Elsevier

Hearing Research

Volume 282, Issues 1–2, December 2011, Pages 128-137
Hearing Research

Research paper
The relationship between MOC reflex and masked threshold

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2011.08.007Get rights and content

Abstract

Otoacoustic emission (OAE) amplitude can be reduced by acoustic stimulation. This effect is produced by the medial olivocochlear (MOC) reflex. Past studies have shown that the MOC reflex is related to listening in noise and attention. In the present study, the relationship between strength of the contralateral MOC reflex and masked threshold was investigated in 19 adults. Detection thresholds were determined for 1000-Hz, 300-ms tone presented simultaneously with one repetition of a 300-ms masker in an ongoing train of masker bursts. Three masking conditions were tested: 1) broadband noise 2) a fixed-frequency 4-tone complex masker and 3) a random-frequency 4-tone complex masker. Broadband noise was expected to produce energetic masking and the tonal maskers were expected to produce informational masking in some listeners. DPOAEs were recorded at fine frequency intervals from 500 to 4000 Hz, with and without contralateral acoustic stimulation. MOC reflex strength was estimated as a reduction in baseline level and a shift in frequency of DPOAE fine-structure maxima near 1000-Hz. MOC reflex and psychophysical testing were completed in separate sessions. Individuals with poorer thresholds in broadband noise and in random-frequency maskers were found to have stronger MOC reflexes.

Highlights

► We investigate the relationship between the MOC reflex and masked threshold. ► Listeners with poorer thresholds were found to have stronger MOC reflexes. ► The MOC reflex plays a role in selective listening.

Introduction

The medial olivocochlear (MOC) efferent loop is thought to have a direct effect on cochlear function (e.g., Fex, 1967, Francis and Nadol, 1993, Mountain, 1980; reviewed by Thiers et al., 2002). Neurons in the superior olivary complex receive afferent input via the cochlear nucleus and project to both the ipsilateral and contralateral ears through the crossed and uncrossed olivocochlear bundle, respectively (e.g., Guinan, 2010). By hyperpolarizing outer hair cells, the MOC system reduces cochlear amplifier gain, decreasing the output of the cochlea.

In humans, MOC effects on the cochlear response can be investigated using otoacoustic emissions (OAE). Early studies showed that contralateral acoustic stimulation (CAS) presented simultaneously with the OAE-evoking stimulus, altered the level of the OAE (e.g., Collet et al., 1990, Puel and Rebillard, 1990). An ipsilateral MOC elicitor can also alter the OAE but introduces confounds (Guinan, 2006). The contralateral MOC (MOC) reflex is mediated by the uncrossed efferent fibers from the medial olivary complex (Liberman and Brown, 1986, Robertson and Gummer, 1985).

It is thought that the MOC reflex increases the effective signal-to-noise ratio in the auditory nerve response, thus improving perception in noise. This effect is known as “MOC unmasking” (Guinan, 2006, Guinan, 2010, Kujawa and Liberman, 2001). Physiological evidence in animals (e.g., Kawase et al., 1993, Liberman, 1988, Winslow and Sachs, 1988) has shown that MOC activity improves the auditory nerve’s response to signals by reducing the response to a noisy background, effectively shifting the dynamic range of hearing (e.g., Dolan and Nuttall, 1988, Kawase et al., 1993, Kujawa and Liberman, 2001).

Psychophysical studies have linked the MOC reflex to “overshoot”, the elevated threshold observed when the probe is presented at the onset of a simultaneous masker, compared to the case when the probe is presented some time after the onset of a simultaneous masker (Keefe et al., 2010, Schmidt and Zwicker, 1991, Zwicker, 1965). The magnitude of the MOC reflex has also been linked to attention (Froehlich et al., 1993, Garinis et al., in press, Giard et al., 1994, Harkrider and Bowers, 2009, Perrot et al., 2005) and auditory training (de Boer and Thornton, 2008). However, these studies only indirectly address the question of whether this reflex improves hearing in noise generally.

Three approaches have been used to show a more direct connection between hearing in noise and the MOC effect. One approach has been to show that presenting a sound that would be expected to activate the MOC reflex also improves hearing in noise (Giraud et al., 1997, Kumar and Vanaja, 2004, Micheyl and Collet, 1996, Zeng et al., 2000). These studies demonstrated that the magnitude of improvement in perception with CAS was correlated with the magnitude of the MOC reflex; individuals who showed greater CAS-induced improvements in hearing, also had stronger MOC reflexes. One problem of interpretation in such studies is the possibility of centrally mediated unmasking effects. Although the cited experiments used uncorrelated noise to avoid a binaural masking level difference, interaural level differences can shift the perceived intracranial position of the image evoked by binaural uncorrelated noise (Hartmann and Constan, 2002). This could lead to a release from masking.

