Natural and synthetic sex hormones: Effects on higher-order cognitive function and prepulse inhibition

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.02.001Get rights and content

Abstract

Little is known about the effects of the commonly-used oral contraceptive pill (OC) on cognition. This study compared matched healthy women across the menstrual cycle (early-follicular ‘Low E/P’, mid-luteal ‘High E/P’), women using the combined OC, and men. The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) was used to assess immediate and delayed memory, visuospatial ability, language and attention. Assessment of prepulse inhibition (PPI) included 21 pulse-alone trials (115 dB) and 42 prepulse-pulse trials (74, 78, 86 dB). The OC and High E/P groups outperformed men on the total RBANS score, an overall measure of cognition. For the immediate memory/learning and attention domains, the OC group outperformed men; for the delayed memory domain, the OC and High E/P groups outperformed men. In conclusion, high levels of natural or synthetic sex hormones had a positive effect on higher-order cognition but had little effect on baseline PPI.

Highlights

► We measured cognition and PPI in men and in women during different hormonal stages. ► Women using the contraceptive pill or women during the high hormone stage outperformed men on cognition. ► Different hormonal states in women had little effect on baseline PPI.

Introduction

Estrogens and progestins are groups of sex steroid hormones predominantly found in women, but are also found in smaller concentrations in men. During the menstrual cycle, there are two peaks of estrogen secretion, one just before ovulation and a smaller peak during the mid-luteal phase. Progesterone levels remain low during the follicular phase and there is one peak in its secretion during the mid-luteal phase (Ganong, 1979, Ross and Vande Wiele, 1974). Synthetic estrogens and progestins are used in preparations such as the commonly-used combined oral contraceptive pill (OC). Among young Australian women the OC is the most popular contraceptive method, with 76% of women having ever used it and 45% of 20–24 year olds currently using it (Yusuf and Siedlecky, 2007).

Sex hormones have a number of non-reproductive effects; of interest here are their effects on cognitive function. Women have been found to perform better on verbal and memory tests and poorer on spatial ability tests during the high estrogen/progesterone phase of the menstrual cycle (Hampson, 1990, Maki et al., 2002, Rosenberg and Park, 2002, Sherwin, 2012). A number of studies have examined the effects of hormone replacement therapy on cognition in post-menopausal women (LeBlanc et al., 2001, Maki and Sundermann, 2009, Zec and Trivedi, 2002). Overall, it is suggested that estrogen has a small positive effect on cognition, particularly verbal memory (Joffe et al., 2006, Sherwin, 2012, Zec and Trivedi, 2002), however the positive effect may depend on the cognitive domain (Bayer and Hausmann, 2009), type of estrogen (Maki and Sundermann, 2009) and the time period when the estrogen was administered (Craig and Murphy, 2010, Maki, 2006, Sherwin, 2012). Less is known about the effect of estrogen administration in healthy, pre-menopausal women or of the effect of the OC on cognition. OC users showed enhanced verbal memory during the active pill phase compared to the inactive pill phase, while naturally cycling women showed no change across the menstrual cycle (Mordecai et al., 2008). Other studies showed that OC use improved spatial attention (Cicinelli et al., 2011) but had no effect on recognition memory (Wharton et al., 2008). On the other hand, mental rotation task performance is dependent on the type of progestin used in the OC (Griksiene and Ruksenas, 2011), whereby highly androgenic OCs improved performance (Wharton et al., 2008). The present study measured various domains of cognitive function, including memory, attention, language and visuospatial ability, in OC users and non-users.

Prepulse inhibition (PPI) is a measure of sensorimotor gating or information processing. It is proposed to reflect a normal protective mechanism in the brain that filters irrelevant sensory, motor or cognitive information, thus allowing for coherent thought (Braff and Geyer, 1990). Deficits in gating incoming information may result in information overload and misinterpretation of stimuli (Braff et al., 2001). There is an extensive literature describing how PPI is disrupted in schizophrenia (Braff et al., 2001), and other illnesses including bipolar disorder (Gogos et al., 2009), suggesting that PPI deficits are the result of abnormalities within a specific brain circuit rather than a specific psychopathology (Swerdlow et al., 2001).

A limited number of studies have recently examined PPI in sex hormone affected states, such as during the menstrual cycle, pregnancy and premenstrual dysphoric disorder (Kask et al., 2008a, Kask et al., 2008b, Kumari et al., 2010). For example, women have the greatest PPI during the early follicular (low estrogen/progesterone) phase of the menstrual cycle (Jovanovic et al., 2004, Kumari et al., 2010, Swerdlow et al., 1997). Kask et al. (2008a) found that women had lower PPI during late pregnancy, a time of high circulating levels of estrogen and progesterone, compared to the postpartum period when these hormone levels decline (Kask et al., 2008a). Further, lower levels of PPI were found in cycling women compared with post-menopausal women (Bannbers et al., 2010), where menopause is a time of dramatic decline of sex hormone levels. These studies suggest that sex steroid hormones, such as estrogen, play a role in modulating PPI, whereby high levels of estrogen may reduce PPI. We previously showed that estrogen treatment reversed drug-induced disruptions of PPI in healthy women and in female rats (Gogos et al., 2006, Gogos et al., 2010b, Gogos and Van den Buuse, 2004), and suggested that the dopaminergic system is involved in mediating the effects of estrogen on PPI (Chavez et al., 2010, Gogos et al., 2010b). Little is known about the effects of the OC on PPI. One study found that women with adverse mood-related side-effects from the OC had lower levels of PPI, compared to women with no adverse effects of the OC (Borgstrom et al., 2008). This study did not, however, statistically compare PPI in OC users with naturally cycling women or with men; this will be examined in the present study.

