for everyday action impairment. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 311–339.
Zurück zum Zitat Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727–741.PubMed Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727–741.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Shallice, T., Burgess, P. W., Schon, F., & Baxter, D. M. (1989). The origins of utilisation behaviour. Brain, 112, 1587–1598.PubMed Shallice, T., Burgess, P. W., Schon, F., & Baxter, D. M. (1989). The origins of utilisation behaviour. Brain, 112, 1587–1598.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Shaw, R. (2003). The agent-environment interface: Simon’s indirect or Gibson’s direct coupling. Ecological Psychology, 15, 37–106. Shaw, R. (2003). The agent-environment interface: Simon’s indirect or Gibson’s direct coupling. Ecological Psychology, 15, 37–106.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaw, R., Turvey, M. T., & Mace, W. (1982). Ecological psychology: The consequence of a commitment to realism. In W. Weimer & D. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic process (pp. 159–226). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Shaw, R., Turvey, M. T., & Mace, W. (1982). Ecological psychology: The consequence of a commitment to realism. In W. Weimer & D. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic process (pp. 159–226). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Zurück zum Zitat Silveri, M. C., & Ciccarelli, N. (2009). Semantic memory in object use. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2634–2641.PubMed Silveri, M. C., & Ciccarelli, N. (2009). Semantic memory in object use. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2634–2641.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Singh, D. (1970). Preference for bar pressing to obtain reward over freeloading in rats and children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 73, 320–327. Singh, D. (1970). Preference for bar pressing to obtain reward over freeloading in rats and children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 73, 320–327.
Zurück zum Zitat Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J.-R., & Poncet, M. (1991). The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition. Brain, 114, 2555–2573.PubMed Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J.-R., & Poncet, M. (1991). The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition. Brain, 114, 2555–2573.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Solomon, R. L. (1948). The influence of work on behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 45, 1–40.PubMed Solomon, R. L. (1948). The influence of work on behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 45, 1–40.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Solway, A., & Botvinick, M. M. (2012). Goal-directed decision making as probabilistic inference: A computational framework and potential neural correlates. Psychological Review, 119, 120–154.PubMedCentralPubMed Solway, A., & Botvinick, M. M. (2012). Goal-directed decision making as probabilistic inference: A computational framework and potential neural correlates. Psychological Review, 119, 120–154.PubMedCentralPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Spatt, J., Bak, T., Bozeat, S., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Apraxia, mechanical problem solving and semantic knowledge: Contributions to object usage in corticobasal degeneration. Journal of Neurology, 249, 601–608.PubMed Spatt, J., Bak, T., Bozeat, S., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Apraxia, mechanical problem solving and semantic knowledge: Contributions to object usage in corticobasal degeneration. Journal of Neurology, 249, 601–608.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Symes, E., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2005). Dissociating object-based and space-based affordances. Visual Cognition, 12, 1337–1361. Symes, E., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2005). Dissociating object-based and space-based affordances. Visual Cognition, 12, 1337–1361.
Zurück zum Zitat Tarte, R. D. (1981). Contrafreeloading in humans. Psychological Reports, 49, 859–866. Tarte, R. D. (1981). Contrafreeloading in humans. Psychological Reports, 49, 859–866.
Zurück zum Zitat Thill, S., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Ziemke, T., & Baldassarre, G. (2013). Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror system: An integrative review. Neuroscience & BioBehavioral Reviews, 37, 491–521. Thill, S., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Ziemke, T., & Baldassarre, G. (2013). Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror system: An integrative review. Neuroscience & BioBehavioral Reviews, 37, 491–521.
Zurück zum Zitat Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., & Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision-for-action: the effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 493–498. Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., & Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision-for-action: the effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 493–498.
Zurück zum Zitat Tolman, E. C. (1949). The nature and functioning of wants. Psychological Review, 56, 357–369.PubMed Tolman, E. C. (1949). The nature and functioning of wants. Psychological Review, 56, 357–369.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Tomasello, M. (1999). The human adaptation for culture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 509–529. Tomasello, M. (1999). The human adaptation for culture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 509–529.
Zurück zum Zitat Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.PubMed Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381–403). New York: New York Academic Press. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381–403). New York: New York Academic Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 25, 1–12. Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 25, 1–12.
