Error-related processing following severe traumatic brain injury: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.06.019Get rights and content

Abstract

Continuous monitoring of one's performance is invaluable for guiding behavior towards successful goal attainment by identifying deficits and strategically adjusting responses when performance is inadequate. In the present study, we exploited the advantages of event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine brain activity associated with error-related processing after severe traumatic brain injury (sTBI). fMRI and behavioral data were acquired while 10 sTBI participants and 12 neurologically-healthy controls performed a task-switching cued-Stroop task. fMRI data were analyzed using a random-effects whole-brain voxel-wise general linear model and planned linear contrasts. Behaviorally, sTBI patients showed greater error-rate interference than neurologically-normal controls. fMRI data revealed that, compared to controls, sTBI patients showed greater magnitude error-related activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and an increase in the overall spatial extent of error-related activation across cortical and subcortical regions. Implications for future research and potential limitations in conducting fMRI research in neurologically-impaired populations are discussed, as well as some potential benefits of employing multimodal imaging (e.g., fMRI and event-related potentials) of cognitive control processes in TBI.

Highlights

► Severe TBI (sTBI) survivors show impairments in performance monitoring. ► fMRI examined error-related brain activation in sTBI survivors and controls. ► Individuals with sTBI showed greater error-related ACC activation than controls. ► sTBIpatients also showed greater overall spatial extent of error-related activity. ► sTBIs exhibit altered neural substrates for error-related processing.

Introduction

Physical and neurobehavioral impairments are common sequelae of traumatic brain injury (TBI; Horn and Sherer, 1999), however, even in patients with good neurological recovery, persistent cognitive deficits are among the most pronounced and frequent complaints of TBI survivors (Cicerone et al., 2005, Lovell and Franzen, 1994). Severity-related impairments in “cognitive control,” a set of higher-order executive processes supported by the prefrontal cortex and critical to executive function (Lorist et al., 2005, Miller, 2000, Miller and Cohen, 2001), are thought to underlie some aspects of enduring cognitive dysfunction after brain injury (Larson et al., 2006, Larson et al., 2007a, Larson et al., in press, Perlstein et al., 2004, Perlstein et al., 2006, Scheibel et al., 2007, Seignourel et al., 2005, Soeda et al., 2005), and current theories of neurobehavioral dysfunction in TBI have been based on observed impairments in cognitive control component processes (Anderson et al., 2002, Burgess and Robertson, 2002, Larson et al., 2006, Larson et al., 2007a, Levine et al., 2002, Perlstein et al., 2006).

Numerous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies (i.e., Carter et al., 1999, MacDonald et al., 2000) suggest that cognitive control comprises two broad component processes implemented in a closely interactive, yet dissociable frontal neural network: a regulative/strategic component supporting the maintenance of task goals, allocation of limited attentional resources, and the implementation of top-down control (MacDonald et al., 2000), and an anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)-mediated evaluative component that supports conflict processing and performance monitoring (i.e., Carter and van Veen, 2007, Kerns et al., 2004, van Veen and Carter, 2002, van Veen and Carter, 2006). These evaluative monitoring processes serve to adjust behavioral performance toward goal attainment based on the detection of performance errors (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004). Continuous performance monitoring is important for guiding behavior towards successful goal-attainment by detecting deficiencies and strategically adjusting responses when current performance is inadequate. An understanding of the neural basis underlying error-related processing is critical not only to identifying the mechanisms through which cognitive control is executed, but also because impairments in self-awareness in TBI patients may partially arise from impaired performance-monitoring abilities (Larson and Perlstein, 2009, O'Keeffe et al., 2004).

