Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 51, Issue 7, June 2013, Pages 1377-1381
Neuropsychologia

The genetic impact (C957T-DRD2) on inhibitory control is magnified by aging

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.01.014Get rights and content

Abstract

Healthy aging beyond the age of 65 is characterized by a general decrease in cognitive control over actions: old adults have more difficulty than young adults in stopping overt responses. Responsible for this cognitive decrement is the continuous decline of striatal and extrastriatal dopamine (DA). The resource-modulation hypothesis assumes that genetic variability is more likely to result in performance differences when brain resources move away from close-to-optimal levels, as in aging. To test this hypothesis we investigated, first, whether individual differences in the C957T polymorphism at DRD2 gene (rs6277) contribute to individual differences in the proficiency to inhibit behavioral responses in a stop-signal task. Second, we assessed whether this genetic effect is magnified in older adults, due to the considerable decline in dopamine function. Our findings show that individuals carrying genotype associated with higher density of extrastriatal D2 receptors (C957T CC) were more efficient in inhibiting unwanted action tendencies, but not in term of response execution. This effect was stronger in older than in younger adults. Our findings support the idea that aging-related decline in dopamine availability alters the balance between genotypes and cognitive functions.

Highlights

► Old adults have difficulty in stopping overt responses. ► In aging takes place continuous decline of striatal and extrastriatal DA. ► Aging alters the balance between genotypes and cognitive functions.

Introduction

Healthy aging beyond the age of 65 is characterized by a general decrease in cognitive control: older adults find it increasingly difficult to apply new or coordinate multiple rules, and to discriminate relevant from irrelevant information. Working memory, reasoning (Ball, Vance, Edwards, & Wadley, 2004 (chap. 36); Brehmer, Li, Müller, von Oertzen, & Lindenberger, 2007; Li, Schmiedek, Huxhold, Röcke, Smith, & Lindenberger, 2008; Shing, Werkle-Bergner, Brehmer, Muller, Li, & Lindenberger, 2010), attention abilities and speed of processing (Faust and Balota, 1997, Ball et al., 1998, Colzato et al., 2011) show large decrements with increasing age. Cognitive inhibition also declines throughout the life span (Williams, Ponesse, Schachar, Logan, & Tannock, 1999): old adults have more difficulty than young adults in stopping an overt response and changing to new rules in a categorization task (Kramer, Humphrey, Larish, Logan, & Strayer, 1994). Responsible for this cognitive decline is the general loss of neurochemicals, such as the continuous decline of striatal and extrastriatal dopamine (DA) systems from early to late adulthood and old age (Bäckman et al., 2000, Bäckman et al., 2006, Erixon-Lindroth et al., 2005, Volkow et al., 1998).

Interestingly, individual differences in cognitive performance increase from early to late adulthood reflecting genetic differences (Li et al., 2004, Li et al., 2010, Störmer et al., 2012). The resource-modulation hypothesis (Lindenberger et al., 2008) assumes that aging-related losses in neurochemical and structural brain resources modulate the extent to which common genetic variations affect cognitive functioning. In particular, the function relating brain resources to cognitive performance is assumed to be nonlinear, so that genetic variability is more likely to result in performance differences when resources move away from close-to-optimal levels, as in aging. In other words, the genetic setup of an individual matters more the older he or she gets.

The resource-modulation hypothesis has been supported by a number of studies. First, spatial working memory and executive functions in elderly were associated with individual differences in genetic predispositions of the gene that codes for the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), an enzyme that degrades DA in prefrontal cortex, while no such relation was observed in younger adults (Nagel et al., 2008; Störmer et al., 2012). Second, a similar age magnification effect was observed between brain-derived neurotropic factor (BDNF) genotype and episodic memory performance under high associative demands (Li et al., 2010). The third evidence comes from the genetic modulation of training and transfer in older adults. Colzato, Slagter, de Rover, and Hommel (2011) trained participants genotyped for the brain-derived-neurotrophic factor (BDNF) Val66Met polymorphism on cognitive tasks developed to improve dynamic attention. Pre-training (baseline) and post-training measures of attentional processes (divided and selective attention) were acquired. As expected, Val/Val homozygous individuals, associated with increased activity-dependent secretion of BDNF (Egan et al., 2003), showed larger beneficial transfer effects than Met/−carriers.

