Sometimes frequently means seldom: Context effects in the interpretation of quantitative expressions

https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(74)90049-XGet rights and content

Abstract

Subjects supplied numerical definitions for five quantitative expressions embedded in two high-frequency contexts, one moderate-frequency context, two low-frequency contexts, and with no context specified. Mean definitions differed significantly across contexts and expressions with a Context-Expression interaction (p < .01). The variance in definition of an expression embedded in a context generally increased with the discrepancy between its no-context definition and the context event's estimated frequency (p < .05). Variance in the definitions of the sampled expressions gathered over diverse contexts increased with the expression's no-context definition (p < .05). Thus, the mean and variance of an expression's numerical definitions depended upon both the expression and its context.

References (3)

  • M.D. Hakel

    How often is often?

    American Psychologist

    (1968)
There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (67)

  • I know what you're probably going to say: Listener adaptation to variable use of uncertainty expressions

    2020, Cognition
    Citation Excerpt :

    Uncertainty expressions have several properties that make them a good testing ground for studying semantic and pragmatic adaptation. First, there is no consistent mapping between uncertainty expressions and event probabilities (e.g., Clark, 1990; Pepper & Prytulak, 1974), which suggests that listeners have to rely on additional contextual information (such as speaker identity) if they want to infer an event probability that a speaker intended to communicate using an uncertainty expression. Second, there is considerable inter-speaker variability in the use of these expressions (Wallsten et al., 1986) and therefore it is likely that listeners expect different speakers to use these expressions differently.

  • Communicating and reasoning with verbal probability expressions

    2018, Psychology of Learning and Motivation - Advances in Research and Theory
    Citation Excerpt :

    This evidence comes overwhelmingly from translation and membership-function studies. Numerical translation is, for instance, affected by perceived base rates (Fischer & Jungermann, 1996; Pepper & Prytulak, 1974; Wallsten, Fillenbaum, & Cox, 1986; Weber & Hilton, 1990). For instance, translations are higher for “probable snow” in December than October (Wallsten, Fillenbaum, et al., 1986).

  • Talker-specificity and adaptation in quantifier interpretation

    2016, Journal of Memory and Language
    Citation Excerpt :

    We close by discussing venues for future research on lexical adaptation that, we think, are facilitated by the current paradigm. It is well-known that there are gradient context-dependent differences in the interpretation of quantifiers (e.g., Hörmann, 1983; Newstead, 1988; Pepper & Prytclak, 1974). It is less clear, however, whether talkers differ in their use of quantifiers.

  • Communicating quantities: A psychological perspective

    2023, Communicating Quantities: A Psychological Perspective
View all citing articles on Scopus

The data described in this report were partially presented in Pepper (1972).

View full text