The role of the MOC reflex in auditory perception has also been tested by examining the psychoacoustical performance of patients who have had their efferent fibers severed during vestibular neurectomy (VNT) (Giraud et al., 1997, Scharf et al., 1994, Scharf et al., 1997, Zeng et al., 2000). Two of these studies reported that speech recognition in noise was poorer in the operated ear than in the unoperated ear of VNT patients (Giraud et al., 1997, Zeng et al., 2000), although Zeng et al. noted that peripheral hearing loss in the operated ear might have contributed to this effect. Scharf et al. reported that neither tone detection in quiet nor tone-in-noise detection was affected by VNT (but see Tan et al., 2008). Forward masked intensity discrimination at mid levels, overshoot, intensity discrimination in noise and the detection of unexpected frequencies were affected by the VNT surgery, but other psychoacoustical measures appeared to be unaffected (Scharf et al., 1997, Zeng et al., 2000, Zwicker, 1965). Giraud et al. (1997) also found that speech recognition did not improve with CAS nor was OAE level altered. Thus, these studies provide somewhat inconsistent evidence that disruption of the MOC reflex is associated with hearing-in-noise deficits. It is possible that the OCB had only partially been transected in some cases.

Finally, several studies of adults with normal hearing have examined the relationship between the absolute level of psychoacoustic performance in noise and the magnitude of the MOC reflex, reasoning that people with stronger reflexes should be better at processing signals in noise. The results have also been inconsistent. Micheyl and Collet (1996) reported that individuals with stronger MOC reflexes had better thresholds for a 2000-Hz tone in dichotic noise; no relationship was found for a 1000-Hz tone. Bhagat and Carter (2010) reported stronger MOC reflexes in individuals who had better 1000-Hz thresholds; no relationship was found for a 2000-Hz tone. The opposite result has also been reported, as Micheyl et al. (1995) found that individuals with stronger MOC reflexes had poorer thresholds for a three-tone complex in noise. Recently Wagner et al. (2008) failed to detect any relationship between MOC reflex strength and speech recognition in noise.

The present study sought to clarify the relationship between the contralaterally evoked MOC reflex and masked sensitivity in normal-hearing adult listeners. As in the previous studies, the relationship between MOC reflex strength and threshold for a tone in noise was examined. However, threshold was also examined in several masking conditions. Masking of a tone by a broadband noise results from the interaction of the tone and masker at the auditory periphery. This is referred to as energetic masking. Masking also occurs in conditions where alterations in cochlear gain would have little effect on threshold. For example, for many listeners masking occurs when the frequencies in a masker change randomly from presentation to presentation even when there is little or no spectral overlap between the signal and masker. This is called informational masking (Neff and Green, 1987, Pollack, 1975). Recent studies indicate that informational masking of a tone by a tonal complex—with no components near the target-tone frequency—is possible, even when the masker frequencies do not vary (Bonino and Leibold, 2008, Leibold and Werner, 2006; Leibold and Bonino, 2009).

Informational masking reflects a failure of selective listening, rather than limited spectral resolution. As noted above, Scharf et al. (1997) found that, unlike normal-hearing subjects, surgical VNT patients did not detect expected frequencies better than unexpected frequencies in the operated ear. This suggests a deficit in selective listening. Tan et al. (2008) argued that the advantage of expected over unexpected sounds in normal-hearing listeners (tested in the typical psychoacoustic “probe-signal” paradigm) results from an enhancement of activity at the expected signal frequency, mediated by MOC activity. If the MOC reflex plays a role in selective listening, then it may be associated with informational masking; hence, variation in the strength of the MOC reflex could at least partially account for the observed between-subject variability in this type of masking. To address this possibility, the present study assessed the relationship between several indices of MOC reflex strength and masked sensitivity in both energetic and informational masking conditions.

Section snippets

Overview

The relationship between the strength of the MOC effect and masked sensitivity was investigated by taking psychophysical and physiologic measurements from the same subjects. Detection of a 1000-Hz target tone was measured in broadband noise (BBN), a 4-tone complex of fixed frequencies more than an auditory-filter width away from 1000-Hz (FIXED) and a 4-tone complex of randomly varying frequencies more than an auditory-filter width away from 1000-Hz (RANDOM). Energetic masking would be the

Psychophysical results

Mean thresholds are displayed for each masking condition in Fig. 2. The mean thresholds are consistent with those reported in previous studies (e.g., Leibold and Werner, 2006, Leibold et al., 2010). Masked thresholds in the FIXED condition were lower than those in the BBN masker condition, as expected. The between-subject variability in masked threshold in the FIXED condition was high. The FIXED thresholds, on average, were about 10 dB higher than the unmasked threshold for the 1000-Hz tone,

Discussion

The goal of this investigation was to examine the relationship between the strength of contralaterally evoked MOC inhibition and masked threshold in energetic and informational masking conditions. The original hypothesis was that listeners with stronger MOC reflexes would exhibit better thresholds form asked tone detection. Contrary to expectation, the results indicate that adults with stronger MOC effects had higher masked thresholds in the broadband noise masking condition and in one

Conclusion

The present study shows that a stronger contralateral MOC inhibition effect is associated with poorer monaural masked sensitivity in broadband noise and random-frequency tonal maskers in normal-hearing adults tested in a condition with high temporal uncertainty. These results and those of previous studies of the same type suggest that the utility of MOC reflex-related mechanisms in extracting signals from noise may vary with the stimulus and the listening conditions in a complex way. Additional

Acknowledgments

Supported in part by research grants R01 DC000396, R01 DC003552, T32 DC005361 and P30 DC004661 from the National Institutes of Health. The authors would like to express their gratitude to Ashley L. Flad for data analysis and Ping Luo for programming contributions.

