Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 25, Issue 2, 1 April 2005, Pages 462-470
NeuroImage

Connectivity exploration with structural equation modeling: an fMRI study of bimanual motor coordination

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.007Get rights and content

Abstract

The present fMRI study explores the connectivity among motor areas in a bimanual coordination task using the analysis framework of structural equation modeling (SEM). During bimanual finger tapping at different frequency ratios, temporal correlations of activations between left/right primary motor cortices (MI), left/right PMdc (caudal dorsal premotor area) and supplementary motor cortex (SMA) were detected and used as inputs to the SEM analysis. SEM was extended from its traditional role as a confirmatory analysis to be used as an exploratory technique to determine the most statistically significant connectivity model given a set of cortical areas based on anatomic constraints. The resultant network exhibits coupling from left MI to right MI, links from both PMs to the two MIs, a negative interaction from left PM to right PM, and functional influence from SMA to right MI and right PM, revealing contributions of these areas to bimanual coordination.

Introduction

The coupling or interference between dual motor tasks by two hands is familiar in our daily experience and has been investigated in many psychophysical and MEG experiments (Chan and Chan, 1995, Franz et al., 1991, Jirsa et al., 1998, Mayville et al., 2001, Tuller and Kelso, 1989, Yamanishi et al., 1980). For example, in an experimental study on spatial coupling of dual motor tasks (Chan and Chan, 1995), participants were asked to perform continuous simultaneous drawing of circles and straight lines using both hands simultaneously. There was a strong tendency for the line to become more circle-like and the circle to become more line-like, as measured by orientation ratio (i.e., height/width) of the circle or line. Such phenomena were termed a spatial magnet effect, and such interference was referred to as “coupling” between the left and right motor systems (Franz et al., 1991, Kelso et al., 1988).

In addition to the interaction described above, the frequency ratios (between two hands) for which polyrhythmic bimanual movement is stable were found to follow a Farey series. Specifically, with a/b and c/d as two base levels of stable frequency ratios, the ratio (a + b) / (c + d) produces the frequency ratio for the next level of (decreased) stability (Treffer and Turvey, 1993). In the experiments of bimanual circle/line drawing task described above, it was shown that the spatial coupling (or interference, i.e., the deviation of orientation ratio of the drawn circles from that of the perfect circle) was the strongest at the ratio of 1/1 (Level 0), the second strongest at 1/2 (Level 1), and the third strongest at 2/3 (Level 2) (Chan and Chan, 1995).

In the context of similar bimanual coordination tasks, such as anti-phase vs. in-phase hand movement or poly-rhythmic finger tapping, recent functional imaging studies (Debaere et al., 2003, Debaere et al., 2004, De Weerd et al., 2003, Immisch et al., 2001, Jancke et al., 2000, Koeneke et al., 2004, Sadato et al., 1997, Toyokura et al., 1999) indicated strong involvement of SMA in motor coordination between two hands. The role of premotor area in the bimanual motor coordination was also studied in depth by fMRI (Debaere et al., 2003, Debaere et al., 2004, De Weerd et al., 2003, Immisch et al., 2001, Koeneke et al., 2004, Sadato et al., 1997). In recent fMRI and electrophysiological studies (Debaere et al., 2003, Debaere et al., 2004, De Weerd et al., 2003, Donchin et al., 1998, Koeneke et al., 2004, Toyokura et al., 1999), an interaction between contralateral primary motor cortices was found during bimanual coordination tasks. Subcortical structures such as basal ganglia and cerebellum were also found activated during bimanual tasks (Tracy et al., 2001, Debaere et al., 2003, Debaere et al., 2004).

Two biological models of motor coordination attempted to explain these coupling phenomena (Cardoso de Oliveira, 2002, Debaere et al., 2003, Debaere et al., 2004): generalized motor programs (GMP) and intermanual crosstalk model. The GMP was inspired by the strong tendency for spatiotemporal similarity of bimanual movements and proposed that there could be a common motor plan for both hands (Schmidt, 1975). In contrast to GMP, the theory of intermanual crosstalk generally suggested that the interactions between the movements of the two arms resulted from partial intermingling (crosstalk) between two independent manual motor plans (Marteniuk and MacKenzie, 1980). Alternatively, GMP could be applied in a hand-specific manner in which the two independent motor plans of the crosstalk model can be viewed as the lowest level of the GMP.

Despite this great body of work, the underlying neural network responsible for bimanual coordination is far from well understood; questions remaining to be answered include where the GMP is generated and whether there exists crosstalk between bilateral primary motor cortices, premotor areas and SMA (Cardoso de Oliveira, 2002, Kelso et al., 1988). Neural interactions, or interchanges of neuronal (electrical or chemical) signals between different brain sites, may be one of the keys to understanding these motor coordination phenomena (Sadato et al., 1997), given that most existing literature (De Weerd et al., 2003, Immisch et al., 2001, Jancke et al., 2000, Koeneke et al., 2004, Toyokura et al., 1999) indicates that bimanual motor coordination requires a group of cortical regions but does not provide detailed information on interactions and couplings between these regions.

Many challenges exist in the analysis of fMRI data to ascertain connectivity relationships across motor cortices during bimanual coordination tasks. First, the widely used correlation analysis across multiple spatial regions cannot reveal causal directions. This is because a correlation coefficient only indicates the degree with which two time courses co-vary with each other, but is not able to provide directional information on the interaction between these two regions. A second challenge is that the observed temporal latency of the BOLD response between different brain areas cannot be used to ascertain the temporal order of neural activities. This occurs because the local hemodynamic response, which depends on the local physiology, could be region specific, blur or delay the temporal evolution of BOLD signals, and make it unable to reflect the timing of neuronal events at different brain sites. Consequently, observed temporal differences in event-related paradigms are not reliable for retrieving information about the direction of the interaction. The third challenge is that many traditional analysis methods fail to provide a complete account of interactions if more than two brain regions are involved. When studying more than two regions, each one can interact with several other regions. One region can affect another directly or indirectly, i.e., passing through a third party in the model, and can be affected by several regions. As a result, correlation of two areas could include both direct and indirect effects, and a correlation analysis cannot tease these apart. For this kind of “many-body problem” in neural interactions, structural equation modeling provides a unique analysis framework.

