Performing under pressure: stress and cognitive function

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00088-XGet rights and content

Abstract

The way in which cognitive functioning is affected by stressors is an important area of research for applied ethologists because stress caused by captive conditions may disrupt cognitive processes and lead to welfare and husbandry problems. Such problems may be minimised through an understanding of the links between stress and cognition. The effects of stress on cognitive function have been studied in disciplines ranging from human perceptual psychology to animal neuroscience. The aim of this paper is to provide an introduction to this research, focusing on the effects of stressors on attention, memory formation and memory recall. Findings from such a diverse literature with little apparent inter-disciplinary communication are inevitably complex and often contradictory. Nevertheless, some generalities do emerge. The idea that an inverted U-shaped relationship exists between an individual's state of stress or arousal and its ability to perform a cognitive task effectively, the so-called Yerkes–Dodson law, is commonly encountered. The law has limited explanatory value because it is unlikely that different stressors act on cognitive function via the same intervening, non-specific state. Furthermore, the law only provides a very general description of the relationship between stress and cognitive function. Empirical research on attention and memory processes reveals more specific findings. Stressors appear to cause shifts, lapses and narrowing of attention, and can also influence decision speed. These processes may be viewed as serving an adaptive role helping the animal to search for and scrutinise a source of danger. There is conflicting evidence as to whether hormones involved in the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal stress response play a part in these processes. These hormones and those involved in the sympathetic-adrenomedullary stress response do appear to play an important role in memory formation. Low or moderate concentrations of circulating glucocorticoids and catecholamines can enhance memory formation, while excessively high or prolonged elevations of these hormones can lead to memory disruption. The effects of stressors on memory recall are less clear. There is evidence for disruptive effects, and for facilitatory effects indicating state-dependent memory recall; events experienced under conditions of high arousal may be best recalled under similar conditions. Applied ethologists have the opportunity to extend work in this area, which often involves studies of single stressors/stress hormones acting in isolation and limited measures of cognitive function, by focusing on real-life husbandry stressors encountered by captive animals. This will yield fundamental information which also has direct relevance to animal welfare and management issues.

Introduction

The aim of this paper is to provide an introduction to research that investigates how stress affects cognitive function. This is a very broad question which raises the issues of how to define stress and cognition and whether stress-related states and cognitive processes can be treated as distinct, separate entities. For example, there is disagreement as to whether or not an organism's emotional responses to a stressor can occur independently of cognitive processes. Although these are important issues, they are beyond the scope of this paper and are discussed in detail elsewhere (e.g., Zajonc, 1980; Lazarus, 1982; Broom and Johnson, 1993; Nicol, 1995; Toates, 1995, Toates, 1997). Instead, I have chosen to take a pragmatic, empirical approach to this research area. In searching the literature, I have selected studies as being relevant to this topic if they investigate how apparently threatening and challenging stimuli (including manipulation of “stress hormones”) affect attention, learning and memory. Within these general boundaries, I have not defined the terms “stress” and “cognition” any more precisely but have used them simply as convenient labels which capture the essence of the area that I want to explore (cf. Rushen, 1986a).

Understanding how stressors modulate cognitive processes is important in its own right (e.g., Broadbent, 1971; Hockey et al., 1986), but I suggest that it may also be of value to applied ethology for the following reasons. If the stress of the housing and husbandry of farmed, laboratory and zoo animals impairs cognitive abilities such as learning and memory, this may lead to welfare and management problems. For example, the occurrence of aggression between previously familiar animals (e.g., Luescher et al., 1990; Mount and Seabrook, 1993) may be the result of stressful conditions, such as mixing or temporary removal from a group, disrupting individual recognition processes. Similarly, efficient husbandry and management will be hindered if animals fail to learn and remember the relative harmlessness of routine procedures such as weighing or veterinary inspection, and how to use equipment such as electronic feeders, or robotic milkers.

