The house money effect on investment risk taking: Evidence from Taiwan

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pacfin.2012.08.005Get rights and content

Abstract

This paper investigates the effect of house money on the risk taking behavior of individual investors. When gains are more substantial, individuals tend to take greater risk. The house money effect seems to decline over time because the propensity for risk taking following gains is diminished with time. This study shows that when evaluating investment gains, the reference points for investors are adapted over time, with the current salient reference point being the highest stock price attained at a given time in the past. The empirical evidence suggests that the house money effect is actually discernible in the real world financial markets and not just in artificial laboratory experiments.

Highlights

► The effect of house money on the risk-taking behavior is investigated. ► The relationship between prior gains and the change in risk taking is examined. ► When gains are more substantial, individuals tend to take greater risk. ► The propensity for risk taking following gains diminishes with time. ► The salient reference point for gains is the highest price attained previously.

Introduction

Psychological studies have revealed that prior outcome affects feeling. However, does it affect decision-making strategy and risk taking in financial markets? The modern rationality-based economic theory suggests that prior outcomes do not affect investors' decisions. Investors should decide to invest only when future benefits are greater than future costs. Losses or costs that one may have experienced in the past but which are not expected to recur in the future are sunk costs and should not be part of the decision calculations. Therefore, the only question at issue is whether the anticipated return is in accord with the risk one takes. Prior outcomes of financial investments are sunk and irrelevant and should not influence one's risk taking behavior.

A competing view is that prior outcomes do affect decision-making. According to the psychological literature on the sunk costs effect, prior outcomes directly affect an individual when making real decisions. Indeed, numerous studies have established that sunk costs often influence decision makers.1 Based on the findings on sunk costs, the argument is that people will always escalate their commitment to their chosen course of action, as opposed to cutting their losses and ending their questionable line of behavior. It therefore appears that individuals will tend to adhere to their losing ways and throw away good money in their attempts to rescue themselves.

The house money effect, which Thaler and Johnson (1990) first propose and document based on experimental evidence, refers to a pattern whereby people tend to take on increased risk subsequent to a successful investment experience. That is, prior gains lead to greater risk taking in subsequent periods. Gamblers refer to this feeling as “playing with the house's money.” After a large win, gamblers do not yet regard the new money as their own, and because they do not fully integrate their winnings with their own money, they act as if they are betting with the casino's money. People are more willing to take risks after a windfall, even when they do not conventionally lean toward risk taking. This phenomenon is observed essentially because after a gain, the feeling of satisfaction can cancel out the pain of a subsequent loss. It therefore appears that people become less risk-averse and have a greater propensity for risk taking behavior.

The experimental evidence on how prior gains and losses influence risk taking behavior is rather inconclusive, with a portion of the evidence supporting the house money effect2 and several studies demonstrating otherwise (Clark, 2002). Barberis et al. (2001) develop a theoretical asset-pricing model that considers the house money effect. The settings on their model focus on the experimental evidence relating to the dynamic features of risk aversion in their attempt to exhibit and explain both the equity-premium and volatility puzzles.

Despite strong psychological foundations and extensive experimental evidence, little empirical evidence is currently available on the house money effect. Several recent empirical studies focus on intraday trading in the futures market. Frino et al. (2008) find evidence of the house money effect in the behavior of professional futures traders. Furthermore, Liu et al. (2010) examine the behavior of market-makers in the futures market and discover that morning gains lead to above average risk taking in afternoon trading. However, Locke and Mann (2003) find little change in risk taking after abnormal morning gains. Moreover, Coval, and Shumway (2005) argue that morning gains actually lead to decreased risk taking in the afternoon, while traders with morning losses are more likely to take above average afternoon risk.

We add to previous studies by analyzing the trading behavior of individual investors in the stock market and examining the variations in the house money effect over time and across gain sizes, as well as the impact on the house money effect from countervailing factors, such as the setting of reference points. The results reveal compelling evidence of the house money effect. Individual investors tend to increase their risk taking based on prior investment gains. Specifically, individual investors tend to buy stocks with higher volatility once they have experienced a prior gain.

To examine whether such behavior holds when investors experience relatively small prior gains or whether the gain is sufficiently large to be perceived as playing with the house's money, we analyze the house money effect based on gain size. Consistent with the results of Arkes and Blumer (1985) and Arkes et al. (1994), we find that the house money effect is associated with the size of the prior gain and that this gain must be substantial enough to be perceived as playing with the house's money.

Moreover, Arkes and Blumer (1985) and Gourville and Soman (1998) argue that the sunk costs effect may weaken over time. Conversely, Thaler (1980) and Kahneman et al. (1990) state that although a newly obtained asset may initially be viewed as a gain, it will gradually be incorporated into a person's wealth and became a part of the status quo. This paper argues that the same type of adjustment occurs in the case of investors selling stocks with gains.

We hypothesize that investors obtain considerable pleasure from substantial investment gains in the stock market and that they gradually adapt to such gains over time, which ultimately weakens the house money effect. Consistent with this hypothesis, we find that the house money effect attenuates with time, resulting in a greater willingness of investors to ignore a prior gain and far less willingness to increase risk taking in a later investment decision-making process.