Structural equation modeling (SEM) is a statistical technique that is able to examine causal relationships between multiple variables. SEM approaches the data differently from the usual statistical methods such as multiple regression or ANOVA. The parameters in the SEM model are connection strengths or path coefficients between different variables, which reflect the effective connectivity in our neural network model. Parameters are estimated by minimizing the difference between the observed covariances and those implied by a structural or path model. SEM was initially developed and applied in biology, psychology, economics, and other social sciences (Wright, 1920). In 1994, hypothesizing a connectivity model based on prior knowledge of anatomy and connectivity, McIntosh and Gonzalez-Lima (1994) applied SEM to PET data, demonstrating the dissociation between ventral and dorsal visual pathways in object and spatial vision. SEM was also used to characterize connectivity changes within the motor system of Parkinsonian patients (Grafton et al., 1994). Büchel and Friston (1997) used SEM analysis in an fMRI study to investigate the nonlinear interactions among V1, V5, posterior parietal cortex, and prefrontal cortex. More recently, Maguire et al. (2001) compared the structural equation models between human brains with and without bilateral hippocampal damages during a memory retrieval task.

To date, SEM has been used as a confirmatory analysis technique. Most existing studies only compare a couple of possible connectivity models derived from prior knowledge. But in the case when most connections are not known or the complexity of the anatomic network leads to a large number of possibilities, the traditional network analysis method cannot be applied. In this paper, we adapt SEM for exploratory analysis of fMRI data and demonstrate that such an analysis is capable of identifying the best model from an ensemble of all possible models using comprehensive sorting of multiple fit indices. This is demonstrated in the functional neural network for bimanual motor coordination, using a finger-tapping task involving two hands at two frequency ratios.

Section snippets

fMRI experiment

Seven right-handed subjects participated in this study according to the guidelines set forth by the institutional review board at Emory University. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects. Of these, five were male and two were female with age range from 18 to 31 years, and subjects with life backgrounds that would lead to special motor skills, e.g., extensive training in piano, were excluded. Anatomical scans were acquired using a T1-weighted MPRAGE sequence (TI/TR/TE/Flip angle: 800

Results

In the bimanual task, the left and right primary motor cortices (MI areas) were robustly activated. The BOLD response in the left MI, corresponding to the movement of the right hand that interchanged between the 1/2 frequency ratio condition and 1/1 condition, was stronger at the 4 Hz rate than at the 2 Hz rate, as illustrated in Fig. 6. The BOLD signal in the left MI (LMI) averaged across all subjects in the bimanual tasks is approximately linear with the tapping frequency (green line in Fig. 6

Discussion

In confirmatory studies (Büchel and Friston, 1997, Grafton et al., 1994, Maguire et al., 2001, McIntosh and Gonzalez-Lima, 1994), only a few selected models are usually examined in the structural equation estimations. Our exploratory approach, based on anatomic constraints, compares a large number of possible models and effectively avoids the possibility of missing the most appropriate model in the final result. The comparison between models themselves is a complicated procedure, and multiple

Conclusion

Exploratory SEM analysis was applied to investigate the coupling mechanism between cortical motor areas in bimanual coordination. Given all probable models with connections between left/right MI, left/right PMdc (caudal dorsal premotor area), and SMA (supplementary motor area), the exploratory analysis was able to find a “best” model, which was confirmed to be reproducible across data subsets. The present study demonstrates the capability of SEM for exploring candidate models of effective

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (Grants R01EB002009 and RO1EB000331), the Georgia Research Alliance, the Whitaker Foundation, and National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant 39800049).

References (43)

  • P.E. Roland et al.

    Functions and structures of the motor cortices in humans

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (1996)
  • M. Toyokura et al.

    Relation of bimanual coordination to activation in the sensorimotor cortex and supplementary motor area: analysis using functional magnetic resonance imaging

    Brain Res. Bull.

    (1999)
  • W. Zwicker

    The voters' paradox, spin, and the Borda count

    Math. Soc. Sci.

    (1991)
  • J.T. Baker et al.

    Gaze direction modulates finger movement activation patterns in human cerebral cortex

    J. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • C. Büchel et al.

    Modulation of connectivity in visual pathways by attention: cortical interactions evaluated with structural equation modelling and fMRI

    Cereb. Cortex

    (1997)
  • B.M Byrne

    Structural Equation Modeling and EQS and EQS Windows: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming

    (1994)
  • T. Chan et al.

    Effect of frequency ratio and environmental information on spatial coupling: a study of attention

    Ecol. Psychol.

    (1995)
  • O. Donchin et al.

    Primary motor cortex in involved in bimanual coordination

    Nature

    (1998)
  • R.P. Dum et al.

    The origin of corticospinal projections from the premotor areas in the frontal lobe

    J. Neurosci.

    (1991)
  • C.L. Dym et al.

    Rank ordering engineering designs: pairwise comparison charts and borda counts

    Res. Eng. Design

    (2002)
  • D. Gerbing et al.

    Monte Carlo evaluations of goodness-of-fit indices

  • Cited by (92)

    • Two hands, one brain, and aging

      2017, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text