Choice testing is a widely used procedure in animal welfare research, and animal choices are usually interpreted from a motivational perspective as demonstrating the importance, value or aversiveness of particular resources or stimuli to the animal (e.g., Dawkins, 1990). However, it is also important to consider what might be termed the cognitive components of choice testing; for example, the learning and memory of an association between a location in a choice test apparatus and a resource or stimulus. If conditions of testing are stressful to the animal and impair learning and memory, this may disrupt choices and hence give spurious results with potentially misleading welfare implications. In a number of papers on pigs, sheep, cattle and deer, the suggestion has been made that agitated animals are indeed poorer at making clear-cut choices in preference or avoidance tests (Rushen, 1986b; van Rooijen and Metz, 1987; Pollard et al., 1994; Grandin et al., 1994; see Mendl et al., 1997).

Stress effects on cognitive function may also prevent us from gaining a true picture of the cognitive abilities of domestic animals; an important goal in applied ethology research (Dawkins, 1993; Nicol, 1996). For example, in experiments using detour behaviour tasks to investigate the ability of chicks to maintain memories of an object in the absence of sensory cues from that object (object permanence), Regolin et al. (1995)reported that this ability was masked when the chicks were tested under the stressful conditions of social isolation.

Understanding the relationship between stress and cognition may also allow the development of more effective methods for training animals, and for using behaviour therapy. For example, Walker et al. (1997)discuss how learning in the dog may be modulated by drugs which appear to alter anxiety or stress states.

Finally, from a welfare perspective, it is important to know whether stress experienced during early life, and long-term stress imposed by some husbandry conditions can lead to changes in the cognitive capacities of animals (e.g., learned helplessness, Overmier et al., 1980).

Section snippets

The literature on stress and cognitive function

Studies of stress effects on cognitive function do not form a single, coherent, evolving discipline. Research which could be categorised under this heading appears in a wide variety of disciplines ranging from human perceptual psychology to animal neuroscience. The result is a literature examining the effects of a diverse selection of threatening and challenging stimuli (e.g., loud noise, social isolation, direct manipulations of “stress hormones”) on a wide range of cognitive tasks (e.g.,

The Yerkes–Dodson law: a unifying model?

One of the few themes that seems to span virtually all the research areas is the intuitively reasonable idea that cognitive performance is best when an individual is in some optimal stress or arousal state, above or below which performance falls away (Fig. 1). This idea has become known as the Yerkes–Dodson law after discrimination learning experiments on mice by Yerkes and Dodson (1908), although historically it also has roots in the work of Hebb (1955)on inverted U relationship between

Experimental investigations of the effects of stress on cognitive function

The effects of short-term stressors on cognitive function have been investigated using the experimental designs illustrated in Fig. 2. A stressor is imposed (A) just before or during an event or task, (B) after an event or task, or (C) before or during recall of the event or task, or any combination of these. The effect of the stressor is evaluated by measuring aspects of performance during the initial task or during the recall test. Different combinations of stressor presentation time and

Discussion and implications for applied ethology

The research summarised in this paper suggests that threatening or challenging stimuli (including manipulations of stress hormones) have marked and varied effects on attention, memory formation and, to a lesser extent, memory recall, all of which play fundamentally important roles in cognitive processes. Some of these effects can be viewed as having adaptive value in helping the organism to search, scrutinise and remember threatening stimuli or situations. The apparent existence of threat

Acknowledgements

I thank Alistair Lawrence for pointing me in the direction of this research area and, together with Marie Haskell, Georgia Mason and Liz Paul, for discussions about the subject. I am also grateful to Christine Nicol and John Webster for commenting on an earlier version of the manuscript.

References (132)

  • M Gallagher et al.

    Effects of opiate antagonists on spatial memory in young and aged rats

    Behav. Neural. Biol.

    (1985)
  • T Grandin et al.

    The reluctance of cattle to change a learned choice may confound preference tests

    Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.

    (1994)
  • T Guilford et al.

    Search images not proven: a reappraisal of recent evidence

    Anim. Behav.

    (1987)
  • P Jensen et al.

    Stress as a state of motivational systems

    Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.

    (1997)
  • G.L Kovacs et al.

    Retention of passive avoidance behavior in rats following α- and gamma-endorphin administration: effects of post-learning treatments

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (1981)
  • V Luine et al.

    Repeated stress causes reversible impairments of spatial memory performance

    Brain Res.

