Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 125, 15 January 2016, Pages 437-445
NeuroImage

Bilingualism at the core of the brain. Structural differences between bilinguals and monolinguals revealed by subcortical shape analysis

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.073Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Subcortical structures are crucial for bilingual language production and perception

  • Evidence suggests that bilingualism shapes brain morphology

  • Little is known about the effect of bilingualism over subcortical structures

  • We used Shape Analysis of basal ganglia and thalamus to address this issue

  • Subcortical expansions were observed in bilinguals compared to monolinguals

Abstract

Naturally acquiring a language shapes the human brain through a long-lasting learning and practice process. This is supported by previous studies showing that managing more than one language from early childhood has an impact on brain structure and function. However, to what extent bilingual individuals present neuroanatomical peculiarities at the subcortical level with respect to monolinguals is yet not well understood, despite the key role of subcortical gray matter for a number of language functions, including monitoring of speech production and language control — two processes especially solicited by bilinguals. Here we addressed this issue by performing a subcortical surface-based analysis in a sample of monolinguals and simultaneous bilinguals (N = 88) that only differed in their language experience from birth. This analysis allowed us to study with great anatomical precision the potential differences in morphology of key subcortical structures, namely, the caudate, accumbens, putamen, globus pallidus and thalamus. Vertexwise analyses revealed significantly expanded subcortical structures for bilinguals compared to monolinguals, localized in bilateral putamen and thalamus, as well as in the left globus pallidus and right caudate nucleus. A topographical interpretation of our results suggests that a more complex phonological system in bilinguals may lead to a greater development of a subcortical brain network involved in monitoring articulatory processes.

Introduction

The human brain displays a considerable degree of structural plasticity. Learning or practicing a given skill can trigger experience-dependent changes in dendrification, vascularization, glial support and axonal myelination and rearrangement that, in some cases, impact macroanatomical brain morphology as measured by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) techniques (Zatorre et al., 2012). A particularly interesting capacity that naturally shapes the human brain is language (Li et al., 2014), one of the most important cognitive attributes of humans. In this context, the study of bilingualism has proved to be a powerful way to understand how this long-lasting language learning process affects brain morphology (Costa and Sebastián-Gallés, 2014, Li et al., 2014).

fMRI studies directly comparing bilinguals and monolinguals show that the former generally make increased activations of cortical areas traditionally related to language processing (mainly because of more demanding word retrieval and articulatory processes; Parker et al., 2012) as well as of brain regions involved in cognitive control (relevant for language switching, error monitoring and language interference inhibition; Abutalebi, 2008, Abutalebi and Green, 2007, Indefrey, 2006). Therefore, the morphology of structures involved in these processes is expected to reflect bilingual experience. Indeed, research on the neuroanatomical differences between bilinguals and monolinguals has observed increased cortical gray matter volume in bilinguals in areas relevant for vocabulary acquisition and low-level auditory processing, among other language functions. Some of those areas are left-lateralized, such as the left inferior parietal cortex (Mechelli et al., 2004, Abutalebi et al., 2015) and left anterior temporal pole (Abutalebi et al., 2014), whereas others show bilateral effects, e.g. inferior frontal gyrus (Klein et al., 2014), Heschl's gyrus (Ressel et al., 2012), anterior cingulate cortex (Abutalebi et al., 2012) and cerebellum (Pliatsikas et al., 2014). Moreover, at the level of the underlying white matter connections, both integrity increases (Luk et al., 2011, Mohades et al., 2012, Pliatsikas et al., 2015) and decreases (Mohades et al., 2012, Gold et al., 2013) have been observed in bilinguals compared to monolinguals in a number of commisural (anterior corpus callosum) and association white matter tracts (e.g. inferior longitudinal and fronto-occipital fasciculus), with mainly bilateral, but also left-lateralized effects being reported. Furthermore, García-Pentón et al. (2014) recently reported that bilinguals display greater structural connectivity and network efficiency in two left-lateralized language-related subnetworks, at the expense of decreased global network efficiency.

