Elsevier

Journal of Human Evolution

Volume 46, Issue 2, February 2004, Pages 185-213
Journal of Human Evolution

Curvilinear, geometric and phylogenetic modeling of basicranial flexion: is it adaptive, is it constrained?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2003.11.001Get rights and content

Abstract

Prior work has shown that the degree of basicranial flexion among primates is determined by relative brain size, with anatomically modern humans possibly having a less flexed basicranium than expected for their relative brain size. Basicranial flexion has also been suggested to be adaptive in that it maintains a spheroid brain shape, thereby minimizing connections between different parts of the brain. In addition, it has been argued that the degree of flexion might be constrained such that increases in relative brain size beyond that seen in Australopithecus africanus were accommodated by mechanisms other than basicranial flexion. These hypotheses were evaluated by collating an extensive data set on basicranial flexion and relative brain size in primates and other mammals. The data were analyzed using standard least squares regression, geometric and curvilinear modeling, and phylogenetically independent contrasts (PICs). Geometric modeling does not support the hypothesis that flexion is an adaptation that facilitates enlargement of a spheroid brain. Whether humans have a less flexed basicranium than expected for their relative brain size depends on the phylogenetic vantage point from which it is evaluated. They are as flexed as expected for a descendant of the last common ancestor of the Paranthropus–Homo clade, but their degree of flexion cannot be predicted from the basal hominoid node, even if their relative brain size is specified. Humans undoubtedly occupy an unusual part of morphospace in terms of basicranial flexion and relative brain size, but this does not mean that their degree of flexion is or is not constrained. Curvilinear regression models and standard linear regression models describe the relationship between flexion and relative brain size equally well. Hypotheses that the degree of flexion is or is not constrained cannot be discriminated at present. Consideration of recently published ontogenetic data in the context of the interspecific data for adults suggests that much of the variance in basicranial flexion can still be explained as a mechanical consequence of brain enlargement relative to basicranial length.

Introduction

The degree of flexion of the midline cranial base in primates is of interest because the cranial base is integrated with many structural and functional systems in the head (Lieberman et al., 2000). Changes in the degree of flexion therefore may impact various functional systems. Several factors have been hypothesized to be important in determining the degree of flexion, including head and neck posture, facial orientation, and brain size. The hypothesis gaining the most attention inrecent years is Gould's (1977) suggestion that the degree of basicranial flexion is determined by the size of the brain relative to the length of the basicranium. Ross and Ravosa (1993)corroborated this hypothesis in interspecific comparisons across non-hominid primates. They found that as neurocranial volume relative to basicranial length increases, so the cranial base becomes more flexed (Ross and Ravosa, 1993). Subsequent work on adult primates has supported the general tenor of Gould's hypothesis, but Jeffery and Spoor's investigations reveal that in fetal primates increasing relative brain size is actually associated with decreased basicranial flexion, or flattening of the cranial base (Jeffery, 1999, Jeffery, 2002a, Jeffery, 2003; Jeffery and Spoor, 2002).

Unresolved questions fall into three groups: Is basicranial flexion adaptive or is it a mechanical consequence of brain enlargement? Are there phylogenetic effects on basicranial flexion and how do they impact on tests of the other hypotheses? Is basicranial flexion functionally constrained, as hypothesized by Ross and Henneberg (1995)?

Adaptive explanations for basicranial flexion link it to three functions: reducing the energy required to support the head on an orthograde neck, improving braincase strength by maintaining its spheroid shape in context of brain enlargement, or minimizing wiring length in the brain by maintaining a spheroid brain shape. Strait and Ross (1999)have addressed the postural hypothesis elsewhere. Taking relative brain size and head posture into account, they found no evidence to suggest that variation in flexion is related to variation in head and neck orientation. Instead, relative brain size was the more important determinant of flexion. Thus, although basicranial flexion is often regarded as some kind of postural adaptation, there are no data to support this hypothesis (see also Lieberman et al., 2000).

Is basicranial flexion a mechanism for accommodating increases in relative brain size while retaining a spheroid brain shape (Ross and Henneberg, 1995; Lieberman et al., 2000)? Two possible advantages of a spheroid brain shape have been suggested. First, a spheroid brain might optimize interneuronal communication by minimizing distances between neurons (Ross and Henneberg, 1995). Minimizing of “wiring length” has been suggested to be an important design principle of neural architecture (Allman and Kaas, 1974; Barlow, 1986; Mitchison, 1991; Cherniak, 1995; Van Essen, 1997) and maintenance of a spheroid brain by increased basicranial flexion may be a way to minimize “wiring length” at the level of overall brain shape. Second, a spheroid brain might result from selection for a spheroid braincase. A spheroid braincase provides the strongest and most economical housing for the brain (Ross and Ravosa, 1993) and a spheroid braincase with a flexed base also has the advantage of reducing stresses in the poorly stress resistant region of the basicranium rostral to the occipital condyles (Demes, 1985).