The current study focused, for the first time, on the inhibition of behavioral responses—another key cognitive control function (Logan and Cowan, 1984, Logan, 1994) that is known to decrease with old age (Kramer et al., 1994, Williams et al., 1999). Recently, Colzato, Waszak, Nieuwenhuis, Posthuma, and Hommel (2010) reported response inhibition (assessed in by means of a stop-signal task) to be predicted by the C957T polymorphism at the DRD2 gene, but not by COMT Val158Met (a polymorphism related to frontal DA). Healthy young adults pressed a left or right button as soon as a green left- or right-pointing arrow appeared (go trials). However, if the color of the arrow suddenly changed to red, the participants were supposed to refrain from responding (stop trials). This stop-signal task measures both the efficiency of response execution (by means of reaction times to go-signals) and the efficiency in inhibitory control (by means of the stop-signal reaction time or SSRT, where longer SSRT reflect general slowing of inhibitory processes and indicate a lower level of inhibitory efficiency). C/C homozygotes, associated with increased extrastriatal D2 receptor availability (Hirvonen et al., 2009a) and higher striatal DA levels (Hirvonen et al., 2009b), stopped faster on stop trials than T/−carriers. No association between COMT Val158Met polymorphism and stopping latency was found. This pattern of results was in line with a previous study by Forbes, Brown, Kimak, Ferrell, Manuck, and Hariri (2007), who reported no association between self-reported impulsivity scores (index highly correlated with the inhibitory control) with the COMT gene (Val158Met polymorphism).

A number of patient studies have provided converging evidence for the involvement of striatal DA in response inhibition. Parkinson's patients, who suffer from loss of dopaminergic neurons in the basal ganglia, showed difficulties in stopping (Gauggel, Rieger, & Feghoff, 2004) and impaired suppression of conflicting responses (Wylie et al., 2009) compared to matched healthy controls. Consistent with this picture, ADHD patients (see, Alderson, Rapport, & Kofler, 2007, for a recent review) and recreational users of cocaine (Colzato, van den Wildenberg, & Hommel, 2007), who are likely to suffer from reduced dopamine D2 receptors in the striatum (Volkow, Fowler, & Wang, 1999), need significantly more time to inhibit responses to stop-signals than non-users. Moreover, spontaneous eyeblink rate, a marker of striatal dopaminergic functioning, reliably predicts the efficiency in inhibiting unwanted action tendencies in a stop-signal task (Colzato, van den Wildenberg, van Wouwe, Pannebakker, & Hommel, 2009).

Given the role of striatal DA in modulating response inhibition, we expected DRD2 C957T C/C homozygotes to be better than T/−carriers in stopping control, replicating Colzato, Waszak, et al. (2010). Moreover, based on the resource-modulation hypothesis (Lindenberger et al., 2008), we expected this effect to be magnified in old age.

Section snippets

Participants

170 healthy Caucasian adults from two age groups (younger [mean age of 21.5 years] and older adults [mean age of 69.15 years]) served as participants, see Table 1. The sample was drawn from adults from the Leiden and Rotterdam metropolitan area (The Netherlands), who volunteered to participate in studies of behavioral genetics. The advertisement was posted on the internet. To take part in the study, potential participants had to subscribe per e-mail. Given that this subscription criterion

Participants

Sample information and genotype-specific demographics are shown in Table 1. All resulting genotype frequencies from our cohort of participants did not deviate from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (p>0.1). No significant differences were found among genotype frequencies with respect to age, IQ or sex.

SSRTs for stop-signal trials

First, replicating previous work (Colzato, van den Wildenberg, van der Does, & Hommel, 2010), SSRTs yielded a significant effect of genotype for C957T polymorphism at DRD2, F(3,166)=17.08, p<0.0001,

Conclusions

We investigated the relationship between DRD2 genetic variability and response inhibition. Replicating an earlier study (Colzato, van den Widenberg, et al., 2010), DRD2 C957T C/C homozygotes, associated with higher density of extrastriatal D2 receptors, were more efficient in inhibiting unwanted action tendencies than T/−carriers were. In line with the resource-modulation hypothesis (Lindenberger et al., 2008), the observed genetic effect was larger in older adults, who are likely to suffer

Acknowledgments

We thank our bachelor students for their enthusiasm and invaluable assistance in recruiting, testing the participants of this study and collecting the data.

