Skip to main content
Log in

Cultural Evolution: A Review of Theory, Findings and Controversies

  • Synthesis Paper
  • Published:
Evolutionary Biology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The last two decades have seen an explosion in research analysing cultural change as a Darwinian evolutionary process. Here I provide an overview of the theory of cultural evolution, including its intellectual history, major theoretical tenets and methods, key findings, and prominent criticisms and controversies. ‘Culture’ is defined as socially transmitted information. Cultural evolution is the theory that this socially transmitted information evolves in the manner laid out by Darwin in The Origin of Species, i.e. it comprises a system of variation, differential fitness and inheritance. Cultural evolution is not, however, neo-Darwinian, in that many of the details of genetic evolution may not apply, such as particulate inheritance and random mutation. Following a brief history of this idea, I review theoretical and empirical studies of cultural microevolution, which entails both selection-like processes wherein some cultural variants are more likely to be acquired and transmitted than others, plus transformative processes that alter cultural information during transmission. I also review how phylogenetic methods have been used to reconstruct cultural macroevolution, including the evolution of languages, technology and social organisation. Finally, I discuss recent controversies and debates, including the extent to which culture is proximate or ultimate, the relative role of selective and transformative processes in cultural evolution, the basis of cumulative cultural evolution, the evolution of large-scale human cooperation, and whether social learning is learned or innate. I conclude by highlighting the value of using evolutionary methods to study culture for both the social and biological sciences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Confusingly, the terms ‘social learning’, ‘social transmission’, ‘cultural transmission’, ‘cultural inheritance’ and variants thereof are used interchangeably within the field, to denote the passing of information non-genetically from one individual to another. Here I stick to the term ‘social learning’, although this may differ from cited sources.

  2. Some of this latter school (e.g. Claidière et al. 2014) have argued that the existence of these transformative processes requires a major revision of the standard approach to cultural evolution presented in this article; I deal with this critique separately in a later section.

  3. Earlier I discussed nineteenth century progressive Spencerian theories of cultural evolution. Currie et al.’s (2010) analysis presents an interesting empirical test of a version of those claims that societies increase in complexity, although it should be noted that (1) Currie et al.’s analysis is an empirical test, whereas Tylor and Morgan offered little empirical support for their progressive schemes; (2) Currie et al. precisely defined ‘complexity’ in terms of political hierarchy, whereas Tylor and Morgan were vague and conflated social organisation, technology and many other traits into a single scheme; and (3) Currie et al. showed that cultural evolution is not inevitably progressive, in that societies often lost social hierarchical levels.

References

  • Abbot, P., Abe, J., Alcock, J., Alizon, S., Alpedrinha, J. A. C., Andersson, M., et al. (2011). Inclusive fitness theory and eusociality. Nature, 471(7339), E1–E4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Acerbi, A., Enquist, M., & Ghirlanda, S. (2009). Cultural evolution and individual development of openness and conservatism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(45), 18931–18935.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Acerbi, A., & Mesoudi, A. (in press). If we are all cultural Darwinians what’s the fuss about? Clarifying recent disagreements in the field of cultural evolution. Biology and Philosophy.