Here we focused on the morphological peculiarities that bilinguals might show at the subcortical level, namely, the basal ganglia and thalamus. The basal ganglia have received increased attention regarding their functional role in a wide array of language-related processes, such as speech production (Binder et al., 2005, Bohland and Guenther, 2006, Kuljic-Obradovic, 2003, Riecker et al., 2002, Rosen et al., 2000, Sakurai et al., 1993), rule learning (De Diego-Balaguer et al., 2008), and phonological processing (Tettamanti et al., 2005, Tricomi et al., 2006, Watkins et al., 2002), among others. The basal ganglia are an important component of the procedural memory system, which underlies the extraction and computation of language regularities and rules (e.g., mental grammar) (Ullman, 2004). Similarly, the thalamus is itself a key structure for language function with a well-established role in language production and lexical decision (for a review of fMRI studies, see Llano, 2013), articulation, prosody, semantic processing, and verbal memory (for a review of electrical stimulation studies, see Hebb and Ojemann, 2013). Importantly to our current goals, fMRI studies specifically focused on bilingualism have tended to underscore the relevance of subcortical structures for managing two or more languages. In particular, different studies have reported increased activation of the caudate nucleus in language switching (Abutalebi et al., 2007a, Crinion et al., 2006, Garbin et al., 2011, Wang et al., 2007, Wang et al., 2009). Also, there is evidence of recruitment of the putamen during demanding articulatory and motor control processes, generally observed in bilinguals (Abutalebi et al., 2013, Klein et al., 1994, Tettamanti et al., 2005). In this vein, bilingualism is hypothesized to train a gating system in the striatum that modulates prefrontal cortex activation for language control and application of language rules (Stocco et al., 2012). In spite of the substantial evidence, the literature is almost inexistent regarding the subcortical structural signature of bilingualism, with only two small-sized, whole-brain voxel-based morphometry (VBM) studies reporting subcortical gray matter differences between bilinguals and monolinguals: Zou et al. (2012) showed greater volume in the head of the left caudate nucleus in bimodal bilinguals, who use spoken and sign languages, compared to monolinguals. Also, Abutalebi et al. (2013) showed an increased volume in the left putamen of female multilinguals compared to a group of monolingual participants. These studies suggest that bilingualism may shape the morphology of subcortical brain regions involved in language switching and articulatory processes, respectively. However, their limitations in terms of sample size and other restrictions (e.g. only females, language bimodality), as well as the lack of specificity of VBM approaches to study subcortical morphology, may have well reduced their sensitivity to fully capture the effect of bilingualism on subcortical gray matter.

Recent advances in brain morphometry now allow investigating subcortical morphology in an anatomically meaningful fashion while boosting sensitivity for capturing potential effects. Whereas VBM has long been considered a standard approach in morphometric studies, it is acknowledged that it cannot differentiate between size, shape and/or positional effects (Zatorre et al., 2012). At the cortical level, surface-based morphometry techniques have addressed this issue by allowing the measurement of different aspects of the cortex (e.g., thickness and surface area) while providing a more accurate characterization of cortical anatomy. However, at the subcortical level, it was not until the advent of shape analysis techniques (e.g., Patenaude et al., 2011) that successfully estimating regional shape variations in subcortical structures with high anatomical precision was possible. This approach has been mainly applied to unveil abnormal subcortical morphology in mental disorders or disease (Coscia et al., 2009, Harms et al., 2007, Kang et al., 2008, McKeown et al., 2008, Qiu et al., 2008, Xu et al., 2008) but recently also to understand the role of subcortical shape in high-order cognition in healthy populations (Burgaleta et al., 2014).

