Elsevier

Child Abuse & Neglect

Volume 44, June 2015, Pages 170-183
Child Abuse & Neglect

Are parents reliable in reporting child victimization? Comparison of parental and adolescent reports in a matched Chinese household sample

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2014.11.001Get rights and content

Abstract

There is ongoing debate about the reliability of parental reports on child victimization. Some studies have shown that they are useful, whereas some others have provided contrary evidence suggesting that parents are not accurate in reporting child victimization, especially when they are the one who inflicted the violence. This study aimed to (a) examine the reliability of parental reports of adolescents’ experiences of victimization, including that inflicted by parents as well as others, by comparing them with self-reports using a parent–child matched sample from China; and (b) explore the possible reasons underlying any disagreement between the parental and adolescent reports. A total of 2,624 parent–adolescent pairs were recruited during 2009 and 2010 in 6 cities in China. Parents were asked to report the victimization experiences of their child using of the Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire, and these reports were matched with the adolescents’ self-reports of victimization. Low levels of parent–adolescent agreement in reporting were found (Cohen's kappa = .04–.29). Except for sexual violence, parents were significantly less likely to report all types of victimization. Overall, lower levels of agreement were found in the reporting of (a) less severe types of victimization, (b) victimization outside the family, and (c) victimization involving parents as perpetrators. Intimate partner violence between parents was significantly associated with discrepancies between reports. The findings suggest that parents might not be reliable as a single source of information on certain types of adolescent victimization.

Introduction

Child victimization is a broad concept that includes being a victim of conventional crime, child abuse and neglect within the family, peer and sibling victimization, sexual victimization by known or unknown individuals, and exposure to victimization within and outside the family (Finkelhor, Hamby, Ormrod, & Turner, 2005). Whether parents are reliable to serve as the single source of information of child victimization has long been debated. Some researchers have suggested that parental reports of child victimization can be a reliable source of information. For example, Kerker, Horwitz, Leventhal, Plichta, and Leaf (2000) studied 1,148 mothers of children 4–8 years of age in a clinical setting and showed that reports of the mothers detected more child victimization cases than those of the pediatricians. Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore, and Runyan (1998), in their validation study of the Parent-Child Conflict Tactics Scale (CTSPC), demonstrated that parental reports compared well to those made by children under the age of 18 years in identifying child abuse and neglect.

Other researchers have been doubtful about the accuracy of parental reports. They assert that existing evidence for the usefulness of parental reports is not strong enough to support their use as a single source of information and that parental reports may be prone to response bias. Child victimization, in particular child abuse and neglect, may involve parents; and it may be possible that the parent informant may also be the one who inflicts violence against the child. In such situations, it is not unlikely for these parents to underreport or refuse to disclose the victimization incidents as a result of shame, denial, or fear of future legal consequences (Appel & Holden, 1998).

It is noteworthy that, in the midst of the ongoing discussion on the accuracy of parental reports, a recurring conclusion has been put forward that there has not yet been a generally accepted, scientifically validated procedure for absolutely reliable identification of child victimization cases (Goodman et al., 1998, Guyer, 1995). Nonetheless, this does not mean that one could not assess the relative accuracy of reports. Instead, a number of researchers have tried to do so by conducting studies to compare parental reports and child reports using matched samples. Overall, a low-to-moderate degree of agreement has been identified (e.g., Jouriles and Norwood, 1995, Kolko et al., 1996). A recent study with a sample of 1,093 parent–child matched pairs has replicated this finding (Chan, 2012). The level of agreement among reports of severe physical victimization was found to be even lower, suggesting that parents may be inaccurate as the single source of information in detecting or reporting the victimization of their children.

The discrepancy between parental and child reports of child victimization may be even greater when the parent informant is the one who is inflicting the violence. Parental self-reports are potentially biased in that the informant may try to present him/herself to others in a socially desirable manner (Miller-Perrin & Perrin, 1999). Parents are likely to “fake being good” in the sense of minimizing or denying negative behaviors (Milner & Crouch, 1997). In the case of child victimization, they are likely to conceal violent acts against their child. Indeed, several studies have addressed the challenges of identifying parent perpetrators. It may be even more difficult to do so when the child victims are too young to provide consistent and reliable reports (Ghetti, Goodman, Eisen, Qin, & Davis, 2002). Despite the general acknowledgment of the impact of social desirability on parent-reported child violence, the degree of concealment or underreporting due to this factor has not yet been conclusively established. One study involving records from child protective services estimates that 47% of parents with a history of child abuse perpetration partially deny or conceal this behavior when under investigation (Lanyon, Dannenbaum, & Brown, 1991), reflecting a high likelihood of underreporting among abusive parents.

