Skip to main content
Log in

The Anatomy of Motivation: An Evolutionary-Ecological Approach

  • Long Article
  • Published:
Biological Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There have been few attempts to bring evolutionary theory to the study of human motivation. From this perspective motives can be considered psychological mechanisms to produce behavior that solves evolutionarily important tasks in the human niche. From the dimensions of the human niche we deduce eight human needs: optimize the number and survival of gene copies; maintain bodily integrity; avoid external threats; optimize sexual, environmental, and social capital; and acquire reproductive and survival skills. These needs then serve as the foundation for a necessary and sufficient list of 15 human motives, which we label: lust, hunger, comfort, fear, disgust, attract, love, nurture, create, hoard, affiliate, status, justice, curiosity, and play. We show that these motives are consistent with evidence from the current literature. This approach provides us with a precise vocabulary for talking about motivation, the lack of which has hampered progress in behavioral science. Developing testable theories about the structure and function of motives is essential to the project of understanding the organization of animal cognition and learning, as well as for the applied behavioral sciences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson J, Lux W (2004) Knowing your own strength: accurate self-assessment as a requirement for personal autonomy. Philos Psychiatr Psychol 11:279–294

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ash DW, Holding DH (1990) Backward versus forward chaining in the acquisition of a keyboard skill. Hum Factors 32:139–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson JW (1964) An introduction to motivation. Van Nostrand, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger R, Curtis V (2008) Kinds of behaviour. Biol Philos 23:317–345

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura A (1999) Social cognitive theory of personality. In: Pervin LA, John OP (eds) Handbook of personality: theory and research, 2nd edn. Guilford Press, New York, pp 154–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh JA, Chartrand TL (1999) The unbearable automaticity of being. Am Psychol 54:462–479

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes T, Kubota Y, Hu D, Jin DZ, Graybiel A (2005) Activity of striatal neurons reflects dynamic encoding and recoding of procedural memories. Nature 437:1158–1161

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett LF, Russell JA (1999) Structure of current affect. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 8:10–14

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barton RA (2008) Brain modules: mosaic evolution. In: Squire LR (ed) Encyclopedia of neuroscience, vol 2. Academic Press, New York, pp 389–394

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister RF, Leary MR (1995) The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol Bull 117:497–529

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentham J (1983) Deontology together with a table of the springs of action and the article on utilitarianism. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernard LC, Mills M, Swenson L, Walsh RP (2005) An evolutionary theory of human motivation. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr 131:129–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernard LC, Mills M, Swenson L, Walsh RP (2008) Measuring motivation multidimensionally: development of the assessment of individual motives questionnaire (AIM-Q). Assessment 15:16–35

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge KC (2003) Comparing the emotional brain of humans to other animals. In: Davidson RJ, Goldsmith HH, Scherer KR (eds) Handbook of affective sciences. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Beswick DG (1971) Cognitive process theory of individual differences in curiosity. In: Day HI, Berlyne DE, Hunt DE (eds) Intrinsic motivation: a new direction in education. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Toronto, pp 156–170

  • Bindra D (1978) How adaptive behaviour is produced: a perceptual-motivational alternative to response reinforcement. Behav Brain Sci 1:41–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block J (1995) A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description. Psychol Bull 117:187–215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bugental D (2000) Acquisition of the algorithms of social life: a domain-based approach. Psychol Bull 126:187–219

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt GM (2005) The genesis of animal play. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss D (2003) The evolution of desire, 2nd edn. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss DM, Dedden L (1990) Derogation of competitors. J Soc Pers Relat 7:395–422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver C, Scheier MF (1981) Attention and self-regulation: a control theory approach to human behavior. Springer, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Carver CS, Scheier MF (1998) On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell RB (1957) Personality and motivation structure and measurement. World Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen Y, Dammers J, Boers F, Leiberg S, Edgar J, Roberts T, Mathiak K (2009) The temporal dynamics of insula activity to disgust and happy facial expressions: a magnetoencephalography study. Neuroimage 47:1921–1928

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chulef AS, Read SJ, Walsh DA (2001) A hierarchical taxonomy of human goals. Motiv Emot 3(25):191–232

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland PS, Sejnowski TJ (1992) The computational brain. MIT Press, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Clutton-Brock TH (1991) The evolution of parental care. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides L, Tooby J (2005) Neurocognitive adaptations designed for social exchange. In: Buss DM (ed) The handbook of evolutionary psychology. Wiley, New York, pp 584–627