Here we test the hypothesis that basicranial flexion accommodates increasing brain size relative to basicranial length in order to keep the shape of the brain and braincase spheroid. Geometric models are used to generate a variety of predictions regarding relationships between basicranial flexion, basicranial length, and brain shape in the mid-sagittal plane if sphericity in the measured neurocranial volumes is being optimized. The primate data are compared with these models and the closeness of fit between them is used to evaluate the models.

As noted above, it has been suggested that ontogenetic data from Homo sapiens, Macaca nemestrina and Alouatta caraya falsify thehypothesis that increasing basicranial flexion is a mechanical consequence of increasing relative brain size (Jeffery, 1999, Jeffery, 2003; Jeffery and Spoor, 2002). Here we present all the available adult and ontogenetic data and address the issues raised by Jeffery and Spoor.

Recent comparisons of various ways of measuring flexion and relative brain size revealed taxonomic differences in patterns of variation in basicranial flexion measures. The definitions of the different measures are given in Table 1. Analysis of variance components at different taxonomic levels (Smith, 1994) reveals that for one of the measures of relative brain size and one of the measures of basicranial flexion (IRE1 and CBA4) most of the variance is found at the family level, followed by the superfamily and then the genus levels. In contrast, for the other measures of relative brain size and basicranial flexion (IRE5 and CBA1), most of the variance is found at the Infraorder level (Lieberman et al., 2000). Adjusting degrees of freedom to account for these taxonomic effects (Smith, 1994), IRE1 and CBA4 were found to be significantly correlated with each other at P<0.01, as were IRE5 and CBA1. However, correlations between IRE5 and CBA4, and between IRE1 and CBA1 were only significant at P<0.05 (Lieberman et al., 2000). Thus, the highest correlation coefficients and degrees of significance were found between variables that showed maximum variance at the same taxonomic levels. This suggests that phylogenetic effects can affect patterns of correlation between basicranial variables and that phylogenetic relationships among primate taxa need to be accounted for in evaluating hypotheses of relationship.

Following the publication of Felsenstein's seminal 1985 paper, techniques for incorporating patterns of phylogenetic relatedness into comparative analyses have been developed and made available in the form of computer software such as CAIC, COMPARE, and PDAP (Garland et al., 1993, Garland et al., 1999; Garland and Ives, 2000; Blomberg et al., 2003; Rohlf, 2002). In this study the PDTREE component of the PDAP software is used to test hypotheses regarding correlations and regressions between measures of basicranial flexion andrelative brain size using Phylogenetically Independent Contrasts (PICs). To explore the importance of phylogenetic relatedness in analyzing these data, the relationships between these variables estimated using “tip” data (data from the tips of the phylogenetic tree without regard to branching pattern) are compared with the relationships estimated using PICs. In addition, the descriptive statistic, K, is calculated to estimate the strength of the phylogenetic signal in each variable (Blomberg et al., 2003). Various branch length options available in PDTREE are explored to determine the relative importance of tree topology and branch length on the patterns of correlation and regression measured.

Ross and Henneberg (1995)extended the Ross and Ravosa (1993)study to hominins and found Homo sapiens to be less flexed than expected. They interpreted these results as suggesting that the degree of basicranial flexion is constrained by the functional necessities of pharyngeal anatomy. This possibility arises because increased basicranial flexion among anthropoids is associated with increased facial kyphosis (Ross and Ravosa, 1993), so that as the anterior cranial base flexes ventrally relative to the posterior cranial base, so the palate (and orbits) rotate ventrally (i.e., facial kyphosis increases). Ross and Henneberg argued that the functional exigencies of pharyngeal anatomy must limit such facial kyphosis, hence constraining basicranial flexion as well. They also suggested that the brain dorsal to the notochord might preclude retroflexion of the basicranium because this would impinge upon the space available for the brain, leading them to suggest that 180 degrees might be the upper limit for basicranial flexion.