  • Aoki, K., & Feldman, M. W. (2014). Evolution of learning strategies in temporally and spatially variable environments: A review of theory. Theoretical Population Biology, 91, 3–19. doi:10.1016/j.tpb.2013.10.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aplin, L. M., Farine, D. R., Morand-Ferron, J., Cockburn, A., Thornton, A., & Sheldon, B. C. (2014). Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature,. doi:10.1038/nature13998.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Atkisson, C., O’Brien, M. J., & Mesoudi, A. (2011). Adult learners in a novel environment use prestige-biased social learning. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(3), 519–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger, R. (2000). The life history of culture learning in a face-to-face society. Ethos, 28, 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barbrook, A. C., Howe, C. J., Blake, N., & Robinson, P. (1998). The phylogeny of the canterbury tales. Nature, 394(6696), 839.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Oxford: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beheim, B. A., Thigpen, C., & McElreath, R. (2014). Strategic social learning and the population dynamics of human behavior: The game of Go. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(5), 351–357. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.04.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, A. V., Richerson, P. J., & McElreath, R. (2009). Culture rather than genes provides greater scope for the evolution of large-scale human prosociality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(42), 17671–17674.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bloch, M. (2000). A well-disposed social anthropologist’s problems with memes. In R. Aunger (Ed.), Darwinizing culture (pp. 189–204). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boas, F. (1940). Race, language and culture. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouckaert, R., Lemey, P., Dunn, M., Greenhill, S. J., Alekseyenko, A. V., Drummond, A. J., et al. (2012). Mapping the origins and expansion of the Indo-European language family. Science, 337(6097), 957–960.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1995). Why does culture increase human adaptability? Ethology and Sociobiology, 16(2), 125–143. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(94)00073-G.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2009). Culture and the evolution of human cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364(1533), 3281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 10918–10925.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (1998). Cognitive tracks of cultural inheritance: How evolved intuitive ontology governs cultural transmission. American Anthropologist, 100(4), 876–889.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bromham, L., Hua, X., Fitzpatrick, T. G., & Greenhill, S. J. (2015). Rate of language evolution is affected by population size. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(7), 2097–2102. doi:10.1073/pnas.1419704112.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1965). Variation and selective retention in socio-cultural evolution. In H. R. Barringer, G. I. Blanksten, & R. W. Mack (Eds.), Social change in developing areas (pp. 19–49). Cambridge, MA: Schenkman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, W., Cathcart, C., Hall, D., & Garrett, A. (2015). Ancestry-constrained phylogenetic analysis supports the Indo-European steppe hypothesis. Language, 91(1), 194–244. doi:10.1353/lan.2015.0005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Claidière, N., Scott-Phillips, T. C., & Sperber, D. (2014). How Darwinian is cultural evolution? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1642), 20130368. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Claidière, N., & Sperber, D. (2007). The role of attraction in cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7(2), 89–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collard, M., Buchanan, Briggs, & O’Brien, M. J. (2013). Population size as an explanation for patterns in the Paleolithic archaeological record: More caution is needed. Current Anthropology, 54(S8), S388–S396. doi:10.1086/673881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collard, M., Shennan, S., & Tehrani, J. J. (2006). Branching, blending, and the evolution of cultural similarities and differences among human populations. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 169–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Currie, T. E., Greenhill, S. J., Gray, R. D., Hasegawa, T., & Mace, R. (2010). Rise and fall of political complexity in island South-East Asia and the Pacific. Nature, 467(7317), 801–804. doi:10.1038/nature09461.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danchin, E., Charmantier, A., Champagne, F. A., Mesoudi, A., Pujol, B., & Blanchet, S. (2011). Beyond DNA: Integrating inclusive inheritance into an extended theory of evolution. Nature Reviews Genetics, 12, 475–486.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man. London: Gibson Square.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, E. H., Avarguès-Weber, A., Chittka, L., & Leadbeater, E. (2013). Learning by observation emerges from simple associations in an insect model. Current Biology, 23(8), 727–730. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.035.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335(6072), 1114–1118.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dean, L. G., Vale, G. L., Laland, K. N., Flynn, E., & Kendal, R. L. (2014). Human cumulative culture: A comparative perspective. Biological Reviews, 89(2), 284–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derex, M., Beugin, M.