Elsevier

Physiology & Behavior

Volume 62, Issue 1, July 1997, Pages 193-197
Physiology & Behavior

A Comparison of the Ability of 8–9-Year-Old Children and Adults to Detect Taste Stimuli

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(97)00030-9Get rights and content

Abstract

James, C. E., D. G. Laing and N. Oram. A comparison of the ability of 8–9-year-old children and adults to detect taste stimuli. Physiol Behav 62(1) 193–197, 1997.—Conflicting data exist in the literature regarding the maturity of the human sense of taste during childhood and if gender influences gustatory development. To investigate these 2 questions, taste detection thresholds for the 4 common tastants sucrose, sodium chloride, citric acid, and caffeine were established for 61 young adults and 68 children aged 8–9 years old, using a paired-comparison forced-choice procedure. No significant differences were found between the mean thresholds of women and men, or between those of female children and adults. In contrast, male children had significantly higher thresholds for all 4 tastants than adult females, for all tastants except caffeine than adult men, and for sucrose and sodium chloride than female children. It is concluded that the taste sensitivity of 8–9-year-old males, although well developed, has not fully matured, and that taste sensitivity is not affected by gender in young adults.

Section snippets

General

Taste solutions were prepared from deionised water that was obtained from a Millipore Milli-Ro 6 Plus System and had a conductivity of 0.9 uS. The tastants were sucrose, sodium chloride, citric acid, and caffeine. All were purchased from Ajax Chemicals and were reagent grade, except caffeine, which was LABCHEM grade. Solutions of tastants were dispensed in approximately 10-ml aliquots in 50-ml plastic cups that were labeled with 3-digit code numbers. Solutions containing sucrose were

Results

The detection threshold within a replicate was defined for each subject as the geometric mean of the highest concentration at which a subject recorded an incorrect choice and the concentration above which all the responses were correct. A set of 2-factor repeated measures ANOVAS (subject group ([4]) × replicate ([2]) as the factors) showed that the mean thresholds calculated for the 2 replicates obtained for each tastant and subject group were not significantly different, indicating that there

Discussion

The 3 most significant findings of the present study were that, first, the mean detection thresholds for 8–9-year-old boys were significantly higher for 3 and 4 of the 4 common tastants tested than men and women, respectively, and were higher than the thresholds of girls of the same age for sucrose and sodium chloride. The ability of 8–9-year-old boys to detect common tastants, therefore, may not be fully mature. Second, 8–9-year-old females had thresholds that were similar to those of adults,

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the adults and children who participated in the study as subjects, and the University of Western Sydney, Hawkesbury, for the award of a postgraduate scholarship to C. E. James.

References (21)

  • J.A. Anliker et al.

    Children's food preferences and genetic sensitivity to the bitter taste of 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP)

    Am. J. Clin. Nutr.

    (1991)
  • R.J. Hyde et al.

    Age and sex effects on taste of sucrose, NaCl, citric acid and caffeine

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (1981)
  • L.M. Bartoshuk

    The psychophysics of taste

    Am. J. Clin. Nutr.

    (1978)
  • R.M. Cooper et al.

    The effect of age on taste sensitivity

    J. Gerontol.

    (1959)
  • B.J. Cowart

    Development of taste perception in humans: Sensitivity and preference throughout the life span

    Psychol. Bull.

    (1981)
  • E. Di Mattei

    La sensibilita nei fanciulli in rapporto al sesso ed all'eta

    Arch. Antropol. Crim. Psichiatr. Med. Legal.

    (1901)
  • E.V. Glanville et al.

    Age, sex and taste sensitivity

    J. Gerontol.

    (1964)
  • P.B. Grzegorczyk et al.

    Age-related differences in salt acuity

    J. Gerontol.

    (1979)
  • K.M. Hambridge et al.

    Low levels of zinc in hair, anorexia, poor growth and hypogeusia in children

    Pediatr. Res.

    (1972)
  • R.I. Henkin et al.

    Taste localization on the tongue, palate and pharynx of normal man

    J. Appl. Physiol.

    (1967)
There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (92)

  • Correlates of bitter, sweet, salty and umami taste sensitivity in European children: Role of sex, age and weight status - The IDEFICS study

    2022, Appetite
    Citation Excerpt :

    This might be due to the concentration range of caffeine that we used in our study. James et al. for example used much higher concentrations of caffeine when testing children than we used (James et al., 1997). Our highest concentration was 0.00139 mol/l vs. 0.01277 mol/l in study of James et al.

  • Sensory testing in new product development: Working with children

    2022, Rapid Sensory Profiling Techniques: Applications in New Product Development and Consumer Research, Second Edition
  • Taste and Smell

    2020, Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Childhood Development
  • Sweetness but not sourness enhancement increases acceptance of cucumber and green capsicum purees in children

    2018, Appetite
    Citation Excerpt :

    Aspartame was the most effective bitterness suppressor and increased liking for all vegetables in adults. Taste perception and discriminatory abilities for taste often differ within children and between children and adults (De Graaf & Zandstra, 1999; James, Laing, & Oram, 1997), which might contribute to differences in acceptance. Children can prefer higher intensities of sweetness than adults in a variety of foods (Hoffman, Salgado, Dresler, Faller, & Bartlett, 2016).

View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text