Elsevier

Physiology & Behavior

Volume 97, Issue 1, 20 April 2009, Pages 44-51
Physiology & Behavior

Variety enhances food intake in humans: Role of sensory-specific satiety

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.01.019Get rights and content

Abstract

Twenty-one subjects were studied to evaluate the effect of renewal of sensory stimulations of previously eaten foods on sensory-specific satiety and intake. The subjects ate French fries then brownie cakes ad libitum in three situations: “monotonous” — fries then brownies were consumed alone; “simultaneous” — condiments (ketchup and mayonnaise for the fries, vanilla cream and whipped cream for the brownies) were added during intakes; “successive” — after intake of fries alone, ketchup then mayonnaise were available with fries and, after intake of brownies alone, vanilla cream then whipped cream were offered with brownies. The quantities eaten in the “simultaneous” and “successive” situations were higher (p < 0.001) than those in the “monotonous” one (1485 ± 582 and 1682 ± 777 kcal vs 1195 ± 552 kcal, respectively). In the “successive” situation, hedonic ratings for fries diminished during intake but increased after the introduction of ketchup, leading to additional intake of fries. Similarly, hedonic ratings for brownies diminished during intake and increased after the introduction of vanilla cream leading to additional brownie intake (mayonnaise and whipped cream had no significant effect). Food variety, obtained by adding condiments can increase food intake in the short term. The mechanism by which food consumption is increased after the addition of condiments is introduced is at least partly related to the attenuation of sensory-satiety for a given food.

Introduction

Obesity is increasing world-wide [1], especially in western nations [2]. The reasons leading to the imbalance between energy intake and energy expenditure are multifactorial [3]. Among the different factors, food variety could play a role [4], [5], [6], [7]. Actually, several animal studies have shown that variety increases food intake and leads to body-weight gain [8], [9]. It has also been observed in normal-weight humans that food variety may stimulate food intake in the short term [10], [11], [12], [13], [14].

It is not clear how variety stimulates food-intake. From one point of view, variety may increase intake by an appetite-stimulating mechanism. This could be due to renewed palatability (i.e. the ‘liking’ or pleasantness of food) in relation to the diversity of the eaten foods [15], [16], by sustaining interest in eating [17], by another stimulating mechanism involving metabolic and hormonal responses, or environmental, psychological and social variables [6], [18]. From another point of view, food variety may increase intake by delaying satiation (i.e. meal termination). This could be related to a decrease in sensory-specific satiety [19] or by a dishabituation mechanism [5] induced by food variety.

Sensory-specific satiety (SSS) is a satiation mechanism defined as the decline in pleasantness of the flavour of a specific food that has just been eaten in contrast to other non-consumed foods [20], [21]. The decrease in pleasantness linked to SSS is observed very rapidly, 2 min after the end of ad libitum intake, before food enters the intestinal tract and before intestinal absorption [22], [23]. The implication of SSS on elevated food intake associated with varied meals has been known for a long time. Actually, as sensory-specific satiety is specific for eaten foods, when a great variety of foods is available during a meal, it may take longer for satiation to occur for all foods, which leads to greater intake [12], [24], [25].

Although the link between high consumption of varied meals and sensory-specific satiety is well established, several researchers stressed that this link needs to be strengthened. McCrory et al. [68] wrote, ‘although the mechanism by which a drive for dietary variety gets translated into increased food consumption is not known, the phenomenon of sensory-specific satiety is a probable candidate’. Similarly, Raynor and Epstein [5] wrote that ‘sensory-specific satiety may be the process through which a greater variety of food is associated with increased intake’ and, Norton et al. [31] argued that ‘a potential mechanism underlying the variety effect may be due to the increased stimulation associated with multiple foods delaying the development of sensory-specific satiety’. Furthermore, to our knowledge, neither the temporal evolution of hedonic sensations for a variety of foods offered successively and simultaneously (i.e. change in SSS) [12], nor the effect of introducing of a new flavour in a previously eaten food have been reported in fundamental experiments.

The present experiment was conducted to bring new arguments to the relationship between SSS and the high food-consumption during intake of a varied meal as well as to study how small amounts of variability may disrupt SSS. More precisely, the study set out to determine whether modification of the sensory properties of a food eaten ad libitum by adding condiments could decrease sensory satiety for that food and subsequently lead to additional intake of that food. A within-subject design was created to rate relative preference-changes of an ad libitum two-course meal in either a monotonous form or with simultaneous or successive variety via the addition of condiments. The meal was of the ‘fast-food’ type since it has been suggested that this type of meal could play a role in the prevalence of obesity [26].

Section snippets

Subjects

The participants were 21 male volunteers (age: 22 ± 3 yr, range 18–28; BMI: 22.4 ± 0.9 kg m 2, range 21.0–24.2), who gave written consent to participate in the experiment. All participants were students at the University of Dijon (Burgundy). Female subjects were not retained because of hormonal variations during their menstrual cycle and for their potential higher cognitive control of eating [27]. The ethical committee of the University Hospital of Besançon (France) approved the protocol.