Previous debates regarding these hypotheses of constraint have focused on the methods for quantifying basicranial length and flexion (Spoor, 1997; Lieberman et al., 2000; McCarthy, 2001). McCarthy (2001)showed that basicranial length is most appropriately measured as the sum of the distance from basion to sella to foramen caecum (Basicranial length 2, or BL2 of Lieberman et al., 2000) and relative brain size is best measured using Index of Relative Encephalization 5 (IRE5). When the Ross and Ravosa flexion angle (CBA4) is regressed against IRE5, the hypothesis of constraint is confirmed: Homo sapiens have a less flexed basicranium than predicted (Liebermanet al., 2000; McCarthy, 2001). However, when flexion is measured using the more traditional angle defined by lines joining sella to basion and to foramen caecum, Homo sapiens has the degree of basicranial flexion predicted for its degree of relative encephalization (Spoor, 1997; Lieberman et al., 2000; McCarthy, 2001). Thus, for a primate with its relative brain size, Homo sapiens' planum sphenoideum is less flexed than expected (see above) but its foramen caecum is positioned as predicted. The meaning of these results remains to be fully explored, but it does suggest that different parts of the primate cranial base are either influenced by different factors, or by the same factors but to different degrees (Lieberman et al., 2000).

To test the hypothesis of constraint suggested by Ross and Henneberg (1995)we investigate whether CBA4 of humans is unusual for a primate of their relative brain size. In the past this has been assessed by calculating reduced major axis (RMA) lines calculated for nonhuman primates, and using the position of Homo relative to the estimated 95% confidence limits of those lines to assess whether the mean value for Homo sapiens deviates significantly from the nonhuman primate line (Ross and Henneberg, 1995; Spoor, 1997; Lieberman et al., 2000; McCarthy, 2001). Here we use a method, presented by Garland and Ives (2000), for using PICs to determine whether a species deviates from an allometric relationship. Their method calculates the prediction intervals for traits in a hypothetical species attached to a specific part of the phylogenetic tree by a specific branch length. The prediction intervals are calculated using the standard errors of the least-squares regression (LSR) calculated with PICs. When Homo sapiens falls inside the prediction intervals, this is taken to corroborate the hypothesis that, given their position in the primate phylogenetic tree, their degree of basicranial flexion and relative brain size are not unexpected.

We also evaluate the hypothesis of constraint on flexion by investigating nonlinear models of the relationships between relative brain size and basicranial flexion. Various nonlinear equations describe lines with sigmoidal or logistic shapes, as predicted if the degree of flexion is limited at higher or lower relative brain sizes. We use software (CurveExpert 1.3) to search for the best curve to describe the data and compare these models with the standard linear models traditionally used in studies of basicranial evolution. Because Ross and Henneberg's hypothesis of constraint makes predictions regarding the relationship between flexion and relative brain size at small brain sizes, data on several species of nonprimate mammals are included in these curvilinear analyses.

Section snippets

Sample

The core of the data on basicranial flexion and relative brain size were collected previously (Ross and Ravosa, 1993; Ross and Henneberg, 1995; Strait and Ross, 1999; Lieberman et al., 2000). The complete data set consists of 64 species of living primates, three fossil primate specimens, and 31 species of non-primate mammals (Table 2). The primates, tree shrews and dermopterans were grouped as euarchontans following Murphy et al. (2001).

Radiographic methods

The non-primate mammal and primate specimens were

Raw data

The mean values for all taxa used in this study are given in Table 2.

Does basicranial flexion facilitate spheroid enlargement of the brain?

Do changes in flexion and the AP/SI measure of shape match those predicted by a circular/semi-circular geometric model? In Fig. 4the braincase shape ratio AP/SI is plotted against the two measures of flexion, CBA1 and CBA 4. Also plotted is the relationship between the AP/SI ratio and the CBAs expected under the geometric model (Equation (1)). Although in neither case do the observed relationships very closely match that

Discussion

The basicranial flexion data set presented here constitutes the largest yet gathered and, as such, provides an opportunity for evaluating hypotheses previously advanced in the literature. The nature of these hypotheses also provides an opportunity to evaluate the significance of some techniques for testing hypotheses in a phylogenetic context.

Conclusions

Previous work has indicated that the degree of basicranial flexion in primates is correlated with relative brain size, that humans might be less flexed than predicted for their relative brain size, and that this might suggest that the degree of flexion in relatively large brained species is constrained (Ross and Ravosa, 1993; Ross and Henneberg, 1995). It had also been suggested that flexion might enable the brain to maintain a spheroid shape while increasing in relative size (Lieberman et al.,

Acknowledgements

We thank Matt Cartmill and Dan Lieberman for access to x-rays in their care and Dan Lieberman for the generous loan of an x-ray machine. Rob McCarthy and Kenneth Mowbray assisted in x-raying of specimens. The staff of the AMNH, including the late Wolfgang Fuchs, BMNH, Field Museum and British Museum (Natural History) provided access to specimens in their care. Brigitte Demes, Dan Lieberman, Fred Spoor and three anonymous reviewers provided insightful comments on the manuscript. Bill Jungers and

References (53)

  • D Strait

    The scaling of basicranial flexion and length

    J. Hum. Evol.

    (1999)
  • S.P Blomberg et al.

    Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile

    Evolution

    (2003)
  • A Dabelow

    Über Korrelationenin der phylogenetischen Entwicklung der Schädelform

    Gegenbaurs Morphol Jahrb

    (1985)
  • A Dabelow

    Über Korrelationenin der phylogenetischen Entwicklung der Schädelform. II. Die Beziehungen zwischen Gehirn und Schädelbasisform bei den Mammaliern

    Gegenbaurs Morphol Jahrb

    (1985)
  • A.B Demes
    (1985)
  • E.L DuBrul

    Posture, locomotion and the skull in Lagomorpha

    Am. J. Anat.

    (1950)
  • E Eizirik et al.

    Molecular phylogeny and dating of early primate divergences

  • J Felsenstein

    Phylogenies and the comparative method

    Am. Nat.

    (1985)
  • T Garland et al.

    Procedures for the analysis of comparative data using phylogenetically independent contrasts

    Systematic Biology

    (1985)
  • Garland,T. Jr, Midford, P.E., Jones, J.A., Dickerman, A.W., Diaz-Uriarte, R., PDAP: Phenotypic Diversity Analysis...
  • T Garland et al.

    An introduction to phylogenetically based statistical methods, with a new method for confidence intervals on ancestral states

    Am. Zool.

    (1999)
  • T Garland et al.

    Phylogenetic analysis of covariance by computer simulation

    Syst. Biol.

    (1993)
  • T Garland et al.

    Using the past to predict the present: Confidence intervals for regression equations in phylogenetic comparative methods

    American Naturalist

    (2000)
  • A.W Gentry

    Fossil Bovidae (Mammalia) from Langabaanweg

    Annls S. Afr. Mus.

    (1980)
  • S.J Gould

    Ontogeny and Phylogeny

    (1977)
  • A Grafen

    The phylogenetic regression

    Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B

    (1989)
  • Cited by (58)

    • Facing the facts: The Runx2 gene is associated with variation in facial morphology in primates

      2017, Journal of Human Evolution
      Citation Excerpt :

      The midface is also integrated with the cranial base in anthropoids because its border with the cranial base (the posterior maxillary [PM] plane) is positioned roughly perpendicular to the horizontal axis of the orbits (the neutral horizontal axis; Enlow and McNamara, 1971, 1973; Enlow and Moyers, 1971; Enlow and Azuma, 1975; Enlow, 1990; McCarthy and Lieberman, 2001). These structural constraints may suggest that downstream effects of changes in the cranial base (and the factors that influence the relevant morphology of the cranial base, primarily the relative size of the brain [Ross and Ravosa, 1993, Ross and Henneberg, 1995; Ross et al., 2004; Ravosa et al., 2006]) exert a more powerful effect on facial angle during growth and development. Therefore, these downstream effects may be more important (in both statistical and biological senses) than underlying genetic architecture (e.g., the Runx2 gene) in modulating facial orientation in anthropoid primates.

    • Language and human evolution

      2017, Journal of Neurolinguistics
    • Cranial base topology and basic trends in the facial evolution of Homo

      2016, Journal of Human Evolution
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, hypotheses of biomechanics and respiratory models accounting for distinctive facial morphology are not supported by recent quantitative analyses (O'Connor et al., 2005; Holton and Franciscus, 2008; Rae et al., 2011), and random processes (e.g., genetic drift) have been suggested as ultimate causes of Neanderthal craniofacial form (Weaver, 2009). As alternatives to functional adaptations or random changes, models of evolutionary and developmental integration have been proposed that link large-scale evolutionary changes in size and position of the face to developmental shifts of the brain and/or cranial base topology and embryological brain-face interactions (Enlow, 1990; Rosas, 1992; Ross and Ravosa, 1993; Ross et al., 2004; Lieberman, 2011; Marcucio et al., 2011). According to this perspective, evolutionary brain expansion is a primary agent affecting underlying cranial base topology and this, in turn, would have modified facial form via developmental cascades, because the face is physically attached to the external cranial base (physical “knock-on” effects; Biegert, 1957; Enlow, 1990; Rosas, 1992; Lieberman et al., 2000a, b; Lieberman, 2011; Marcucio et al., 2011; Martínez-Abadías et al., 2012).

    • Effects of brain and facial size on basicranial form in human and primate evolution

      2010, Journal of Human Evolution
      Citation Excerpt :

      Because phylogenetic relatedness can influence the interactions between basicranial morphology, brain and facial size, we performed all analyses also using the comparative method of independent contrasts (Garland et al., 2005). The phylogeny on which the contrasts were built and the branching times of the nodes were taken from Ross et al., (2004). The mapping of the analyses (multiple multivariate regressions, PLS) on phylogeny was performed using MorphoJ (Klingenberg, 2009).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text