Whereas numerous studies of impaired executive function following brain injury have focused their attention on examining the impairment of top-down regulative processes of cognitive control that rely heavily on the dlPFC (i.e., Christodoulou et al., 2001, Larson et al., 2006, McAllister et al., 2001, Perlstein et al., 2004, Perlstein et al., 2006, Seignourel et al., 2005), research examining the impairment of performance monitoring functions of cognitive control, or the potential role that alterations in ACC function contribute to cognitive dysfunction after brain injury is limited (i.e., Larson et al., 2007a, Scheibel et al., 2003, Soeda et al., 2005). Importantly, available neuroimaging and electrophysiological findings from studies conducted both in- and-outside of our laboratory have provided evidence demonstrating alterations in ACC-mediated evaluative activity in TBI patients. Specifically, electrophysiological studies have demonstrated that TBI patients display attenuated scalp-recorded event-related potential (ERP) components thought to reflect ACC-mediated evaluative monitoring aspects of control including the conflict-related N450 (Perlstein et al., 2006), error-related negativity (ERN; Larson et al., 2007a, Stemmer et al., 2004), and feedback-related negativity (FRN; Larson et al., 2007b). Similarly, alterations in ACC-mediated evaluative activation have also been observed using fMRI, however, results have been contradictory. For example, Soeda et al. (2005) observed reduced ACC activation in TBI patients during completion of a modified Stroop task that elicited a high degree of response conflict, Scheibel et al. (2007) observed greater ACC activation in TBI patients during completion of a stimulus-response compatibility cognitive control task. Findings from both studies (Scheibel et al., 2007, Soeda et al., 2005) suggest that neural networks mediating cognitive control and evaluative processes of control are disrupted after brain injury; however, these findings do not account for error-related activity, and methodological limitations (i.e., use of blocked fMRI designs) in the studies described above also preclude full interpretation of results.

In the present study, we build upon available research findings and address the methodological limitations described above by exploiting the advantages of event-related fMRI which enables us to separately evaluate correct- and incorrect-trial response activity and, therefore, to examine potential alterations of activity reflecting error-related processing after TBI. Specifically, we tested the hypothesis that in comparison to healthy controls, patients with severe TBI (sTBI) would show smaller magnitude error-related activation of the ACC during completion of a cued-Stroop task, a cognitive task that has been found to reliably elicit a high degree of cognitive control and response conflict (Kerns et al., 2004, West, 2003).

Section snippets

Participants

Ten individuals with sTBI were recruited from two Northern Florida trauma and rehabilitation hospitals and the local community, including meetings of the Florida Brain Injury Association, the Brain and Spinal Cord Injury Program of Florida, and local Brain Injury Association support groups. Twelve demographically-similar control participants were recruited by advertisement from the local community. All individuals provided written informed consent in accordance with procedures established by

Task performance

RTs and error rates for the cued-Stroop task (Fig. 2) were positively and not significantly correlated for control, r(11) = .26, p > .41, or TBI participants, r(9) = .54, p > .10, suggesting that speed–accuracy trade-off was not a significant factor in task performance for either group.

Discussion

We examined error-related brain activity in survivors of sTBI using event-related fMRI acquired in the context of performing a Stroop task. Our primary aim was to determine if, relative to demographically-similar healthy controls, sTBI participants exhibit altered error-related activity reflective of impaired performance monitoring. Our initial prediction was that sTBI patients would exhibit reduced error-related activity in the ACC. However, contrary to this prediction, sTBI patients showed

Summary and conclusions

In conclusion, results from the present study extend previous findings that the neural networks mediating cognitive control, specifically, error-related processing, are disrupted after sTBI. Despite its limitations, the current study supports the continued neurophysiological examination of complex processes of cognitive control. Future studies will aim to identify outcome measures of “real-world” functioning that will further examine the impact of these deficits and inform rehabilitation

Acknowledgements

This original research manuscript, or parts of it, have not been and will not be submitted elsewhere for publication. This work was supported by NIH grants K01-MH01857 and R21-MH073076, and by grants from the Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Research Grant Program, and the Florida Brain and Spinal Cord Injury Research Trust Fund awarded to WMP. The work was submitted by the first author in partial fulfillment of requirements for the Master of Science degree. We extend our appreciation to Ashley

References (67)

  • R. West

    Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • V. Anderson et al.

    Executive functions after frontal lobe injury: a developmental perspective

  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Beck Depression Inventory — Second Edition (BDI-II)

    (1996)
  • E.D. Bigler

    Neuropathology of traumatic brain injury

  • J.R. Blair et al.

    Predicting premorbid IQ: a revision of the National Adult Reading Test

    The Clinical Neuropsychologist

    (1989)
  • M.R. Bond

    Neurobehavioral sequelae of closed head injury

  • G.J. Bouma et al.