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa PT Jr, McCrae RR (1992) Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: the NEO Personality Inventory. Psychol Assessment 4:5–13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis V, Biran A (2001) Dirt, disgust and disease. Perspect Biol Med 44:17–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis V, Aunger R, Rabie T (2004) Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proc R Soc London B 277:131–133

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis V, de Barra M, Aunger R (2011) Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour. Phil Trans R Soc B 366:389–401

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cziko G (2000) The things we do: using the insights of Bernard and Darwin to understand the what, how, and why of behavior. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahl E (2004) Social status versus formal rank of medical and other officers: an informal survey among passengers and seafarers on cruise ships. Int Maritime Health 55:165–175

    Google Scholar 

  • Dall SRX, Giraldeau L-A, Olsson O, McNamara JM, Stephens DW (2005) Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol Evol 20:187–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio AR (1994) Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. Putnam, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies N, Krebs JR, West S (2012) An introduction to behavioral ecology, 4th edn. Wiley-Blackwell, Chichester

    Google Scholar 

  • de Quervain D, Fischbacher U, Treyer V, Schellhammer M, Schnyder U, Buck A, Fehr E (2004) The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science 305:1254–1258

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci EL, Ryan RM (2000) The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol Inq 11:227–268

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denissen JJA, Penke L (2008) Individual reaction norms underlying the Five Factor Model of personality: first steps towards a theory-based conceptual framework. J Res Pers 42:1285–1302

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Depret E, Fiske ST (1993) Social cognition and power: some cognitive consequences of social structure as a source of control deprivation. In: Weary G, Gleicher F, Marsh KL (eds) Control motivation and social cognition. Springer, New York, pp 176–202

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson A (2012) Associative learning and animal cognition. Phil Trans R Soc B 367:2733–2742

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doyal L, Gough I (1991) A theory of human need. Macmillan, Basingstoke

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar R (1998) Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck CS (1986) Motivational processes affecting learning. Am Psychol 41:1040–1048

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellyson SL, Dovidio JF (1985) Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior. Springer, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Gachter S (2002) Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415:137–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein M, Ajzen I (1975) Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: an introduction to theory and research. Addison–Wesley, Reading

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske ST (2002) Five core social motives, plus or minus five. In: Spencer S, Fein S, Zanna M, Olson J (eds) Motivated social perception. The Ontario Symposium, vol 9. Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 233–246

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske ST, Berdahl JL (2007) Social power. In: Kruglanski A, Higgins ET (eds) Social psychology: a handbook of basic principles. Guilford Press, New York, pp 678–692

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox R (1984) Kinship and marriage: an anthropological perspective. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman WJ (1999) How the brain makes up its mind. Weidenfeld and Nicholson, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary DC, Flinn MV (2001) Evolution of human parental behavior and the human family. Parent Sci Pract 1:5–61

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H (2007) The evolution of private property. J Econ Behav Organ 64:1–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H (2009) The bounds of reason: game theory and the unification of the behavioral sciences. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith P (1996) Complexity and the function of mind in nature. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Goffman E (1956) Embarrassment and social organization. Am J Sociol 62:264–271

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray J (1988) The psychology of fear and stress, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths PE (1997) What emotions really are: the problem of psychological categories. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt J (2003) Elevation and the positive psychology of morality. In: Keyes CLM, Haidt J (eds) Flourishing: positive psychology and the life well-lived. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp 275–289

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt J (2012) The righteous mind: why good people are divided by politics and religion. Pantheon, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall JL, Stanton SJ, Schultheiss OC (2013) Biopsychological and neural processes of implicit motivation. In: Schultheiss OC, Brunstein JC (eds) Implicit motives. Oxford University Press, New York (in press)

  • Hamilton WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour I and II. J Theor Biol 7:1–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebb DO (1949) The organization of behavior. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen H (2008) Historical trends in motivation research. In: Heckhausen J, Heckhausen H (eds) Motivation and action. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 10–41

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hennenlotter A, Schroeder U (2006) Partly dissociable neural substrates for recognizing basic emotions: a critical review. Progr Brain Res 156:443–456

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Gil-White F (2001) The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evol Hum Behav 22:165–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hull CL (1943) Principles of behavior: an introduction to behavior theory. Appleton-Century, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Inglis IR (2000) Review: The central role of uncertainty reduction in determining behaviour. Behaviour 137:1567–1599