    (1994)
  • J.W Mason

    A reevaluation of the concept of “non-specificity” in stress theory

    J. Psychiatr. Res.

    (1971)
  • B.S McEwen et al.

    Stress and cognitive function

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (1995)
  • R McKenzie et al.

    Brief retention deficits associated with cyclically recurring left-hemisphere events

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1996)
  • M Mendl et al.

    Pigs in space: spatial memory and its susceptibility to interference

    Anim. Behav.

    (1997)
  • C Messier et al.

    Memory improvement by glucose, fructose, and two glucose analogs a possible effect on peripheral glucose transport

    Behav. Neural. Biol.

    (1987)
  • N.B Metcalfe et al.

    Predation risk impairs diet selection in juvenile salmon

    Anim. Behav.

    (1987)
  • N.E Mount et al.

    A study of aggression when group housed sows are mixed

    Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.

    (1993)
  • L Munksgaard et al.

    Discrimination of people by diary cows based on handling

    J. Dairy Sci.

    (1997)
  • Baddeley, A., 1996. Your Memory. A User's Guide. Prion,...
  • M.A Baker et al.

    The effects of noise and speech on cognitive task-performance

    J. Gen. Psychol.

    (1993)
  • G Baumler

    On the validity of the Yerkes–Dodson law

    Stud. Psychol.

    (1994)
  • W.W Beatty et al.

    Rat spatial memory: resistance to retroactive interference at long retention intervals

    Anim. Learn. Behav.

    (1980)
  • D.E Berlyne et al.

    Paired-associate learning and the timing of arousal

    J. Exp. Psychol.

    (1966)
  • S.R Bodnoff et al.

    Enduring effects of chronic corticosterone treatment on spatial learning, synaptic plasticity and hippocampal neuropathology in young and mid-aged rats

    J. Neurosci.

    (1995)
  • G.H Bower

    Mood and memory

    Am. Psychol.

    (1981)
  • Broadbent, D.E., 1958. Perception and Communication. Pergamon,...
  • Broadbent, D.E., 1971. Decision and Stress. Academic Press,...
  • Broom, D.M., Johnson, K.G., 1993. Stress and Animal Welfare. Chapman & Hall,...
  • S Cabib et al.

    Impairments produced by amphetamine and stress on memory storage are reduced following a chronic stressful experience

    Psychopharmacology

    (1997)
  • L Cahill et al.

    Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events

    Nature

    (1994)
  • Cherkin, A., Flood, J.F., 1988. Behavioral pharmacology of memory. In: Woody, C.D., Alkon, D.L., McGaugh, J.L. (Eds.),...
  • S Christianson

    Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: a critical review

    Psychol. Bull.

    (1992)
  • S Christianson et al.

    Eye fixations and memory for emotional events

    J. Exp. Psychol.

    (1991)
  • C.D Conrad et al.

    Chronic stress impairs rat spatial memory on the Y maze, and this effect is blocked by tianeptine pretreatment

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (1996)
  • M.A Conway et al.

    The formation of flashbulb memories

    Mem. Cognition

    (1994)
  • M.S Dawkins

    From an animal's point of view: motivation, fitness and animal welfare

    Behav. Brain. Sci.

    (1990)
  • Dawkins, M., 1993. Through Our Eyes Only? Freeman,...
  • D de Wied et al.

    Neuropeptides derived from pro-opiocortin: behavioural, physiological and neurochemical effects

    Physiol. Rev.

    (1982)
  • D.M Diamond et al.

    Stress impairs LTP and hippocampal-dependent memory formation

    Annal. N.Y. Acad. Sci.

    (1994)
  • J.A Easterbrook

    The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior

    Psychol. Rev.

    (1959)
  • H Eichenbaum et al.

    Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (1994)
  • Eysenck, M.W., 1995. Attention. In: French, C.C., Colman, A.M. (Eds.), Cognitive Psychology. Longman, London. pp....
  • Eysenck, M.W., Keane, M.T., 1995. Cognitive Psychology, 3rd edn. Psychology Press,...
  • H.L Fehm et al.

    Effects of corticotropin releasing hormone on human brain function: an analysis based on auditory evoked potentials

    Horm. Metab. Res. Suppl. Ser.

    (1987)
  • Cited by (310)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text