Although millions of women worldwide either currently or have previously used OCs (Burkman et al., 2011), surprisingly little is known about their effects on the brain. This study aimed to examine the effects of natural and synthetic sex hormones on cognitive function, particularly higher-order cognition and PPI. Healthy, pre-menopausal women who were either currently using the OC or were naturally cycling (follicular or mid-luteal phase), and healthy men, were tested.

Section snippets

Participants

The present study tested 60 healthy participants (45F:15M) aged between 20 and 43 years (see Table 1 for demographic information). All participants were recruited from the local community via advertisements and word-of-mouth. The procedures described in this study were approved by the Mental Health Research and Ethics Committee (Melbourne Health, VIC, Australia) and participants gave their written, informed consent prior to testing.

Participants were excluded if they reported a history of brain

Demographic variables

One-way ANOVA showed that the four groups were not significantly different in terms of age, predicted full-scale IQ, education and BPRS (Table 1). Pearson's chi-square analysis showed that the four groups were not significantly different in terms of the number of smokers or handedness. In female participants with a natural cycle, there was no significant difference in average cycle length between the Low E/P and High E/P groups (Table 1).

Repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status

The RBANS total score differed significantly among the

Discussion

This study examined higher cognitive function and PPI in healthy, pre-menopausal women who were either currently using the OC or were naturally cycling. Further, gender differences were examined by comparing these groups to healthy age-, IQ- and education-matched men. There were marked gender differences in cognition, where women in the OC or High E/P groups performed better than men. There was a tendency for women using the OC to perform better on cognitive tasks than women in the Low E/P

Acknowledgements

The author gratefully acknowledges the financial support of the J. & P. Clemenger Trust and the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (A. Gogos, Peter Doherty Fellowship, ID 435690). This research was also supported by Operational Infrastructure Support (OIS) from the Victorian State Government. I would like to acknowledge the assistance of Susan Rossell, Nicole Joshua with recruiting participants, and Sonia Davison and Maarten van den Buuse with critically proof reading the

References (57)

  • K. Kask et al.

    Lower levels of prepulse inhibition of startle response in pregnant women compared to postpartum women

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2008)
  • V. Kumari et al.

    Sex differences in prepulse inhibition deficits in chronic schizophrenia

    Schizophrenia Research

    (2004)
  • P.M. Maki

    Hormone therapy and cognitive function: is there a critical period for benefit?

    Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • P.M. Maki et al.

    Implicit memory varies across the menstrual cycle: estrogen effects in young women

    Neuropsychologia

    (2002)
  • K.L. Mordecai et al.

    Effects of menstrual cycle phase and oral contraceptive use on verbal memory

    Hormones and Behavior

    (2008)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • A.J. Rapkin et al.

    Oral contraceptives and neuroactive steroids

    Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior

    (2006)
  • R. Rivera et al.

    The mechanism of action of hormonal contraceptives and intrauterine contraceptive devices

    American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology

    (1999)
  • L. Rosenberg et al.

    Verbal and spatial functions across the menstrual cycle in healthy young women

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2002)
  • P. Roussos et al.

    Tolcapone effects on gating, working memory, and mood interact with the synonymous catechol-O-methyltransferase rs4818c/g polymorphism

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • N.R. Swerdlow et al.

    Men are more inhibited than women by weak prepulses

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1993)
  • N.R. Swerdlow et al.

    Changes in sensorimotor inhibition across the menstrual cycle: implications for neuropsychiatric disorders

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • W.W. Beatty et al.

    RBANS performance: influences of sex and education

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (2003)
  • D.L. Braff et al.

    Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia. Human and animal model studies

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1990)
  • D.L. Braff et al.

    Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies

    Psychopharmacology

    (2001)
  • M.C. Craig et al.

    Estrogen therapy and Alzheimer's dementia

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

    (2010)
  • P.A. Csomor et al.

    Haloperidol differentially modulates prepulse inhibition and p50 suppression in healthy humans stratified for low and high gating levels

    Neuropsychopharmacology

    (2008)
  • V. Della Casa et al.

    The effects of smoking on acoustic prepulse inhibition in healthy men and women

    Psychopharmacology

    (1998)
  • Cited by (58)

    • Behavioral characterization of co-exposure to cannabinoids and hormonal contraceptives in female rats

      2024, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
    • Sex/gender differences in cognitive abilities

      2023, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    • Oral contraceptives and cognition: A systematic review

      2023, Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text