In addition to social desirability, certain personality characteristics may also contribute to the potential bias in parental reports. For example, Graham, Weiner, Cobb, and Henderson (2001) found that abusive parents are likely to perceive their child as more responsible for harsh punishments, and justify their harsh parenting and dismiss their own abusive behaviors. Furthermore, shame and fear of future legal consequences have also been found to play a role in underreporting among parents (Appel & Holden, 1998). Parents with a history of child maltreatment are more willing to acknowledge their nonviolent disciplinary acts, psychological violence, and neglect than to report their use of physical violence (Bennett, Sullivan, & Lewis, 2006). This discrepancy in the willingness to report across maltreatment types may result from fear of the more severe legal consequences of physical violence than of the other three kinds of child victimization.

But what if the victimization does not involve parents? For example, when the victimization occurs outside the family and the perpetrator is a stranger, the effect of social desirability, external attribution, and fear of legal consequences on underreporting should be minimal. If these possible contributing factors to parents’ inaccuracy are suppressed, parental reports should be at least better matched to those from the child or the third parties. Given the scarcity of existing studies which match parental and child reports of victimization that occurs outside the family, one could not make concrete conclusion on whether parental reports could be as accurate as those reported by children. However, the work of Lee, Lansford, Pettit, Bates, and Dodge (2012), which compares fathers’ and mothers’ reports of victimization in the context of spouses’ abusive parenting, might provide some insights to this issue. Lee and colleagues show that parents are likely to underreport their spouse's abusive behaviors toward their child. They suggest that there are some factors other than social desirability contributing to the low accuracy of parental reports.

Because of the scarcity of studies comparing parental reports and child reports of the latter's experience of violence in contexts other than maltreatment by parents, it remains uncertain whether or not parents are reliable to serve as the single source of information about child victimization. One may argue for the usefulness of parental reports of victimization in practice; however, it could not be omitted that the importance of parental reports undoubtedly increases when the child victims are too young or are not mentally capable of reporting their victimization experiences. Assessing the accuracy of parental reports in comparison to child reports is therefore of great significance.

As discussed earlier, parents may not be very reliable as the single informants of child victimization for several reasons. For example, they may fail to detect the victimization incidents that occur outside the family; they may have memory limitations or recall biases; and they may be reluctant to report the incidents when they are the one who inflict the violence against their children (Briere, 1992, McGee et al., 1995). Parents’ failure to detect victimization against their children is not a rare case. Research shows that parents are not sufficiently aware of school bullying in part because their children do not disclose their experiences with bullying to their parents (Mishna, 2004). In fact, less than two-thirds of the victims would tell their parents about their victimization outside the family (Fekkes et al., 2005, Unnever and Cornell, 2004). In order to assess the reliability of parental reports, it is therefore important to employ a broad definition of child victimization that not only includes child maltreatment by parents but also violence inflicted by others such as siblings, schoolmates, other relatives, and even strangers.

Using a large, matched sample of parents and their children, this study set out to examine the agreement and disagreement between reports of child victimization as well as other forms of victimization. In particular, it intended to (a) investigate the reliability of parental reports compared to those of children; and (b) explore the possible reasons for the disagreement between the two types. In this study, victimization was defined using the Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire (Finkelhor et al., 2005), which covered four extra aspects of violence against children in addition to maltreatment by parents. The extra aspects were conventional crime, peer and sibling victimization, sexual victimization, and witnessing or indirect victimization. It was assumed that social desirability and fear of future legal consequences would have a significant impact on parents’ reporting of child maltreatment as inflicted by parents themselves, but not on violence inflicted by others. Based on this assumption, it was hypothesized that there would be a significant disagreement between reports in child victimization inflicted by the parental informants. When taking into considerations of the possibility that the parents are not aware of the victimization because their children are reluctant to disclose (Mishna, 2004), it was also hypothesized that there would be a significant disagreement between reports in situations where the perpetrator was someone other than the child's parents. With reference to the previous findings that demographic factors, interparental conflicts, and child health status may be associated with discrepancies in reporting of child maltreatment (Chan, 2012, Lee et al., 2012), this study also examined the effects of demographic background and intimate partner violence (IPV) between parents, as well as the child's depression and problematic behaviors, on discrepancies in the reporting of child victimization. According to the previous findings that the discrepancies might be greater among children with behavioral and health problems (Lee et al., 2012), it was hypothesized that child depression and addictive behaviors would be associated to a greater disagreement between reports. In addition, it was also hypothesized that parental IPV, which has been demonstrated to be associated with less time parents spent on interacting with children (Lee et al., 2012), would be associated with a greater discrepancy between parental and child reports.