References (62)

  • B. Scharf et al.

    On the role of the olivocochlear bundle in hearing: a case study

    Hear. Res.

    (1994)
  • M.N. Tan et al.

    Separate contributions of enhanced and suppressed sensitivity to the auditory attentional filter

    Hear. Res.

    (2008)
  • F.G. Zeng et al.

    Auditory research perception in vestibular neurectomy subjects

    Hear. Res.

    (2000)
  • C. Abdala et al.

    Level dependence of distortion product otoacoustic emission phase is attributed to component mixing

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (2011)
  • C.A. Abdala et al.

    Considering distortion product otoacoustic emission fine structure in measurement of the medial olivocochlear reflex

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (2009)
  • C. Abdala et al.

    Maturation of the medial efferent system function in humans

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (1999)
  • A.Y. Bonino et al.

    The effect of signal-temporal uncertainty on detection in bursts of noise or a random-frequency complex

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (2008)
  • S. Buus et al.

    Decision rules in detection of simple and complex tones

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (1986)
  • R. Deeter et al.

    Contralateral acoustic stimulation alters the magnitude and phase of distortion product otoacoustic emissions

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (2009)
  • J. De Boer et al.

    Neural correlates of perceptual learning in the auditory brainstem: efferent activity predicts and reflects improvement at a speech-in-noise discrimination task

    J. Neurosci.

    (2008)
  • S. Dhar et al.

    Multiple internal reflections in the cochlea and their effect on DPOAE fune structure

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (2002)
  • D.F. Dolan et al.

    Masked cochlear whole- nerve response intensity functions altered by electrical stimulation of the crossed olivocochlear bundle

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (1988)
  • M.P. Feeney et al.

    Contralateral acoustic reflex thresholds for tonal activators using wideband energy reflectance and admittance

    J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res.

    (2003)
  • J. Fex

    Efferent inhibition in the cochlea related to hair-cell dc activity: study of postsynaptic activity of the crossed olivocochlear fibers in the cat

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (1967)
  • Garinis, A.C., Glattke, T.G., Cone, Barbara, The MOC reflex during active listening to speech. J. Speech Lang. Hear....
  • A.L. Giraud et al.

    Auditory efferents involved in speech- in-noise intelligibility

    Neuroreport

    (1997)
  • J.J. Guinan

    Cochlear efferent innervation and function

    Curr. Opin. Otolaryngol. Head. Neck. Surg.

    (2010)
  • J.J. Guinan

    Olivocochlear efferents: anatomy, physiology, function, and the measurement of efferent effects in humans

    Ear Hear.

    (2006)
  • A.S. Hadi

    A modification of a method for the detection of outliers in multivariate samples

    J. Royal Stat. Soc. B-Methodol

    (1994)
  • A.W. Harkrider et al.

    Evidence for a cortically mediated release from inhibition in the human cochlea

    J. Am. Acad. Audiol.

    (2009)
  • W.M. Hartmann et al.

    Interaural level differences and the level-meter model

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (2002)
  • Cited by (17)

    • Contralateral suppression of human hearing sensitivity in single-sided deaf cochlear implant users

      2019, Hearing Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      In humans, cochlear mechanical changes caused by the MOCR can be evidenced using otoacoustics emissions (OAEs; e.g. Guinan, 2006; Francis and Guinan, 2010). In a study by Garinis et al. (2011), the MOCR effect elicited by a contralateral broadband noise reduced the level of distortion product OAEs from about 0.34 to 1.78 dB. Cochlear mechanical changes can be also measured through changes in auditory sensitivity.

    • Adversarial relationship between combined medial olivocochlear (MOC) and middle-ear-muscle (MEM) reflexes and alarm-in-noise detection thresholds under negative signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs)

      2018, Hearing Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Wagner et al. (2008) could not find any significant correlation between speech-in-noise (SIN) detection and the strength of MOC efferent feedback. The current study observed a significant negative correlation between AIN detection thresholds and MOC efferent feedback and thus supports the findings of Micheyl et al. (1995) and Garinis et al. (2011). This study differs from related past studies in that a loud (80 dBA) noise was used.

    • Olivocochlear efferents: Their action, effects, measurement and uses, and the impact of the new conception of cochlear mechanical responses

      2018, Hearing Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Recent work conclusively shows this MOC benefit (Abdala et al., 2014; Mishra and Lutman, 2014; Bidelman and Bhagat, 2015; Maruthy et al., 2017). However, other recent work failed to detect a benefit (Wagner et al., 2008; Garinis et al., 2011) which indicates that there is still an incomplete understanding of the conditions under which MOC activity aids hearing in noise. A large body of data indicates that a major role of both LOC and MOC efferents is to reduce the damage from traumatic sounds.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text