Elsevier

Intelligence

Volume 36, Issue 1, January–February 2008, Pages 81-95
Intelligence

Still just 1 g: Consistent results from five test batteries

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.06.001Get rights and content

Abstract

In a recent paper, Johnson, Bouchard, Krueger, McGue, and Gottesman (2004) addressed a long-standing debate in psychology by demonstrating that the g factors derived from three test batteries administered to a single group of individuals were completely correlated. This finding provided evidence for the existence of a unitary higher-level general intelligence construct whose measurement is not dependent on the specific abilities assessed. In the current study we constructively replicated this finding utilizing five test batteries. The replication is important because there were substantial differences in both the sample and the batteries administered from those in the original study. The current sample consisted of 500 Dutch seamen of very similar age and somewhat truncated range of ability. The batteries they completed included many tests of perceptual ability and dexterity, and few verbally oriented tests. With the exception of the g correlations involving the Cattell Culture Fair Test, which consists of just four matrix reasoning tasks of very similar methodology, all of the g correlations were at least .95. The lowest g correlation was .77. We discuss the implications of this finding.

Section snippets

Sample

We made use of the data matrix of 46 mental ability tests published by de Wolff and Buiten (1963). This matrix is reprinted here as Appendix A. We did not have access to individual participant data of any kind. The sample on which the matrix was based consisted of 500 professional seamen of the Royal Dutch Navy. Thus the sample consisted entirely of male naval volunteers. Virtually all the seamen were 16 years of age at time of testing, in the summer of 1961. They were so-called sailors third

Test Battery of the Royal Dutch Navy

For this battery, we extracted three factors, which we labeled Mechanical Ability, Problem Solving, and Perceptual Speed. We chose 3 factors by examining several possible numbers of factors and choosing the solution that caused the RMSEA to be less than .08 (indicating a reasonable fit according to Browne & Cudeck, 1992) and which provided the most clearly interpretable solution. The RMSEA for the model was .071 (chi-square = 24.75, 7 df, p = .001). We would have preferred a model with an RMSEA

Discussion

Our goal in this study was constructively to replicate Johnson et al.'s (2004) observation that the g factors from three independently developed test batteries were completely correlated. We did this in a very demographically different sample from the one that generated the original observation. This sample completed five independently developed test batteries, none of which was included among the batteries used in the earlier study. For these reasons alone, our current results provide a very

Acknowledgement

We thank Arne Evers, Jacqueline van Vliet-Mulder (both of the Dutch Test Committee), Jacques Dane (Archives of Dutch Psychology, University of Groningen), and especially Jan van de Linde for their help in finding information about the older tests. We are grateful to Charl de Wolff and Bert Buiten for enthusiastically giving additional details on their valuable dataset. Wendy Johnson holds a Research Council of the United Kingdom Fellowship.

References (29)

  • BurosO.K.

    The fifth mental measurements yearbook

    (1959)
  • CattellR.B.

    Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action

    (1971)
  • de WolffC.J. et al.

    Een factoranalyse van vier testbatterijen [A factor analysis of four test batteries]

    Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie

    (1963)
  • DearyI.J.

    g and cognitive elements of information processing: An agnostic view

  • Cited by (221)

    • Are Piagetian scales just intelligence tests?

      2022, Intelligence
      Citation Excerpt :

      The shared nature of general intelligence measured with Piagetian and psychometric instruments despite no overlap in test content, construction practices, or underlying theory provides immense support for the construct validity of g and supports it as a reflective construct. However, like earlier findings of this nature have stated (Johnson et al., 2004, 2008), it should be qualified that this does not mean specific abilities are unsystematic or unimportant. Whatever the causes of g, whether they be sampling (Bartholomew, Deary, & Lawn, 2009), process overlap (Kovacs & Conway, 2016), mutualistic coupling (Van Der Maas et al., 2006), genetic generalism (de la Fuente, Davies, Grotzinger, Tucker-Drob, & Deary, 2021; Plomin, Kovas, & Haworth, 2007; Trzaskowski, Shakeshaft, & Plomin, 2013), or wiring (Savi, Marsman, van der Maas, & Maris, 2019), they must respect that the general factor – statistical or real – seems to be reflective.

    • It takes more than meta-analysis to kill cognitive ability

      2023, Industrial and Organizational Psychology
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text