-P., Godelle, B., & Raymond, M. (2013). Experimental evidence for the influence of group size on cultural complexity. Nature, 503(7476), 389–391.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, J., & Bellwood, P. (2003). Farmers and their languages: The first expansions. Science, 300(5619), 597–603.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dickins, T. E., & Rahman, Q. (2012). The extended evolutionary synthesis and the role of soft inheritance in evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B,. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.0273.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). The social brain. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 163–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, M., Greenhill, S. J., Levinson, S. C., & Gray, R. D. (2011). Evolved structure of language shows lineage-specific trends in word-order universals. Nature, 473(7345), 79–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • El Mouden, C., André, J.-B., Morin, O., & Nettle, D. (2014). Cultural transmission and the evolution of human behaviour: A general approach based on the Price equation. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 27(2), 231–241. doi:10.1111/jeb.12296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist, M., Eriksson, K., & Ghirlanda, S. (2007). Critical social learning: A solution to Rogers’ paradox of nonadaptive culture. American Anthropologist, 109(4), 727–734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist, M., & Ghirlanda, S. (2007). Evolution of social learning does not explain the origin of human cumulative culture. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 246(1), 129–135. doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2006.12.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist, M., Ghirlanda, S., & Eriksson, K. (2011). Modelling the evolution and diversity of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 412–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksson, K., & Coultas, J. C. (2014). Corpses, maggots, poodles and rats: Emotional selection operating in three phases of cultural transmission of urban legends. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 14(1–2), 1–26. doi:10.1163/15685373-12342107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feher, O., Wang, H., Saar, S., Mitra, P. P., & Tchernichovski, O. (2009). De novo establishment of wild-type song culture in the zebra finch. Nature, 459(7246), 564–568.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, M. W., & Laland, K. N. (1996). Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 11(11), 453–457.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Felsenstein, J. (1985). Phylogenies and the comparative method. American Naturalist, 125, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fracchia, J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1999). Does culture evolve? History and Theory, 38(4), 52–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, D. (1974). The evolutionary theories of Charles Darwin and Herbert Spencer. Current Anthropology, 15(3), 211–237.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galef, B. G., & Laland, K. N. (2005). Social learning in animals: Empirical studies and theoretical models. BioScience, 55(6), 489–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghirlanda, S., Enquist, M., & Nakamaru, M. (2006). Cultural evolution develops its own rules: The rise of conservatism and persuasion. Current Anthropology, 47(6), 1027–1034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, R. D., & Atkinson, Q. D. (2003). Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin. Nature, 426(6965), 435–439.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, R. D., Drummond, A. J., & Greenhill, S. J. (2009). Language phylogenies reveal expansion pulses and pauses in Pacific settlement. Science, 323(5913), 479–483. doi:10.1126/science.1166858.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, R. D., & Jordan, F. M. (2000). Language trees support the express-train sequence of Austronesian expansion. Nature, 405, 1052–1055.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, T. L., Kalish, M. L., & Lewandowsky, S. (2008). Theoretical and empirical evidence for the impact of inductive biases on cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 3503–3514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child’s environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, P. H., & Pagel, M. D. (1991). The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath, C., Bell, C., & Sternberg, E. (2001). Emotional selection in memes: The case of urban legends. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 1028–1041. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2001). Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations. American Anthropologist, 103(4), 992–1013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses—The Tasmanian case. American Antiquity, 69(2), 197–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19(4), 215–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (2001). Why people punish defectors: Weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 208(1), 79–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (2002). On modeling cognition and culture: Why cultural evolution does not require replication of representations. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 2(2), 87–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., et al. (2005). “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(06), 795–815. doi:10.1017/S0140525X05000142.