Zurück zum Zitat Turvey, M. T. (1996). Dynamic Touch. American Psychologist, 51, 1134–1152.PubMed Turvey, M. T. (1996). Dynamic Touch. American Psychologist, 51, 1134–1152.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Vaesen, K. (2012). The cognitive bases of human tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 203–218.PubMed Vaesen, K. (2012). The cognitive bases of human tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 203–218.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Valyear, K. F., Chapman, C. S., Gallivan, J. P., Mark, R. S., & Culham, J. C. (2011). To use or to move: Goal-set modulates priming when grasping real tools. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 125–142.PubMed Valyear, K. F., Chapman, C. S., Gallivan, J. P., Mark, R. S., & Culham, J. C. (2011). To use or to move: Goal-set modulates priming when grasping real tools. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 125–142.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat van der Steen, M. C., & Bongers, R. M. (2011). Joint angle variability and co-variation in a reaching with a rod task. Experimental Brain Research, 208, 411–422.PubMed van der Steen, M. C., & Bongers, R. M. (2011). Joint angle variability and co-variation in a reaching with a rod task. Experimental Brain Research, 208, 411–422.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O., & Merrill, M. Y. (1999). The conditions for tool use in primates: Implications for the evolution of material culture. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 19–41. van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O., & Merrill, M. Y. (1999). The conditions for tool use in primates: Implications for the evolution of material culture. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 19–41.
Zurück zum Zitat Vingerhoets, G., Vandamme, K., & Vercammen, A. (2009). Conceptual and physical object qualities contribute differently to motor affordances. Brain and Cognition, 69, 481–489.PubMed Vingerhoets, G., Vandamme, K., & Vercammen, A. (2009). Conceptual and physical object qualities contribute differently to motor affordances. Brain and Cognition, 69, 481–489.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Vingerhoets, G., Acke, F., Alderweireldt, A. S., Nys, J., Vandemaele, P., & Achten, E. (2012). Cerebral lateralization of praxis in right- and left-handedness: Same pattern, different strength. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 763–777.PubMed Vingerhoets, G., Acke, F., Alderweireldt, A. S., Nys, J., Vandemaele, P., & Achten, E. (2012). Cerebral lateralization of praxis in right- and left-handedness: Same pattern, different strength. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 763–777.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Vingerhoets, G., Alderweireldt, A. S., Vandemaele, P., Cai, Q., van der Haegen, L., Brysbaert, M., et al. (2013). Praxis and language are linked: Evidence from co-lateralization in individuals with atypical language dominance. Cortex, 49, 172–183.PubMed Vingerhoets, G., Alderweireldt, A. S., Vandemaele, P., Cai, Q., van der Haegen, L., Brysbaert, M., et al. (2013). Praxis and language are linked: Evidence from co-lateralization in individuals with atypical language dominance. Cortex, 49, 172–183.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Visalberghi, E., & Limongelli, L. (1994). Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 15–22.PubMed Visalberghi, E., & Limongelli, L. (1994). Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 15–22.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Visalberghi, E., & Trinca, L. (1989). Tool use in capuchin monkeys: Distinguishing between performing and understanding. Primates, 30, 511–521. Visalberghi, E., & Trinca, L. (1989). Tool use in capuchin monkeys: Distinguishing between performing and understanding. Primates, 30, 511–521.
Zurück zum Zitat Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Boston: Harvard University Press. Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Boston: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Wagman, J. B., & Abney, D. H. (2012). Transfer of recalibration from audition to touch: Modality independence as a special case of anatomical independence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 589–602.PubMed Wagman, J. B., & Abney, D. H. (2012). Transfer of recalibration from audition to touch: Modality independence as a special case of anatomical independence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 589–602.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2001). Affordances and inertial constraints on tool use. Ecological Psychology, 13, 173–195. Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2001). Affordances and inertial constraints on tool use. Ecological Psychology, 13, 173–195.
Zurück zum Zitat Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2003). Haptically creating affordances: The user-tool interface. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 175–186. Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2003). Haptically creating affordances: The user-tool interface. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 175–186.
Zurück zum Zitat Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 635–657.PubMed Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 635–657.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Waters, R. H. (1937). The principle of least effort in learning. Journal of General Psychology, 16, 3–20. Waters, R. H. (1937). The principle of least effort in learning. Journal of General Psychology, 16, 3–20.
Zurück zum Zitat Witt, J. K. (2011a). Action’s effect on perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 201–206. Witt, J. K. (2011a). Action’s effect on perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 201–206.
Zurück zum Zitat Witt, J. K. (2011b). Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serves as indirect measures of perceived distance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1148–1156.PubMed Witt, J. K. (2011b). Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serves as indirect measures of perceived distance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1148–1156.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1479–1492.PubMedCentralPubMed Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1479–1492.PubMedCentralPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2005). Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 880–888.PubMed Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2005). Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 880–888.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–1882.PubMed Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–1882.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Zadikoff, C., & Lang, A. E. (2005). Apraxia in movement disorders. Brain, 128, 1480–1497.PubMed Zadikoff, C., & Lang, A. E. (2005). Apraxia in movement disorders. Brain, 128, 1480–1497.PubMed
Metadaten
Titel
What Neuropsychology Tells us About Human Tool Use? The Four Constraints Theory (4CT): Mechanics, Space, Time, and Effort
verfasst von
François Osiurak
Publikationsdatum
01.06.2014
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Neuropsychology Review / Ausgabe 2/2014
Print ISSN: 1040-7308
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6660
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-014-9260-y

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2014

Neuropsychology Review 2/2014 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.