References (46)

  • M.M. Hirvonen et al.

    C957T polymorphism of the human dopamine D2 receptor gene predicts extrastriatal dopamine receptor availability

    Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology

    (2009)
  • S.-C. Li et al.

    Aging magnifies the effects of dopamine transporter and D2 receptor genes on backward serial memory

    Neurobiology of Aging

    (2013)
  • A. Miyake et al.

    The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis

    Cognitive Psychology

    (2000)
  • Y.L. Shing et al.

    Episodic memory across the lifespan: The contributions of associative and strategic components

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2010)
  • S.A. Wylie et al.

    The effect of Parkinson's disease on interference control during action selection

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • R.M. Alderson et al.

    Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and behavioral inhibition: a meta-analytic review of the stop-signal paradigm

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2007)
  • L. Bäckman et al.

    Age-related cognitive deficits mediated by changes in the striatal dopamine system

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • Ball, K., Vance, D. E., Edwards, J. D., & Wadley, V. G. (2004). Aging and the brain. In Principles and practice of...
  • K.K. Ball et al.

    Age and visual search: expanding the useful field of view

    Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and Image Science

    (1998)
  • Y. Brehmer et al.

    Memory plasticity across the lifespan: Uncovering children's latent potential

    Developmental Psychology

    (2007)
  • L.S. Colzato et al.

    Dopamine and the management of attentional resources: Genetic markers of striatal D2 dopamine predict individual differences in the attentional blink

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2011)
  • L.S. Colzato et al.

    Impaired inhibitory control in recreational cocaine users

    PLoS ONE

    (2007)
  • L.S. Colzato et al.

    Dopamine and inhibitory action control: Evidence from spontaneous eye blink rates

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2009)
  • Cited by (51)

    • Interactive effects of DRD2 rs6277 polymorphism, environment and sex on impulsivity in a population-representative study

      2021, Behavioural Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The mean age of the participants used in White and colleagues’ study (2009) study was 19.3 years (SD = 1.89), rather similarly to the current study population. On the other hand, our previous study population had the mean age of 35.2 years (SD = 11.32; [45]) and it has been suggested that the effect of DRD2 rs6277 on response inhibition may be magnified by ageing [35]. A general decline in the dopaminergic functioning takes place from early to late adulthood, with gradual reductions seen in the number of dopaminergic cell bodies, synapses, as well as brain dopamine levels [74].

    • The relationship of age and DRD2 polymorphisms to frontostriatal brain activity and working memory performance

      2019, Neurobiology of Aging
      Citation Excerpt :

      The Taq1a A allele of the ANKK1 gene has been linked to reduced striatal D2 receptor binding (Eisenstein et al., 2016; Jönsson et al., 1999; Pohjalainen et al., 1998; Thompson et al., 1997). Several studies have investigated the effects of C957T on WM and executive function (Bolton et al., 2010; Byrne et al., 2016; Colzato et al., 2013; Huertas et al., 2012; Klaus et al., 2017; Li et al., 2013; Rodriguez-Jimenez et al., 2007), but results regarding the direction of the effects are mixed. For example, it has been demonstrated that C carriers have worse general cognitive ability compared with T carriers (Bolton et al., 2010) and that they score lower on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Rodriguez-Jimenez et al., 2007).

    • The effect of ANKK1 Taq1A and DRD2 C957T polymorphisms on executive function: A systematic review and meta-analysis

      2019, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, clear conclusions cannot be drawn, as these results were only based on two samples and were largely driven by Stelzel et al.’ (2009) findings. Furthermore, quality assessment showed that 12 studies failed to control (or did not report controlling) for several of the potential confounders, including age, education level, ethnicity, and history of mental illness, all of which may affect outcomes on the cognitive tests either directly or via interaction with genotype (Brockmole and Logie, 2013; Byrd et al., 2004; Colzato et al., 2013; Huguelet et al., 2000; Nkam et al., 2017; Villalba et al., 2015; Wight et al., 2006). The current meta-analyses might also have been biased because some studies that reported significant association between the Taq1A or C957T genotype and the performance on executive function tasks could not be included in the analyses.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text