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(3), 165–196. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00071-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Henrich, N. (2010). The evolution of cultural adaptations: Fijian food taboos protect against dangerous marine toxins. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 3715–3724.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12(3), 123–135. doi:10.1002/evan.10110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. S., Fouts, H. N., Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. L. (2011). Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1168–1178. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0373.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2012). What’s social about social learning? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 193–202. doi:10.1037/a0025180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C., & Pearce, J. M. (2015). Not-so-social learning strategies. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282(1802), 20141709. doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.1709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holden, C. J., & Mace, R. (2003). Spread of cattle led to the loss of matrilineal descent in Africa: A coevolutionary analysis. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 270(1532), 2425–2433.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Horner, V., Whiten, A., Flynn, E., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(37), 13878.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kandler, A., & Steele, J. (2009). Social learning, economic inequality and innovation diffusion. In M. O’Brien & S. J. Shennan (Eds.), Innovation and evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kashima, Y. (2000). Maintaining cultural stereotypes in the serial reproduction of narratives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(5), 594–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempe, M., Lycett, S., & Mesoudi, A. (2012). An experimental test of the accumulated copying error model of cultural mutation for Acheulean handaxe size. PLoS One, 7(11), e48333.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kempe, M., Lycett, S. J., & Mesoudi, A. (2014). From cultural traditions to cumulative culture: Parameterizing the differences between human and nonhuman culture. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 359, 29–36. doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2014.05.046.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kempe, M., & Mesoudi, A. (2014). An experimental demonstration of the effect of group size on cultural accumulation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 285–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(31), 10681–10686.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S., Dowman, M., & Griffiths, T. L. (2007). Innateness and culture in the evolution of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(12), 5241–5245.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, M. A., & Boyd, R. (2010). Population size predicts technological complexity in Oceania. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1693), 2559–2564.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi, Y., & Aoki, K. (2012). Innovativeness, population size and cumulative cultural evolution. Theoretical Population Biology, 82(1), 38–47. doi:10.1016/j.tpb.2012.04.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W. (1972). Sociolinguistic patterns. Oxford: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Animal Learning and Behavior, 32(1), 4–14. doi:10.3758/BF03196002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Galef, B. G. (2009). The question of animal culture. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Myles, S. (2010). How culture shaped the human genome: Bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11(2), 137–148.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Sterelny, K., Odling-Smee, J., Hoppitt, W., & Uller, T. (2011). Cause and effect in biology revisited: Is Mayr’s proximate-ultimate dichotomy still useful? Science, 334(6062), 1512–1516.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamba, S. (2014). Social learning in cooperative dilemmas. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 281(1787), 20140417. doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.0417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamba, S., & Mace, R. (2011). Demography and ecology drive variation in cooperation across human populations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(35), 14426–14430.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leadbeater, E. (2015). What evolves in the evolution of social learning? Journal of Zoology, 295(1), 4–11. doi:10.1111/jzo.12197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann, L., Aoki, K., & Feldman, M. W. (2011). On the number of independent cultural traits carried by individuals and populations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1563), 424–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann, L., Feldman, M. W., & Foster, K. R. (2008). Cultural transmission can inhibit the evolution of altruistic helping. The American Naturalist, 172(1), 12–24. doi:10.1086/587851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, H. S. (2001). Boas, Darwin, science, and anthropology. Current Anthropology, 42(3), 381–406. doi:10.1086/320474.