Thaler and Johnson (1990) demonstrate that individuals have increasing risk tolerance when their wealth exceeds a specific reference point. This study examines the house money effect in light of this important issue. Kahnemn and Tversky's (1979) prospect theory depicts the shape of the value function around a certain reference point, but they do not specify its location.

Recent studies suggest that people tend to consider the average purchase price and the prior maximum price as their reference points in financial investments.3 This paper evaluates the role of different reference points associated with the house money effect. Our results reveal strong evidence of the house money effect irrespective of the reference point measures used. However, the house money effect associated with maximum stock price is more significant than that associated with other reference points. It could be that the feeling of happiness arising from any profit achieved over the prior high price is strong, and the effect of such feelings on risk taking behavior is also strong.

The organization of this paper is as follows: Section 2 discusses the data and methodology. Section 3 presents our empirical results. Section 4 summarizes the conclusions drawn from our study.

Section snippets

Data

The data for this study comprise all intraday transactions on the Taiwan Stock Exchange (TSE) from January 1, 1995 to December 31, 1999. For each transaction, we can identify the stock code, the trade price, the trade date and time (to one-hundredth of a second), the number of shares traded, and the identity of every buyer and seller involved in the transactions. To protect the privacy of traders, the TSE has replaced the true names of the traders with distinct codes that have allowed us to

The house money effect

The house money effect manifests itself in the propensity to take greater risk once gains or profits have been experienced. Psychological explanations for such behavior are predicated on the belief shared by investors that they are playing with the house's money, not their own money.

Our analysis begins with the regression of changes in risk taking on the investment gains in the previous sale period. The trading profit is calculated through the FIFO method with adjustments for dividends. Table 4

Conclusions

Despite the strong psychological foundations and the extensive experimental evidence of the house money effect, there have been few empirical studies undertaken on this issue because of data limitations. The database used in the present study is a comprehensive set of records on stock market trading, which allows us to directly examine the relationship between prior investment gains and the change in risk taking among individual investors.

This study provides clear support for the theory and

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our sincere gratitude to the Taiwan Stock Exchange Corporation for providing valuable data and Meir Statman for his comments.

References (41)

  • A. Tversky et al.

    Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1973)
  • M. Weber et al.

    The disposition effect in securities trading: an experimental analysis

    Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization

    (1998)
  • L.F. Ackert et al.

    An experimental examination of the house money effect in a multi-period setting

    Experimental Economics

    (2006)
  • J.R. Anderson

    Meaning-based knowledge representations, cognitive psychology and its implication

    (1990)
  • B.M. Barber et al.

    Trading is hazardous to your wealth: the common stock investment performance of individual investors

    Journal of Finance

    (2000)
  • B.M. Barber et al.

    Just how much do individual investors lose by trading?

    Review of Financial Studies

    (2008)
  • N. Barberis et al.

    Prospect theory and asset prices

    Quarterly Journal of Economics

    (2001)
  • R.C. Battalio et al.

    Testing between alternative models of choice under uncertainty: some initial results

    Journal of Risk and Uncertainty

    (1990)
  • J. Clark

    House money effects in public good experiments

    Experimental Economics

    (2002)
  • J.D. Coval et al.

    Do behavioral biases affect prices?

    Journal of Finance

    (2005)
  • Cited by (24)

    • Idiosyncratic skewness and cross-section of stock returns: Evidence from Taiwan

      2021, International Review of Financial Analysis
      Citation Excerpt :

      This result supports the house money effect that investors' demand for lottery-like stocks are stronger in the gain domain. It also corroborates Hsu and Chow (2013) that individuals in the Taiwan stock market take greater risks when their gains are substantial. In the real world, due to arbitrage limits, rational investors' trading cannot immediately correct the mispricing.

    • Revisiting the house money effect in the field: Evidence from casino jackpots

      2019, Economics Letters
      Citation Excerpt :

      The tendency of prior gains to induce risk-seeking behavior is generally referred to as the house money effect (Thaler and Johnson, 1990). This effect has been documented in several laboratory experiments (e.g., Keasey and Moon, 1996; Ackert et al., 2006; Weber and Zuchel, 2005; Houser and Xiao, 2015) and field studies (e.g., Frino et al., 2008; Hsu and Chow, 2013; Suhonen and Saastamoinen, 2017). However, although the house money effect is usually illustrated and motivated in terms of gambling at a casino (e.g., Thaler and Johnson, 1990; Arkes et al., 1994) and etymologically “house” refers to a casino, no study has investigated casino players’ behavior after hitting a jackpot.

    • Examining return predictability of industry style portfolios with prior return relative to a benchmark

      2017, Quarterly Review of Economics and Finance
      Citation Excerpt :

      Ackert, Charupat, Church, and Deaves (2006), Frino, Grant, and Johnstone (2008), Liu, Tsai, Wang, and Zhu (2010) present evidence of house money effect in the risk taking behavior of investors as well as traders. Froot, Arabadjis, Lawrence, and Cates (2011) and Hsu and Chow (2013) also support the presence of house money effect.1 In this paper, we examine whether returns on industry based portfolios can be predicted, where the predicting variable is the differential return between a particular portfolio and a wide market benchmark.

    • How rational is gambling?

      2023, Journal of Economic Surveys
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text