Negative affectivity and the influence of suggestion on asthma symptoms

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00541-5Get rights and content

Abstract

Objective

To investigate the effect of suggestion on subjective and objective asthma symptoms as a function of negative affectivity of the patients.

Methods

Asthmatics (n=32) took puffs from three separate placebo inhalers, being described as an inert (practice) substance, a bronchoconstrictor, and a bronchodilator. Negative affectivity, social desirability, probability of medication-intake, intensity of asthma symptoms and total respiratory resistance were measured at onset. The latter three measures were repeated after each trial. Heart rate, end tidal PCO2, and breathing behaviour were measured during each trial.

Results

Asthmatics with high negative affectivity had overall more intense asthma symptoms. They also reported more airway obstruction after suggested bronchoconstriction and less after suggested bronchodilation, whereas persons with low negative affectivity did not show such variation. These effects were unrelated to social desirability. Respiratory symptoms correlated with the odds of medication intake. Neither negative affectivity nor suggestion influenced lung function and only breathing parameters under voluntary control changed as a function of suggestion.

Conclusion

Self-reported symptoms of asthmatics with high negative affectivity are more influenced by suggestion than those of patients with low negative affectivity.

Introduction

Asthma is one of the few medical areas where suggestion has been used as an essential part of a research paradigm. However, the term suggestion has been used in a number of ways. McDougall's definition (1908; see in Ref. [1]) of suggestion is one of the oldest: suggestion is a method of communication, resulting in the acceptance as convictive of the communicated proposition in the absence of logically adequate grounds for its acceptance. A more specific definition is the one proposed by Eysenck [2] who stated that suggestion occurs “when one person attempts to influence another person to believe something, to act in a certain way, or to perceive something along lines that are not congruent with reality. This influence has to be asserted indirectly, i.e., not through bribing or punishment, or attempting to influence him overtly”. From a cognitive–behavioural and social psychological perspective one mechanism responsible for the suggestion effect is referred to as “response expectancy” [3]. It is assumed that people have beliefs or cognitions about their own responses in a certain situation, generating subjective probabilities that nonvolitional subjective experiences and associated behavioural and physiological correlates will occur. These cognitions are influenced by verbal and contextual information.

The mechanism of response expectancy is particularly relevant to asthmatic patients: based upon previous experiences they learn to expect changes in symptoms in specific situations [4]. Case stories have been reported where asthmatics are indirectly confronted with certain eliciting factors (e.g., looking outside and seeing how cold it is outside) or given verbal information (e.g., being told that the doctor is on his/her way) resulting in changes in shortness of breath. To make scientifically sound deductions about the influence of suggestion in asthmatics, several paradigms have been developed. The most widely used method was first described by Luparello et al. [5] and McFadden et al. [6]. Subjects were told they were inhaling a bronchoconstrictor, but actually it was a relatively inert substance (saline). The effect of this intervention on lung function was then evaluated. A literature review [7] revealed that up to 35% of the subjects could be considered “responders” using a criterion of 20% change of pulmonary function (lung volume), which is considered to reflect a clinically significant airway change. Lewis et al. [8], however, showed that the observed suggestion effect was largely due to airway cooling in response to the inhalation of cool nebulized saline as a “neutral” substance. Controlling for this “saline-effect”, the subgroup of asthmatics objectively responding to suggestion, appeared much smaller [9]. A general finding in most studies was that subjects' perception of breathing difficulty was consistent with suggestions [8], [9]. Furthermore, there was no clear-cut correlation between subjective perceptions and objective measures of pulmonary physiology [8], [10].

The present study had several purposes. Firstly, as reported above, most studies' priority concern was the influence of suggestion on airway changes. When the patients' subjective symptoms were examined at all, they were assessed by means of a Visual Analogue Scale (VAS), resulting in a score that integrates several different aspects of asthma symptoms. In a previous study, we demonstrated that asthmatic subjects were able to describe their symptoms in terms of separate qualitative aspects, such as symptoms of airways obstruction, dyspnea, fatigue, hyperventilation symptoms, anxiety and irritation [11]. Secondly, the relationship between responsiveness to suggestion and negative affectivity (NA) was investigated. NA or the tendency to experience negative emotions has been found to correlate with elevated symptom levels and illness behaviour both in the general population and in several subgroups of patients [12]. Previously, we found that asthmatic patients characterized by high NA report more symptoms, regardless of their current clinical status [11]. There is no link between the personality trait neuroticism and physiological responses to suggestion [8], but its association with subjective responses to suggestion has not been explored yet. Because elevated symptom levels in high NA subjects have been explained by symptom perception variables, such as attentional bias to internal state [13], it may be hypothesized that NA modulates the effect of suggestion. Thirdly, we wanted to explore the extent to which suggestion of airway changes would lead to changes in the intention of medication intake. Lastly, we wanted to differentiate a subjective suggestion effect from the concept “social desirability”. Considering response expectancy as an important part of suggestion, one could suspect that a mechanism such as social desirability, defined as a deliberate intention to comply with the expectation of the experimenter/doctor, would explain the symptomatological responses to suggestion.

Section snippets

Subjects

Thirty-two subjects were consecutively recruited from the outpatient clinic of the Department of Pulmonary Diseases at the University Hospital Gasthuisberg, Leuven (Belgium). All subjects had the diagnosis of asthma confirmed according to the guidelines of the American Thoracic Society [14] and were between the age of 18 and 65. On the day of the study, patients were free of respiratory infections and had not taken β2-agonists during the last 4 (short-acting) or 12 h (long-acting).