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, H. M., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 2171–2180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewontin, R. C. (1970). The units of selection. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, E., Michel, J. B., Jackson, J., Tang, T., & Nowak, M. A. (2007). Quantifying the evolutionary dynamics of language. Nature, 449(7163), 713–716.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lindeyer, C. M., Meaney, M. J., & Reader, S. M. (2013). Early maternal care predicts reliance on social learning about food in adult rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(2), 168–175. doi:10.1002/dev.21009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lipo, C. P., O’Brien, M. J., Collard, M., & Shennan, S. J. (Eds.). (2006). Mapping our ancestors: Phylogenetic approaches in anthropology and prehistory. New York: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lycett, S. J. (2009). Understanding ancient hominin dispersals using artefactual data: A phylogeographic analysis of Acheulean handaxes. PLoS One, 4(10), 1–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lycett, S. J., Collard, M., & McGrew, W. C. (2007). Phylogenetic analyses of behavior support existence of culture among wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(45), 17588.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lycett, S. J., & von Cramon-Taubadel, N. (2008). Acheulean variability and hominin dispersals: A model-bound approach. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35(3), 553–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(50), 19751–19756.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, R., & Pagel, M. D. (1994). The comparative method in anthropology. Current Anthropology, 35(5), 549–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall-Pescini, S., & Whiten, A. (2008). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and the question of cumulative culture: An experimental approach. Animal Cognition, 11(3), 449–456.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, D., Hutchison, J., Slessor, G., Urquhart, J., Cunningham, S. J., & Smith, K. (2014). The spontaneous formation of stereotypes via cumulative cultural evolution. Psychological Science, 25(9), 1777–1786. doi:10.1177/0956797614541129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, L. J., Tehrani, J. J., Jordan, F. M., Collard, M., & Nunn, C. L. (2011). Testing for divergent transmission histories among cultural characters: A study using Bayesian phylogenetic methods and Iranian tribal textile data. PLoS One, 6(4), e14810. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0014810.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and effect in biology: Kinds of causes, predictability, and teleology are viewed by a practicing biologist. Science, 134(3489), 1501–1506. doi:10.1126/science.134.3489.1501.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1982). The growth of biological thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McElreath, R., Bell, A. V., Efferson, C., Lubell, M., Richerson, P. J., & Waring, T. (2008). Beyond existence and aiming outside the laboratory: Estimating frequency-dependent and pay-off-biased social learning strategies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 3515–3528. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McElreath, R., & Henrich, J. (2006). Modeling cultural evolution. In R. I. M. Dunbar & L. Barrett (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuigan, N., & Cubillo, M. (2013). Cultural transmission in young children: When social information is more important than non-social information. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 174(6), 605–619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McMahon, A., & McMahon, R. (2003). Finding families: Quantitative methods in language classification. Transactions of the Philological Society, 101, 7–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2011a). Cultural evolution. Chicago, IL: University Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2011b). An experimental comparison of human social learning strategies: Payoff-biased social learning is adaptive but underused. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(5), 334–342. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.12.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2011c). Variable cultural acquisition costs constrain cumulative cultural evolution. PLoS One, 6(3), e18239.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Blanchet, S., Charmantier, A., Danchin, E., Fogarty, L., Jablonka, E., et al. (2013). Is non-genetic inheritance just a proximate mechanism? A corroboration of the extended evolutionary synthesis. Biological Theory, 7(3), 189–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Chang, L., Murray, K., & Lu, H. (2015). Higher frequency of social learning in China than in the West shows cultural variation in the dynamics of cultural evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 282, 20142209.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2004). The hierarchical transformation of event knowledge in human cultural transmission. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4(1), 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2008). The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 3489–3501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Dunbar, R. (2006). A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology, 97(3), 405–423. doi:10.1348/000712605X85871.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2004). Is human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of The Origin of Species. Evolution, 58(1), 1–11. Accessed 27 May 2014.