Section snippets

Sample and Procedure

This study used data from a cross-sectional survey conducted in China during 2009 and 2010. As pointed out in previous research (Chan, Yan, Brownridge, & Ip, 2013), recruiting a sample that truly represents the Chinese population, given its enormous size and diverse ethnicities, is an extremely challenging task. Therefore, this study adopted a more practical method of sampling by purposively selecting six cities from different geographical regions in China, making the sample more representative

Prevalence of Adolescent Victimization

Table 2 shows the prevalence rates of the various aspects of victimization among the adolescents. More than half (52.5%) reported that they had experienced at least one incident of violence in their lives, and almost 40% had been victimized during the year before survey. The most prevalent form of victimization was conventional crime (34.6%), followed by witnessing or indirect victimization (25.3%), peer and sibling victimization (19.0%), and maltreatment by adults (17.5%). The least reported

Discussion

Using a large and diverse parent–adolescent matched sample in China, this study is among the first to compare the prevalence rates of adolescent victimization as reported by parents and their children in the Chinese population. Over half of the adolescents reported having been victimized at least once in their lives, and almost 40% had such an experience during the year preceding the survey. It is noteworthy that the self-reported prevalence of victimization was generally lower than that found

Conclusion

Using a large and diverse sample of adolescents and their parents in China, this study has provided evidence for a large discrepancy between parents and adolescents in reporting victimization of the latter, suggesting that the Chinese parents might not know their children as well as they think they do. The high levels of disagreement also reflect the importance of relying on more than one source of information in identifying victims of violence. This is especially crucial to the detection or

References (35)

  • A.E. Appel et al.

    The co-occurrence of spouse and physical child abuse: A review and appraisal

    Journal of Family Psychology

    (1998)
  • D.S. Bennett et al.

    Relations of parental report and observation of parenting to maltreatmemt history

    Child Maltreatment

    (2006)
  • J. Briere

    Methodological issues in the study of sexual abuse effects

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1992)
  • K.L. Chan

    Comparison of parent and child reports on child maltreatment in a representative household sample in Hong Kong

    Journal of Family Violence

    (2012)
  • K.L. Chan et al.

    Validation of the Chiense Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire

    Hong Kong Journal of Paediatrics

    (2011)
  • J.P.J. Dussich et al.

    Physical child harm and bullying-related behaviors

    International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

    (2007)
  • M. Fekkes et al.

    Bullying: Who does what, when and where? Involvement of children, teachers, and parents in bullying behavior

    Health Education Research: Theory and Practice

    (2005)
  • Cited by (23)

    • Discrepancies between children's and caregivers' child maltreatment reporting and their associations with child wellbeing

      2022, Child Abuse and Neglect
      Citation Excerpt :

      First, the study data only allowed for examining reporting discrepancies on the frequencies of physical and psychological maltreatment between children and caregivers. Previous studies based on dichotomized maltreatment measurement have shown that reporting discrepancies are common across information sources such as children, caregivers, and CPS case files, and the discrepancies further vary by the reporting of physical, sexual, emotional maltreatment, and neglect (Chan, 2012, 2015; Cooley & Jackson, 2022; Fung & Lau, 2010; Havlicek & Courtney, 2016; Kobulsky et al., 2017; Naicker et al., 2017; Sierau et al., 2017). Future research can extend the current investigation in reporting discrepancies and their consequences by considering more inclusive reporting sources.

    • Childhood abuse and internalizing symptoms: Exploring mediating & moderating role of attachment, competency, and self-regulation

      2020, Children and Youth Services Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      This aligns with previous work which has indicated that parents tend to report significantly lower levels of abuse compared to adolescents (Chan, 2015; Compier-de Block et al., 2017). Even when slight agreement has been found between caregiver and adolescent reports of abuse, adolescents still tend to report greater abuse frequency and severity than caregivers (Chan, 2015; Kobulsky et al., 2017). These results indicate that scholars may be better served to examine both adolescent and caregiver perceptions of behaviors and symptoms, rather than relying solely on caregiver reports.

    • Child victimization in China: Prevalence and links to family contextual characteristics using a representative sample

      2020, Children and Youth Services Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      The lower estimates of child victimization were possibly due, in part, to the use of parent report measures of child victimization. Previous studies have found that Chinese parents were significantly less likely to report child victimization when compared to children’s self-report, regardless of whether the child victimization occurred within the family or outside of the home (Chan, 2015). Parents may have feared interviewer judgment or repercussions related to disclosure.

    • What prevents Chinese parents from reporting possible cases of child sexual abuse to authority? A holistic-interactionistic approach

      2017, Child Abuse and Neglect
      Citation Excerpt :

      His studies revealed low to moderate levels of parent-child agreement when it came to reporting. Chan (2015) suggested that social desirability and violence approval were the common predictors of parent-child disagreement in reports in Chinese society. However, little is known about parental responses to CSA cases that occur within or outside those parents’ own households.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    The Optimus Study was initiated and funded by the UBS Optimus Foundation.

    View full text