In the present study we used the subcortical shape analysis approach to explore, for the first time, whether bilinguals display a distinctive morphology of the subcortical gray matter compared to monolinguals. This approach allowed us to robustly characterize the anatomical boundaries of subcortical structures for each participant and to test for potential morphological differences between groups at the vertex level. We recruited monolinguals (Spanish speakers) and simultaneous bilinguals (Catalan–Spanish speakers) who did not differ in average educational level, socioeconomic background and proficiency in other languages. Spanish and Catalan are two Romance languages that share a great amount of lexical cognates (words that share a common etymological origin and differ in their phonology) and differ mainly at the phonological level. All participants underwent MRI acquisition, and subsequent subcortical segmentation and surface reconstruction were performed for all striatal structures (caudate, accumbens, putamen, and globus pallidus) and thalamus. We then computed the perpendicular vertex displacements with respect to a sample-specific average surface, thus representing relative surface expansions or contractions at the regional level. Because bilinguals are hypothesized to more strongly recruit brain areas involved in articulatory and language switching processes, we expected to find a plasticity effect (expansion) primarily on the putamen (articulation) and the caudate nucleus (language switching) in bilinguals compared to monolinguals, as well as in the thalamus, given its key role in language functions. Nucleus accumbens was included under the assumption that, given its reduced volume as well as the low spatial resolution typically found in fMRI studies, its potential relevance for language might have been obscured by the structures in its vicinity. In addition to these analyses, and for the sake of completeness, we also applied voxel-based morphometry to address potential differences in cortical gray matter between bilinguals and monolinguals. Based on our previous work on Catalan–Spanish bilinguals, we mainly expected significant differences favoring bilinguals in the auditory cortex (Heschl's gyrus, Ressel et al., 2012).

Section snippets

Participants

88 right-handed participants took part in the study. All participants were undergraduate students at the University Jaume I of Castellón de la Plana (Spain) so they had the same educational level. None of them reported known auditory or neurological deficits. Forty-six participants were monolingual Spanish speakers (26 females; mean age = 21.85 years, SD = 4.13) and 42 were simultaneous Catalan–Spanish bilinguals (22 females; mean age = 21.64 years, SD = 2.17). The study followed the ethical protocol of

Subcortical shape analysis

At the subcortical level, we observed that bilinguals displayed bilateral expansion of putamen and thalamus with respect to monolinguals, as well as of the left globus pallidus and right caudate (P < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons) (Fig. 1). No other significant effects were observed for any other structure. Similarly, no significant expansions were observed for monolinguals with respect to bilinguals. Fig. 2 displays density plots for the surface displacements in bilinguals and

Discussion

In the present study we addressed the question of whether monolinguals and bilinguals differ in the morphology of those subcortical structures typically involved in language processing. We investigated this issue by applying subcortical shape analysis for the first time in a large sample of monolinguals and simultaneous bilinguals, an approach that allowed us to unveil fine-grained regional differences between these two groups in the morphology of the basal ganglia and thalamus. Interestingly,

Conclusion

How does lifelong experience in two languages affect subcortical morphology? Here we show that early bilinguals display localized expansions in the putamen, globus pallidus, caudate and thalamus. This pattern of results suggests that subcortical morphology may be affected by language use. Furthermore, our findings also highlight the role of subcortical structures in language and suggest that phonological and articulatory enrichment entails structural changes in striatal and thalamic structures

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (JCI-2009-04492, PSI2010-20168), the Spanish Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (MINECO-PSI2012-34071), the European Community's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) / ERC grant agreement n°323961, and Grup de Recerca en Neurociència Cognitiva (GRNC), AGAUR (2014 SGR 1210 N). M.B. was funded by the Spanish Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (MINECO-FPDI-2013-17528). N.S.G. received the prize

References (104)

  • B. Crosson

    Subcortical functions in language: A working model

    Brain Lang.

    (1985)
  • G. Garbin et al.

    Bridging language and attention: brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control

    NeuroImage

    (2010)
  • G. Garbin et al.

    Neural bases of language switching in high and early proficient bilinguals

    Brain Lang.

    (2011)
  • L. García-Pentón et al.

    Anatomical connectivity changes in the bilingual brain

    NeuroImage

    (2014)
  • B.T. Gold et al.

    Lifelong bilingualism contributes to cognitive reserve against white matter integrity declines in aging

    Neuropsychologia

    (2013)
  • A.O. Hebb et al.

    The thalamus and language revisited

    Brain Lang.