    Cerebral circulation and metabolism after severe traumatic brain injury: the elusive role of ischemia

    Journal of Neurosurgery

    (1991)
  • G.A. Boynton et al.

    Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1996)
  • P.W. Burgess et al.

    Principles of the rehabilitation of frontal lobe function

  • C.S. Carter et al.

    Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: an update of theory and data

    Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • C.S. Carter et al.

    The role of the anterior cingulate cortex in executive processes of cognition

    Reviews in the Neurosciences

    (1999)
  • C. Christodoulou et al.

    Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working memory impairment following traumatic brain injury

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences

    (1988)
  • J.D. Cohen et al.

    Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: converging evidence from three theoretically motivated cognitive tasks

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (1999)
  • N. Doñamayor et al.

    Coupling electrophysiological and hemodynamic responses to errors

    Human Brain Mapping

    (2011)
  • C. Easdon et al.

    Neural activity associated with response inhibition following traumatic brain injury: an event-related fMRI investigation

    Brain and Cognition

    (2004)
  • S.D. Forman et al.

    Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold

    Magnetic Resonance in Medicine

    (1995)
  • K.J. Friston et al.

    Statistical parametric maps in functional neuroimaging

    NeuroImage

    (1995)
  • R.D. Frostig et al.

    Cortical functional architecture and local coupling between neuronal activity and the microcirculation revealed by in vivo high-resolution optical imaging of intrinsic signals

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (1990)
  • R. Goebel et al.

    Analysis of function image analysis contest (FIAC) data with Brainvoyager QX: from single-subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self-organizing group independent component analysis

    Human Brain Mapping

    (2006)
  • J.P. Hayes et al.

    Alterations in neural circuitry for emotion and attention associated with posttraumatic stress symptomatology

    Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging

    (2009)
  • D.J. Heeger et al.

    Spikes versus BOLD: what does neuroimaging tell us about neuronal activity?

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • F.G. Hillary et al.

    Functional magnetic resonance imaging technology and traumatic brain injury rehabilitation: guidelines for methodological and conceptual pitfalls

    The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation

    (2002)
  • Cited by (23)

    • Effects of inter-alpha inhibitor proteins on brain injury after exposure of neonatal rats to severe hypoxia-ischemia

      2020, Experimental Neurology
      Citation Excerpt :

      The brain weights and the percent infarct volumes between the subjects in the immediate and delayed IAIP treatment groups were also compared by ANOVAs with the Tukey's honestly significant difference (HSD) test for multiple comparisons used as a post-hoc test. The generalized extreme studentized deviate (ESD) test was used to detect outliers (Rosner, 1983; Sozda et al., 2011; You et al., 2016) using Excel software (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, Washington, USA). Outliers were only apparent in the infarct volume analysis for the delayed treatment group.

    • The association between a history of concussion and variability in behavioral and neuroelectric indices of cognition

      2015, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Sport-related concussions represent a growing public-health threat with conservative estimates indicating that nearly 4 million concussive injuries occur annually (Langlois et al., 2006) and account for almost 9% of all high school sport-related injuries (Gessel et al., 2007). While concussion related symptoms appear to resolve within 10 to 14 days in acutely injured individuals (McCrea et al., 2009; McCrory et al., 2013), a number of concussion-related patterns of neurocognitive impairment appear to persist well beyond this time frame (Giza and Hovda, 2001; Halterman et al., 2006; Howell et al., 2013; Sozda et al., 2011). Indeed, previous research has observed persistent decrements in neurocognition for years following concussive injuries (de Beaumont et al., 2007; Moore et al., 2014).

    • Making sense of all the conflict: A theoretical review and critique of conflict-related ERPs

      2014, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      If conflict is not adequately detected and subsequent adjustments in behavior and neural resources are not implemented, goal-directed behaviors will falter. For example, neuropsychologists working with individuals who have experienced moderate or severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) frequently report that these individuals make the same mistakes repeatedly even when corrective feedback is provided (Demery et al., 2010; Larson et al., 2009a, 2007a, 2007b; Slomine et al., 2002; Sozda et al., 2011). Studies from a conflict monitoring perspective suggest that the continued errors in those with TBI are due to poor detection and subsequent resolution of response conflict (Duncan et al., 2011; Larson et al., 2011a, 2009a, 2007a).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text