Inclusion

Food intake and Flavour–Pleasure ratings

Ingested quantities and durations of intake differed according to the sessions as shown in Table 1. Actually, the mean total ingested food weight was 40% and 35% higher (F(2,40) = 12.75, p < 0.001) in the successive and simultaneous sessions, respectively, than in the monotonous one. In the successive session, mean intake of fries eaten alone was 270 ± 128 g, fries eaten with tomato ketchup 27 ± 27 g, and fries eaten with mayonnaise 22 ± 26 g (i.e. 740 ± 341, 74 ± 74, and 60 ± 71 kcal, respectively); mean

Discussion

The present study confirms the stimulating effects of food variety on food intake in normal-weight adults in the short term [10], [11], [12], [13], [14]. In both the simultaneous and successive sessions, in which condiments were offered, food intake at lunch increased in comparison with the “monotonous” session. This increase in food consumption was similar (25% to 40%) to that usually reported in the literature on food variety in humans [6], [17], [24]. Above all, in the “successive” session,

Conclusion

The present experiment shows in humans that food variety, obtained by adding condiments to foods like in a ‘fast food’-style meal, can increase food intake in the short term. This increase in food consumption may stem from disruption of sensory satiety for a given food by renewal of sensory stimulation. This disruption of sensory satiety by food variety could play a role in the obesity epidemic in western countries. This subject warrants further investigation in long-term studies.

Acknowledgment

Special thanks to the English department of the Faculty of Medicine of Dijon for revision of the English.

References (73)

  • B.J. Rolls et al.

    Sensory specific satiety in man

    Physiol Behav

    (1981)
  • M.M. Hetherington et al.

    The time course of sensory-specific satiety

    Appetite

    (1989)
  • E.T. Rolls et al.

    Olfactory sensory-specific satiety in humans

    Physiol Behav

    (1997)
  • B.J. Rolls et al.

    Pleasantness changes and food intake in a varied four-course meal

    Appetite

    (1984)
  • C.E. Cornell et al.

    Stimulus-induced eating when satiated

    Physiol Behav

    (1989)
  • M.A. McCrory et al.

    Dietary variety within food groups: association with energy intake and body fatness in men and women

    Am J Clin Nutr

    (1999)
  • G.N. Norton et al.

    Volume and variety: relative effects on food intake

    Physiol Behav

    (2006)
  • M.A. Pereira et al.

    Fast-food habits, weight gain, and insulin resistance (the CARDIA study): 15-year prospective analysis

    Lancet

    (2005)
  • A.J. Stunkard et al.

    The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger

    J Psychosom Res

    (1985)
  • J.E. Blundell et al.

    Sensitivity of stimulus-induced-salivation (SIS), hunger ratings and alliesthesia to a glucose load: SIS as a measure of specific satiation

    Appetite

    (1981)
  • B.J. Rolls et al.

    Effect of the volume of liquid food infused intragastrically on satiety in women

    Physiol Behav

    (2002)
  • A.A. Devitt et al.

    Effects of food unit size and energy density on intake in humans

    Appetite

    (2004)
  • T.V. Kral et al.

    Energy density and portion size: their independent and combined effects on energy intake

    Physiol Behav

    (2004)
  • B.J. Rolls et al.

    Reductions in portion size and energy density of foods are additive and lead to sustained decreases in energy intake

    Am J Clin Nutr

    (2006)
  • L. Wisniewski et al.

    Effect of food change on consumption, hedonics, and salivation

    Physiol Behav

    (1992)
  • M. Porrini et al.

    Evaluation of satiety sensations and food intake after different preloads

    Appetite

    (1995)
  • M. Myers Ernst et al.

    Habituation of responding for food in humans

    Appetite

    (2002)
  • R. Duclaux et al.

    Effects of a meal on the pleasantness of food and nonfood odors in man

    Physiol Behav

    (1973)
  • P.G. Clifton et al.

    Rapid loss of stimulus-specific satiety after consumption of a second food

    Appetite

    (1987)
  • B.J. Rolls et al.

    Sensory stimulation and energy density in the development of satiety

    Physiol Behav

    (1988)
  • E.M. Bobroff et al.

    Effects of changes in palatability on food intake and the cumulative food intake curve in man

    Appetite

    (1986)
  • M.R. Yeomans

    Palatability and the micro-structure of feeding in humans: the appetizer effect

    Appetite

    (1996)
  • C. de Graaf et al.

    Palatability affects satiation but not satiety

    Physiol Behav

    (1999)
  • S.E. Swithers et al.

    Does oral experience terminate ingestion?

    Appetite

    (1994)
  • L.H. Epstein et al.

    Habituation and dishabituation of human salivary response

    Physiol Behav

    (1992)
  • M.J. Theunissen et al.

    Taste adaptation during the eating of sweetened yogurt

    Appetite

    (2000)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text