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James W (1890) The principles of psychology. Henry Holt, Boston

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe P, Blass EM (2004) Opioid-mediation of separation distress in 10-day-old rats: reversal of stress with maternal stimuli. Dev Psychobiol 19:385–398

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick DT, Maner JK, Butner J, Li NP, Becker V, Schaller M (2002) Dynamical evolutionary psychology: mapping the domains of the new interactionist paradigm. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 6:347–356

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick DT, Li NP, Butner J (2003) Dynamical evolutionary psychology: Individual decision rules and emergent social norms. Psych Rev 110:3–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick DT, Griskevicius V, Neuberg SL, Schaller M (2010) Renovating the pyramid of needs: contemporary extensions built upon ancient foundations. Perspect Psychol Sci 5:292–314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knight C (1995) Blood relations: menstruation and the origins of culture. Yale University Press, New Haven

    Google Scholar 

  • Koechlin E, Summerfield C (2007) An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends Cogn Sci 11:229–235

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lande R (1982) A quantitative genetic theory of life history evolution. Ecology 63:607–615

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence AD, Murphy FC, Calder AJ (2004) Dissociating fear and disgust: implications for the structure of emotions. In: Yiend J (ed) Cognition, emotion and psychopathology: theoretical, empirical and clinical directions. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 149–171

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Llinas R (2002) I of the vortex: from neurons to self. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein G (1994) The psychology of curiosity: a review and reinterpretation. Psychol Bull 116:75–98

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ludvico LR, Kurland JA (1995) Symbolic or not-so-symbolic wounds: the behavioral ecology of human scarification. Ethol Sociobiol 16:155–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maslow AH (1954) Motivation and personality. Harper, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer JD, Faber MA, Xu X (2007) Seventy-five years of motivation measures (1930–2005): a descriptive analysis. Motiv Emotion 31:83–103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland DC (1987) Human motivation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland DC, Franz CE (1992) Motivational and other sources of work accomplishments in mid-life: a longitudinal study. J Pers 60:679–707

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland DC, Watson RI (1973) Power motivation and risk-taking behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 41:121–139

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland DC, Atkinson JW, Clark RA, Lowell EL (1953) The achievement motive. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McDougall W (1908) An introduction to social psychology. Methuen, London

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Miller G (2009) Spent: sex, evolution and consumer behaviour. Viking Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll J, Zahn R, Oliveira-Souza RD, Krueger F, Grafman J (2005) The neural basis of human moral cognition. Nat Neurosci 6:799–809

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray HA (1938) Explorations in personality. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesse RM (1990) Evolutionary explanations of emotion. Hum Nature 1:261–289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nettle D, Clegg H (2006) Schizotypy, creativity and mating success in humans. Proc R Soc B 273:611–615

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nissen HW (1930) A study of exploratory behavior in the white rat by means of the obstruction method. J Gen Psychol 37:361–376

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowak MA, Sigmund K (2005) Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature 437:1291–1298

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odling-smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman M (2003) Niche construction: the neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton

  • Oettingen G, Bulgarella C, Henderson M, Gollwitzer PM (2004) The self-regulation of goal pursuit. In: Wright RA, Greenberg J, Brehm SS (eds) Motivation and emotion in social contexts: Jack Brehm’s influence on contemporary psychological thought. Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 225–244

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohman A, Mineka S (2003) The malicious serpent: snakes as a prototypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 12:5–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe J, Nadel L (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp J (1998) Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp J, Herman BH, Conner R, Bishop P, Scott JP (1978) The biology of social attachments: opiates alleviate separation distress. Biol Psychiat 13:607–618

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips ML, Young AW, Senior C, Brammer M, Andrew C, Calder AJ, Bullmore ET, Perrett DI, Rowland D, Williams SCR, Gray JA, David AS (1997) A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature 389:495–498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pittman TS, Zeigler KR (2006) Basic human needs. In: Kruglanski A, Higgins E (eds) Social psychology: a handbook of basic principles, 2nd edn. Guilford Press, New York, pp 473–489

    Google Scholar 

  • Plourde AM (2008) The origins of prestige goods as honest signals of skill and knowledge. Hum Nature 19:374–388

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price ME, Cosmides L, Tooby J (2002) Punitive sentiment as an anti-free rider psychological device. Evol Hum Behav 23:203–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinlan RJ, Quinlan MB (2007) Evolutionary ecology of human pair-bonds: cross-cultural tests of alternative hypotheses. Cross-Cult Res 41:149–169