Measures and apparatus

The Asthma

Demographics and clinical characteristics

The patients (n=32; mean age=40; S.D.=15; 16 women) consulted the clinic for a regular asthma checkup. Before onset of the test their average spirometric values were within normal limits: FVC %pred=107, S.D.=12; FEV1 %pred=91, S.D.=15. The median for NA was 19.5, and the respective means of the high and low NA-group were 26 (S.D.=6) and 15 (S.D.=2). The median for social desirability was 24; the high-group's mean was 26 (S.D.=2), and the low-group's mean was 19 (S.D.=2).

Physiological changes in response to suggestion

ANOVAs on the

Discussion

Asthmatics were given the suggestion that they were inhaling air, a bronchoconstrictor and a bronchodilator, while in all three instances a placebo inhaler was used. Overall, respiratory symptoms of dyspnea and obstruction tended to follow the direction of the suggestions, but only the decrease of symptoms after the bronchodilation suggestion was significant. There was no suggestion effect for symptoms of fatigue, hyperventilation, anxiety or irritation, meaning that the effects were rather

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by grant BIL01/05 (Bilateral Scientific and Technological Cooperation Flanders, Belgium) and FWO G.0270.01.

References (28)

  • SA Isenberg et al.

    The effects of suggestion and emotional arousal on pulmonary function in asthma: a review and a hypothesis regarding vagal mediation

    Psychosom Med

    (1992)
  • RA Lewis et al.

    Asthma induced by suggestion: is it due to airway cooling?

    Am Rev Respir Dis

    (1984)
  • DJ Horton et al.

    Bronchoconstrictive suggestion in asthma: a role for airways hyperreactivity and emotions

    Am Rev Respir Dis

    (1978)
  • C Put et al.

    Asthma symptoms: influence of personality versus clinical status

    Eur Respir J

    (1999)
  • Cited by (57)

    • Poor respiratory health outcomes associated with high illness worry and alexithymia: Eleven-year prospective cohort study among the working-age population

      2022, Journal of Psychosomatic Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      In further epidemiological studies, they could be used as a risk factor of somatic health outcomes. So far, studies examining the associations between respiratory symptom reports and dispositions have focused on patient populations or a selection of healthy volunteers in experimental studies [7,26–28]. Furthermore, the discrepancy between self-reported and physiologically assessed respiratory health has further been linked with mood and anxiety disorders [13,14].

    • Dissociating breathlessness symptoms from mood in asthma

      2021, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Affective dysfunctions such as increased anxiety and depression are some of the most significant co-morbidities in individuals with asthma (Agnihotri & Kant, 2019; Di Marco et al., 2011; Katon et al., 2007; Katon, Richardson, Lozano & McCauley, 2004; Rimington, Davies, Lowe & Pearson, 2001). It is thought that negative mood states (typical in anxiety and depression) may influence symptom perceptions through a variety of channels (Apter et al., 1997; Chen, Hermann, Rodgers, Oliver-Welker & Strunk, 2006; De Peuter & Lemaigre et al., 2007; De Peuter & Put et al., 2007; Lavietes, Sanchez, Tiersky, Cherniack & Natelson, 2000; Put et al., 2004; Rietveld & Everaerd, 2000; Rimington et al., 2001; Van et al., 2004), including assigning wider (non-specific) symptoms to asthma diagnoses (Main, Moss-Morris, Booth, Kaptein & Kolbe, 2003), reduced perceptions of asthma control (Di Marco et al., 2010) and altered expectations regarding asthma symptoms (Brown, Vornik, Khan & Rush, 2016; Marlow, Faull, Finnegan & Pattinson, 2019; Ritz & Steptoe, 2000). Importantly, the interaction between physiological (dys)function and symptom perception depends critically on our ability to accurately sense, perceive and interpret afferent sensory information from our body, a process termed ‘interoception’ (Garfinkel, Seth, Barrett, Suzuki & Critchley, 2015; Khalsa et al., 2017; Seth, 2013).

    • The influence of personality traits on the placebo/nocebo response: A systematic review

      2020, Journal of Psychosomatic Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      All of the studies examined healthy subjects except for three studies. Kelley et al. [37], looked at patients suffering from irritable bowel syndrome (IBS), Put et al. [53] looked at asthma patients and Sullivan et al. [41] had a sample of neuropathic pain patients. The 24 studies included 14 randomised trials and 10 cohort studies.

    • Effects by inhalation of abundant fragrances in indoor air – An overview

      2017, Environment International
      Citation Excerpt :

      Furthermore, airway symptoms were reported by single patients, but without changes in their lung functions. Learned symptoms, learned cues or past experiences (Bogaerts et al., 2010; Van den Bergh et al., 2002), negative affectivity (Put et al., 2004; Van Diest et al., 2005), and distress and life events (Jaén and Dalton, 2014; Skovbjerg et al., 2012; Zachariae et al., 2001) may influence the outcome among the patients that suffer from skin sensitivity or from idiopathic environmental intolerance (Berg et al., 2011; Elberling et al., 2004), but it is unclear how. Acute studies in a mice inhalation model showed that APN and LIM caused sensory irritation at high concentrations (Nielsen et al., 2005; Larsen et al., 2000); extrapolation to irritation in humans is shown in Table 2, which also includes 3-carene.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text