  • Molleman, L., van den Berg, P., & Weissing, F. J. (2014). Consistent individual differences in human social learning strategies. Nature Communications,. doi:10.1038/ncomms4570.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J. H. (1994). Putting anthropology back together again: The ethnogenetic critique of cladistic theory. American Anthropologist, 96, 925–948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, L. H. (1877). Ancient society. New York: Henry Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, T. J. H., Rendell, L. E., Ehn, M., Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 653–662. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.1172.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Muthukrishna, M., Shulman, B. W., Vasilescu, V., & Henrich, J. (2014). Sociality influences cultural complexity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1774), 20132511.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nowak, M. A., Tarnita, C. E., & Wilson, E. O. (2010). The evolution of eusociality. Nature, 466(7310), 1057–1062.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., Boulanger, M. T., Buchanan, B., Collard, M., Lee Lyman, R., & Darwent, J. (2014). Innovation and cultural transmission in the American Paleolithic: Phylogenetic analysis of eastern Paleoindian projectile-point classes. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 34, 100–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., Darwent, J., & Lyman, R. L. (2001). Cladistics is useful for reconstructing archaeological phylogenies: Palaeoindian points from the Southeastern United States. Journal of Archaeological Science, 28(10), 1115–1136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., & Lyman, R. L. (2003). Cladistics and archaeology. Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pagel, M. (2009). Human language as a culturally transmitted replicator. Nature Reviews Genetics, 10(6), 405–415.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, T. W., Kendal, J. R., Rendell, L. E., & Laland, K. N. (2010). Learning by proportional observation in a species of fish. Behavioral Ecology, 21(3), 570–575. doi:10.1093/beheco/arq025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, T. W., & Laland, K. N. (2010). Conformist learning in nine-spined sticklebacks’ foraging decisions. Biology Letters,. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.1014.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pitt-Rivers, A. L. (1875). On the evolution of culture. Journal of the Anthropological Institute, 4, 293–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, A., Shennan, S. J., & Thomas, M. G. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324(5932), 1298–1301.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Price, G. R. (1970). Selection and covariance. Nature, 227(5257), 520–521.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-Garcia, V., Broesch, J., Calvet-Mir, L., Fuentes-Pel·ez, N., McDade, T. W., Parsa, S., et al. (2009). Cultural transmission of ethnobotanical knowledge and skills: An empirical analysis from an Amerindian society. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(4), 274–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reysen, M. B., Talbert, N. G., Dominko, M., Jones, A. N., & Kelley, M. R. (2011). The effects of collaboration on recall of social information. British Journal of Psychology, 102(3), 646–661. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02035.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., Baldini, R., Bell, A., Demps, K., Frost, K., Hillis, V., et al. (2015). Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,. doi:10.1017/S0140525X1400106X.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (1987). Simple models of complex phenomena: The case of cultural evolution. In J. Dupre (Ed.), The latest on the best: Essays on evolution and optimality (pp. 27–52). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Christiansen, M. H. (2013). Cultural evolution: Society, technology, language, and religion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, A. R. (1988). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90(4), 819–831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, E. (1995). The diffusion of innovations. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, R. M., Greenhill, S. J., & Atkinson, Q. D. (2013). Population structure and cultural geography of a folktale in Europe. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 280(1756), 20123065. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.3065.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, B., & Gross, N. (1943). The diffusion of hybrid seed corn in two Iowa communities. Rural Sociology, 8, 15–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlag, K. H. (1998). Why imitate, and if so, how? A boundedly rational approach to multi-armed bandits. Journal of Economic Theory, 78(1), 130–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott-Phillips, T. C., Dickins, T. E., & West, S. A. (2011). Evolutionary theory and the ultimate–proximate distinction in the human behavioral sciences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 38–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Servedio, M. R., Brandvain, Y., Dhole, S., Fitzpatrick, C. L., Goldberg, E. E., Stern, C. A., et al. (2014). Not just a theory—The utility of mathematical models in evolutionary biology. PLoS Biology, 12(12), e1002017. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002017.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. J. (2001). Demography and cultural innovation: A model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 11(01), 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. J. (2002). Genes, memes and human history. London: Thames and Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., & Hirschfeld, L. A. (2004). The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 40–46. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stubbersfield, J. M., Tehrani, J. J., & Flynn, E. G. (2014). Serial killers, spiders and cybersex: Social and survival information bias in the transmission of urban legends. British Journal of Psychology,. doi:10.1111/bjop.12073.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Syvanen, M. (2012). Evolutionary implications of horizontal gene transfer. Annual Review of Genetics, 46(1), 341–358. doi:10.1146/annurev-genet-110711-155529.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tehrani, J. J. (2013). The phylogeny of little red riding hood. PLoS One, 8(11), e78871. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078871.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tehrani, J. J., & Collard, M. (2002). Investigating cultural evolution through biological phylogenetic analyses of Turkmen textiles. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 21, 443–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675–691.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 495–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turchin, P. (2008). Arise “cliodynamics”. Nature, 454, 34–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turchin, P., Currie, T. E., Turner, E. A. L., & Gavrilets, S. (2013). War, space, and the evolution of Old World complex societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(41), 16384–16389. doi:10.1073/pnas.1308825110.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive culture. London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaesen, K. (2012). Cumulative cultural evolution and demography. PLoS One, 7(7), e40989. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0040989.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., & Haun, D. (2013). Conformity in nonhuman primates: Fad or fact? Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(1), 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Wyhe, J. (2005). The descent of words: Evolutionary thinking 1780–1880. Endeavour, 29(3), 94–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • West, S. A., El Mouden, C., & Gardner, A. (2011). Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 231–262. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007). Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 20(2), 415–432.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A. (2005). The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans. Nature, 437, 52–55.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399(6737), 682–685.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Horner, V., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature, 437, 737–740.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., & Mesoudi, A. (2008). An experimental science of culture: Animal social diffusion experiments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 3477–3488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., & Wilson, E. O. (2007). Rethinking the theoretical foundation of sociobiology. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 82(4), 327–348. doi:10.1086/522809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, J., Dowman, M., & Griffiths, T. L. (2013). Cultural transmission results in convergence towards colour term universals. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1758). doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.3073.

  • Yamamoto, S., Humle, T., & Tanaka, M. (2013). Basis for cumulative cultural evolution in chimpanzees: Social learning of a more efficient tool-use technique. PLoS One, 8(1), e55768.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alex Mesoudi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mesoudi, A. Cultural Evolution: A Review of Theory, Findings and Controversies. Evol Biol 43, 481–497 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11692-015-9320-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11692-015-9320-0

Keywords

Navigation