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen J (1983) Skill, rules, and knowledge; signals, signs, and symbols, and other distinctions in human performance models. Paper presented at the IEEE transactions on systems, man and cybernetics

  • Reeve J (2005) Understanding motivation and emotion. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiss S (2008) The normal personality: a new way of thinking about people. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Reiss S, Havercamp SH (1998) Toward a comprehensive assessment of fundamental motivation. Factor structure of the Reiss profiles. Psychol Assessment 10:97–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson P, Boyd R (1998) The evolution of human ultrasociality. In: Eibl-Eibesfeldt I, Salter F (eds) Indoctrinability, ideology, and warfare. Berghahn Books, New York, pp 71–95

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (1999) The brain and emotion. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell JA (2003) Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psych Rev 110:145–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan R, Deci E (2006) Self-regulation and the problem of human autonomy: does psychology need choice, self-determination, and will? J Pers 74:1557–1585

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schachter S (1959) The psychology of affiliation: experimental studies of the sources of gregariousness. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss OC (2006) Needs. In: Greenhouse JH, Callanan GA (eds) Encyclopedia of career development, vol 1. Sage, Thousand Oaks, pp 532–535

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss OC (2008) Implicit motives. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA (eds) Handbook of personality: theory and research, 3rd edn. Guilford Press, New York, pp 603–633

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzer R (1992) Self efficacy in the adoption and maintenance of health behaviours: theoretical approaches and a new model. In: Schwarzer R (ed) Self efficacy: thought control of action. Hemisphere, Washington, DC, pp 217–243

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon KM, Elliot AJ, Kim Y, Kasser T (2001) What is satisfying about satisfying events? Testing 10 candidate psychological needs. J Pers Soc Psychol 80:325–339

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sloman A (2001) Beyond shallow models of emotion. Cogn Process 2:177–198

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprengelmeyer R, Rausch M, Eysel UT, Przuntek H (1998) Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions. Proc R Soc Lond B 265:1927–1931

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Squire LR (2004) Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective. Neurobiol Learn Mem 82:171–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starch D (1923) Principles of advertising. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephens DW, Krebs JR (1987) Foraging theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Streidter GF (2005) The principles of brain evolution. Sinauer, Sunderland

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson LW (2003) Brain architecture: understanding the basic plan. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Tangney JP, Stuewig J, Mashek DJ (2007) Moral emotions and moral behavior. Ann Rev Psychol 58:345–372

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toates F (1986) Motivational systems. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby J, Cosmides L (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In: Barkow JH, Cosmides L, Tooby J (eds) The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 19–136

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby J, Cosmides L, Sell A, Lieberman D, Sznycer D (2008) Internal regulatory variables and the design of human motivation: a computational and evolutionary approach. In: Elliot AJ (ed) Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation. Psychology Press, New York, pp 251–271

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers RL (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell B (ed) Sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871–1971. Aldine, Chicago, pp 136–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving E (1985) How many memory systems are there? Am Psychol 40:385–398

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westen D (1996) A model and a method for uncovering the nomothetic from the idiographic: an alternative to the Five-Factor Model? J Res Pers 30:400–413

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiecki TV, Frank MJ (2011) A computational model of executive control in frontal cortex and basal ganglia: multiple levels of analysis. arXiv preprint arXiv:1112.0778

  • Willer R (2009) Groups reward individual sacrifice: the status solution to the collective action problem. Am Sociol Rev 74:23–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willis WD (1985) The pain system: the neural basis of nociceptive transmission in the mammalian nervous system. Kargel, Basel

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson T (2004) Strangers to ourselves: discovering the adaptive unconscious. Belknap Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson M, Daly M (1985) Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: the young male syndrome. Ethol Sociobiol 6:59–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winter DG (1973) The power motive. Free Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Winter DG (1996) Personality: analysis and interpretation of lives. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Larry Bernard, Alison Bish, Mícheál de Barra, Diana Fleischman, Gaby Judah, Mark Schaller, and two anonymous referees for reading earlier versions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Valerie Curtis.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (PDF 242 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Aunger, R., Curtis, V. The Anatomy of Motivation: An Evolutionary-Ecological Approach. Biol Theory 8, 49–63 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-013-0101-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-013-0101-7

Keywords

Navigation