    (2013)
  • A. Hervais-Adelman et al.

    Brain functional plasticity associated with the emergence of expertise in extreme language control

    NeuroImage

    (2015)
  • P. Indefrey et al.

    The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components

    Cognition

    (2004)
  • D. Klein et al.

    Age of language learning shapes brain structure: a cortical thickness study of bilingual and monolingual individuals

    Brain Lang.

    (2014)
  • J.C. Lee et al.

    Abnormal subcortical components of the corticostriatal system in young adults with DLI: a combined structural MRI and DTI study

    Neuropsychologia

    (2013)
  • P. Li et al.

    Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: anatomical changes in the human brain

    Cortex

    (2014)
  • D.A. Llano

    Functional imaging of the thalamus in language

    Brain Lang.

    (2013)
  • J.W. Mink

    The basal ganglia: focused selection and inhibition of competing motor programs

    Prog. Neurobiol.

    (1996)
  • S.G. Mohades et al.

    DTI reveals structural differences in white matter tracts between bilingual and monolingual children

    Brain Res.

    (2012)
  • M. Ors et al.

    Spect findings in children with specific language impairment

    Cortex

    (2005)
  • B. Patenaude et al.

    A Bayesian model of shape and appearance for subcortical brain segmentation

    NeuroImage

    (2011)
  • A. Qiu et al.

    Parallel transport in diffeomorphisms distinguishes the time-dependent pattern of hippocampal surface deformation due to healthy aging and the dementia of the Alzheimer's type

    NeuroImage

    (2008)
  • A. Riecker et al.

    Hemispheric lateralization effects of rhythm implementation during syllable repetitions: an fMRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • H.J. Rosen et al.

    Comparison of brain activation during word retrieval done silently and aloud using fMRI

    Brain Cogn.

    (2000)
  • J.E. Scorzin et al.

    Volume determination of amygdala and hippocampus at 1.5 and 3.0T MRI in temporal lobe epilepsy

    Epilepsy Res.

    (2008)
  • S.M. Smith et al.

    Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference

    NeuroImage

    (2009)
  • M.T. Ullman

    Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model

    Cognition

    (2004)
  • Y. Wang et al.

    Neural bases of asymmetric language switching in second-language learners: an ER-fMRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2007)
  • Y. Wang et al.

    Sustained and transient language control in the bilingual brain

    NeuroImage

    (2009)
  • R.J. Wise et al.

    Brain regions involved in articulation

    Lancet

    (1999)
  • J. Abutalebi et al.

    Do subcortical structures control ‘language selection’ in polyglots? Evidence from pathological language mixing

    Neurocase

    (2000)
  • J. Abutalebi et al.

    The neural cost of the auditory perception of language switches: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study in bilinguals

    J. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • J. Abutalebi et al.

    The neural cost of the auditory perception of language switches: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study in bilinguals

    J. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • J. Abutalebi et al.

    Language control and lexical competition in bilinguals: an event-related FMRI study

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2008)
  • J. Abutalebi et al.

    Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2012)
  • E. Bialystok et al.

    Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind

    Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci.

    (2010)
  • T. Bitan et al.

    Shifts of effective connectivity within a language network during rhyming and spelling

    J. Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • M. Burgaleta et al.

    Subcortical regional morphology correlates with fluid and spatial intelligence

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (2014)
  • A.H. Chan et al.

    Neural correlates of nouns and verbs in early bilinguals

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2008)
  • D.M. Coscia et al.

    Volumetric and shape analysis of the thalamus in first-episode schizophrenia

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (2009)
  • A. Costa et al.

    How does the bilingual experience sculpt the brain?

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2014)
  • A. Costa et al.

    The cognate facilitation effect: implications for the models of lexical access

    J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.

    (2000)
  • A. Costa et al.

    How do highly proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional

    J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.

    (2006)
  • J. Crinion et al.

    Language control in the bilingual brain

    Science

    (2006)
  • B. Crosson

    Subcortical Functions In Language And Memory

    (1992)
  • Cited by (95)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text